Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
  • Cited by 7
Publisher:
Cambridge University Press
Online publication date:
December 2012
Print publication year:
2012
Online ISBN:
9781139342223

Book description

Structural information theory is a coherent theory about the way the human visual system organises a raw visual stimulus into objects and object parts. To humans, a visual stimulus usually has one clear interpretation even though, in theory, any stimulus can be interpreted in numerous ways. To explain this, the theory focuses on the nature of perceptual interpretations rather than on underlying process mechanisms and adopts the simplicity principle which promotes efficiency of internal resources rather than the likelihood principle which promotes veridicality in the external world. This theoretically underpinned starting point gives rise to quantitative models and verifiable predictions for many visual phenomena, including amodal completion, subjective contours, transparency, brightness contrast, brightness assimilation and neon illusions. It also explains phenomena such as induced temporal order, temporal context effects and hierarchical dominance effects, and extends to evaluative pattern qualities such as distinctiveness, interestingness and beauty.

Reviews

‘Whether you are familiar with structural information theory or not, you will enjoy this systematic presentation by Emanuel Leeuwenberg, its original proponent, and Peter van der Helm, its main formal developer. Through twelve chapters, including a coding manual, they distil order out of perception and cognition, like the demons invoked in the introduction. SIT provides a powerful language for evaluating how strongly the mind strives for simplicity; the book provides an optimal context for evaluating the strength of SIT.’

Walter Gerbino - University of Trieste

‘Leeuwenberg and van der Helm have assembled the definitive statement on their influential theory of the coding of visual forms. SIT is the most thorough system available for capturing the essence of a structure’s simplicity, so this volume will be required reading for those interested in this far-reaching and quintessentially Gestalt concept.’

James Pomerantz - Rice University

'[This] book is a well-written, well-structured overview of SIT at the endpoint of its development. The underlying (meta)theoretical principles and assumptions are spelled out clearly. The empirical support is reviewed in just enough detail to understand all the necessary details. The systematic and coherent presentation of this material makes it relatively easy to digest. The different sections and chapters contain useful previews and interim summaries. The book has a large number of richly detailed coding examples as well as many visual examples, with Leeuwenberg’s personal touch (in addition to having a keen eye as a visual phenomenologist, he is also very talented in drawing and painting).'

Johan Wagemans Source: Perception

Refine List

Actions for selected content:

Select all | Deselect all
  • View selected items
  • Export citations
  • Download PDF (zip)
  • Save to Kindle
  • Save to Dropbox
  • Save to Google Drive

Save Search

You can save your searches here and later view and run them again in "My saved searches".

Please provide a title, maximum of 40 characters.
×

Contents

References

Ahissar, M. and Hochstein, S. (2004). The reverse hierarchy theory of visual perceptual learning. Trends in Cognitive Science, 8, 457–64.
Alexander, P. A., Willson, V. L., White, C. S., Fuqua, J. D., Clark, G. D., Wilson, A. F., and Kulikowich, J. M. (1989). Development of analogical reasoning in four- and five-year-old children. Cognitive Development, 4, 65–88.
Anderson, N. S. and Fitts, P. M. (1958). Amount of information gained during brief exposures of numerals and colors. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56, 362–9.
Aristotle (1957). On the Soul, Parva Naturalia, On Breath (W. S. Hett, trans.). London: Heinemann (original work published c. 350 bc).
Arnheim, R. (1954). Art and Visual Perception. Berkeley and Los Angeles: University of California Press.
Arnheim, R. (1987). Prägnanz and its discontents. Gestalt Theory, 9, 102–7.
Attneave, F. (1971). Multistability in perception. Scientific American, 225, 62–71.
Attneave, F. (1982). Prägnanz and soap-bubble systems: a theoretical exploration. In J. Beck (ed.), Organization and Representation in Perception (pp. 11–29). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Bachmann, T. and Allik, J. (1976). Integration and interruption in the masking of form by form. Perception, 5, 79–97.
Baldwin, G. W. (1946). Inductive Reasoning Test. Philadelphia: Educational Test Bureau.
Barlow, H. B. and Reeves, B. C. (1979). The versatility and absolute efficiency of detecting mirror symmetry in random dot displays. Vision Research, 19, 783–93.
Barrow, H. G. and Tenenbaum, J. M. (1981). Computational vision. Proceedings of the Institute of Electrical Engeneers, 69, 572–95.
Baylis, G. C. and Driver, J. (1994). Parallel computation of symmetry but not repetition within single visual shapes. Visual Cognition, 1, 377–400.
Beck, J. (1966). Contrast and assimilation in lightness judgments. Perception and Psychophysics, 1, 342–4.
Beck, J. (1982). Textural segmentation. In J. Beck (ed.), Organization and Representation in Perception (pp. 285–318). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Beller, H. (1971). Priming: effects of advance information on matching. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 87, 176–82.
Berkeley, G. (1710). The principles of human knowledge. In G. J. Warnock (ed.), The Fontana Library Phylosophy. London and Glasgow: Collins Clear-Type Press.
Berlyne, D. E. (1971). Aesthetics and Psychobiology. New York: Appleton.
Biederman, I. (1987). Recognition-by-components: a theory of human image understanding. Psychological Review, 94, 115–47.
Binford, T. (1981). Inferring surfaces from images. Artificial Intelligence, 17, 205–44.
Birkhoff, G. D. (1933). Aesthetic Measure. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Blum, H. and Nagel, R. N. (1978). Shape description using weighted symmetry axis features. Pattern Recognition, 10, 167–80.
Bolk, L. (1926). Das problem der menschwerdung [The problem of human evolution]. University paper, University of Jena, Germany.
Bosanquet, B. (1892). A History of Aesthetic. New York: Macmillan.
Boselie, F. (1982). Over visuele schoonheidservaring [On visual aesthetic sensation]. PhD thesis, University of Nijmegen, Netherlands.
Boselie, F. (1983). Ambiguity, beauty, and interestingness of line drawings. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 37, 287–92.
Boselie, F. (1984). The aesthetic attractivity of the golden section. Psychological Research, 45, 367–75.
Boselie, F. (1988). Local versus global minima in visual pattern completion. Perception and Psychophysics, 43, 431–45.
Boselie, F. (1994). Local and global factors in visual occlusion. Perception, 23, 517–28.
Boselie, F. (1997). The golden section and the shape of objects. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 15, 131–41.
Boselie, F. and Leeuwenberg, E. L. J. (1985). Birkhoff revisited: beauty as a function of effect and means. American Journal of Psychology, 98, 1–39.
Boselie, F. and Leeuwenberg, E. L. J. (1986). A test of the minimum principle requires a perceptual coding system. Perception, 15, 331–54.
Boselie, F. and Wouterlood, D. (1989). The minimum principle and visual pattern completion. Psychological Research, 51, 93–101.
Boselie, F. and Wouterlood, D. (1992). A critical discussion of Kellman and Shipley's (1991) theory of occlusion phenomena. Psychological Research, 54, 278–85.
Brigner, W. L. and Gallagher, M. B. (1974). Subjective contour: apparent depth or simultaneous brightness contrast? Perceptual and Motor Skills, 38, 1047–53.
Brunswick, E. (1956). Perception and the Representative Design of Psychological Experiments. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
Buffart, H. F. J. M. and Leeuwenberg, E. L. J. (1983). Structural information theory. In H. Geissler, H. Buffart, E. Leeuwenberg, and V. Sarris (eds.), Modern Issues in Perception (pp. 48–72). Berlin: VEB Deutscher Verlag der Wissenschaften.
Buffart, H. F. J. M., Leeuwenberg, E. L. J., and Restle, F. (1981). Coding theory of visual pattern completion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7, 241–74.
Calis, G., Sterenborg, J., and Maarse, F. (1984). Initial microgenetic steps in single glance face recognition. Acta Psychologica, 55, 215–30.
Chaitin, G. J. (1969). On the length of programs for computing finite binary sequences: statistical considerations. Journal of the Association for Computing Machinery, 16, 145–59.
Chapanis, A. and McCleary, R. (1953). Interposition as a cue for the perception of relative distance. Journal of General Psychology, 48, 113–32.
Chater, N. (1996). Reconciling simplicity and likelihood principles in perceptual organization. Psychological Review, 103, 566–81.
Cherry, C. (1961). On Human Communication. New York: Science Editions.
Churchland, P. S. (1986). Neurophilosophy. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Churchland, P. S. (2002). Brain-Wise: Studies in Neurophilosophy. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Churchland, P. S. and Sejnowsky, T. J. (1990). Neural representation and neural computation. In W. G. Lycan (ed.), Mind and Cognition: A Reader (pp. 224–52). Oxford: Blackwell.
Churchland, P. S. and Sejnowsky, T. J. (1992). The Computational Brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Cohen, D. and Kubovy, M. (1993). Mental rotation, mental representation, and flat slopes. Cognitive Psychology, 25, 351–82.
Colberg, M., Nester, M. A., and Trattner, M. H. (1985). Convergence of the inductive and deductive models in the measurement of reasoning abilities. Journal of Applied Psychology, 70, 681–94.
Collard, R. F. A. and Buffart, H. F. J. M. (1983). Minimization of structural information: a set-theoretical approach. Pattern Recognition, 16, 231–42.
Collard, R. F. A. and Leeuwenberg, E. (1981). Judged temporal order of visual patterns. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 35, 323–9.
Collard, R. F. A. and Povel, D. J. (1982). Theory of serial pattern production: tree traversals. Psychological Review, 89, 693–707.
Cooper, L. A. (1976). Demonstration of a mental analog of an external rotation. Perception and Psychophysics, 19, 296–302.
Corballis, M. (1988). Recognition of disoriented shapes. Psychological Review, 95, 115–23.
Corballis, M. C. and Roldan, C. E. (1974). On the perception of symmetrical and repeated patterns. Perception and Psychophysics, 16, 136–42.
Coren, S. (1972). Subjective contours and apparent depth. Psychological Review, 79, 359–67.
Csathó, Á. (2004). The versality of visual regularity. PhD thesis, Radboud University, Nijmegen, Netherlands.
Csathó, Á., van der Vloed, G., and van der Helm, P. A. (2003). Blobs strengthen repetition but weaken symmetry. Vision Research, 43, 993–1007.
Csathó, Á., van der Vloed, G., and van der Helm, P. A. (2004). The force of symmetry revisited: symmetry-to-noise ratios regulate (a)symmetry effects. Acta Psychologica, 117, 233–50.
de Boer, L. and Keuss, P. (1981). Global precedence as a postperceptual effect: an analysis of speed-accuracy tradeoff functions. Perception and Psychophysics, 31, 358–66.
de Wit, T. (2004). Disoccluding completions. PhD thesis, Radboud University, Nijmegen, Netherlands.
de Wit, T. and van Lier, R. (2002). Global visual completion of quasi-regular shapes. Perception, 31, 969–84.
Dember, W. N. (1965). Psychology of Perception. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston.
Descartes, R. (1644). Les principes de la philosophy [Principles of philosophy]. In A. Bridoux (1953). Oeuvres et Lettres [Works and Letters]. Paris: Gallimard.
Deutsch, J. A. (1955). A theory of shape recognition. British Journal of Psychology, 46, 30–7.
Dijkstra, E. W. (1959). A note on two problems in connexion with graphs. Numerische Mathematik, 1, 269–71.
Dinnerstein, D. and Wertheimer, M. (1957). Some determinants of phenomenal overlapping. American Journal of Psychology, 70, 21–37.
Donderi, D. C. (2006). Visual complexity: a review. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 73–97.
Dumais, S. T. and Bradley, D. R. (1976). Effects of illumination level and retinal size on the apparent strength of subjective contours. Perception and Psychophysics, 19, 339–45.
Duncan, J. and Humphreys, G. (1989). Visual search and stimulus similarity. Psychological Review, 96, 433–58.
Edelman, G. M. (1987). Neural Darwinism: The Theory of Neuronal Group Selection. New York: Basic Books.
Ehrenstein, W. (1941). Ueber abwandlungen der L. Hermannschen helligkeitserscheinung [On changes of L. Hermann brightness sensations]. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 150, 83–91.
Enquist, M. and Arak, A. (1994). Symmetry, beauty and evolution. Nature, 372, 169–72.
Erikson, C. W. and Hake, H. W. (1955). Multidimensional stimulus differences and accuracy of discrimination. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50, 155–60.
Evans, S. (1967). Redundancy as a variable in pattern perception. Psychological Bulletin, 67, 104–13.
Eysenck, H. J. (1942). The experimental study of the ‘Good Gestalt’ – a new approach. Psychological Review, 49, 344–64.
Eysenck, H. J. and Castle, M. (1970). Training in art as a factor in the determination of preference judgments for polygons. British Journal of Psychology, 61, 65–81.
Farah, M. J. (1995). The neural basis of mental imagery. In M. Gazzaniga (ed.), The Cognitive Neurosciences (pp. 963–75). Cambridge, London: MIT Press.
Fechner (1876). Vorschule der Ästhetik. Leipzig: Breitkopf and Härtel.
Felleman, D. J. and van Essen, D. C. (1991). Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 1, 1–47.
Finke, R. A. (1980). Levels of equivalence in imagery and perception. Psychological Review, 87, 113–32.
Finkel, L. H., Yen, S-C., and Menschik, E. D. (1998). Synchronization: the computational currency of cognition. In L. Niklasson, M. Boden, and T. Ziemke (eds.), ICANN 98: Proceedings of the 8th International Conference on Artificial Neural Networks (Skövde, Sweden, 2–4 September). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Fleiss, J. L. (1973). Statistical Methods for Rates and Proportions. New York: John Wiley.
Fodor, J. A. (1983). The Modularity of Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Fodor, J. A. and Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1988). Connectionism and cognitive architecture, a critical analysis. Cognition, 28, 3–71.
Frearson, W., Eysenck, H. J., and Barrett, P. T. (1990). The Furneaux model of human problem solving: its relationship to reaction time and intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 11 (3), 239–57.
Freudenthal, H. (1962). Lincos: Design of a Language for Cosmic Intercourse. Amsterdam: North-Holland.
Frisby, J. and Clatworthy, J. (1975). Illusory contours: curious cases of simultaneous brightness contrast? Perception, 4, 349–57.
Gabor, D. (1946). Theory of communication. Journal of the Institution of Electrical Engineers, 93, 429–57.
Gardner, M. (1964). The Ambidextrous Universe: Mirror Asymmetry and Time-reversed Worlds. New York: Basic Books.
Garner, W. R. (1962). Uncertainty and Structure as Psychological Concepts. New York: Wiley.
Garner, W. R. (1966). To perceive is to know. American Psychologist, 21, 11–19.
Garner, W. R. (1970). Good patterns have few alternatives. American Scientist, 58, 34–42.
Garner, W. R. (1974). The Processing of Information and Structure. Potomac, MD: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Gerbino, W. and Salmaso, D. (1987). The effect of amodal completion on visual matching. Acta Psychologica, 65, 25–46.
Gibson, J. (1966). The Senses Considered as Perceptual Systems. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.
Gick, F. and Holyoak, K. J. (1980). Analogical problem solving. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 306–55.
Gigerenzer, G. and Murray, D. J. (1987). Cognition as Intuitive Statistics. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Gilbert, C. D. (1992). Horizontal integration and cortical dynamics. Neuron, 9, 1–13.
Goldmeier, E. (1972). Similarity in visually perceived forms. Psychological Issues, 8 (1), Monograph 29 (originally published in German, 1937).
Gombrich, E. (1956). Art and Illusion. Princeton University Press.
Goodman, N. (1972). Problems and Projects. Indiana: Bobbs Merrill.
Gottschaldt, K. (1929). Über den Einfluss der Erfahrung auf die Wahrnehmung von Figuren [On the influence of experience on the perception of form]. Psychologischen Forschungen, 129, 1–87.
Gray, C. M. (1999). The temporal correlation hypothesis of visual feature integration: still alive and well. Neuron, 24, 31–47.
Gregory, R. L. (1972). Cognitive contours. Nature, 238, 51–2.
Gregory, R. L. (1980). Perceptions as hypotheses. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 290, 181–97.
Gregory, R. L. and Gombrich, E. H. (1973). Illusion in Nature and Art. London: Duckworth.
Halford, G. S. (1992). Analogical reasoning and conceptual complexity in cognitive development. Human Development, 35, 193–217.
Hanssen, A., Leeuwenberg, E., and van der Helm, P. (1993). Metrical information load of lines and angles in line patterns. Psychological Research, 55, 191–9.
Hardonk, M. (1999). Cross-cultural universals of aesthetic appreciation in decorative band patterns. PhD thesis, Radboud University, Nijmegen, Netherlands
Hatfield, G. C. and Epstein, W. (1985). The status of the minimum principle in the theoretical analysis of visual perception. Psychological Bulletin, 97, 155–86.
Hawking, S. (1988). Brief History of Time. New York: Bantam Dell.
Hebb, D. O. (1949). The Organization of Behavior: A Neurophysiological Theory. New York: Wiley.
Helson, H. (1964). Adaptation-level Theory. New York: Harper and Row.
Herbart, J. H. (1816). Lehrbuch zur Psychologie [Introduction to Psychology]. Königsberg, Germany: August Wilhelm Unzer.
Hermens, F. and Herzog, M. H. (2007). The effects of the global structure of the mask in visual backward masking. Vision Research, 47 (1), 790–7.
Hersh, H. M. (1974). The effects of irrelevant relations on the processing of sequential patterns. Memory and Cognition, 24, 771–4.
Hinton, G. and Parsons, L. (1981). Frames of reference and mental imagery. In J. Long and A. Baddeley (eds.), Attention and Performance, Vol. 9 (pp. 261–77). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Hochberg, J. E. (1968). Perception. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Hochberg, J. E. 1982). How big is a stimulus? In J. Beck (ed.), Organization and Representation in Perception (pp. 191–217). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Hochberg, L. and Gellman, L. (1977). The effect of landmark features on mental rotation times. Memory and Cognition, 5 (16), 23–6.
Hochberg, J. E. and McAlister, E. (1953). A quantitative approach to figural ‘goodness’. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46, 361–4.
Hochstein, S. and Ahissar, M. (2002). View from the top: hierarchies and reverse hierarchies in the visual system. Neuron, 36, 791–804.
Høffding, H. (1891). Outlines of Psychology. New York: Macmillan.
Hoffman, D. D. (1996). What do we mean by ‘The structure of the world’? In D. K. Knill and W. Richards (eds.), Perception as Bayesian Inference (pp. 219–21). Cambridge University Press.
Hoffman, D. D. and Richards, W. A. (1984). Parts of recognition. Cognition, 18, 65–96.
Hoffman, J. (1975). Hierarchical stages in the processing of visual information. Perception and Psychophysics, 18, 348–54.
Hofstadter, D. R. (1985). Metamagical Themas: Questing for the Essence of Mind and Pattern. New York: Basic Books.
Holzman, T. G., Pellegrino, J. W., and Glazer, R. (1983). Cognitive variables in series completion. Journal of Educational Psychology, 75, 603–18.
Hubel, D. H. and Wiesel, T. N. (1968). Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey striate cortex. Journal of Physiology (London), 195, 215–43.
Hulleman, J. and Boselie, F. (1999). Perceived shape regularity does not depend on regularities along the contour. Perception, 28, 711–24.
Johansson, G. (1975). Visual motion perception. Scientific American, 232 (6), 76–88.
Julesz, B. (1971). Foundations of Cyclopean Perception. University of Chicago Press.
Kanizsa, G. (1975). The role of regularity in perceptual organization. In F. D’Arcais (ed.), Studies in Perception (pp. 48–66).Florence, Italy: Martello.
Kanizsa, G. (1976). Subjective contours. Scientific American, 234 (4), 48–52.
Kanizsa, G. (1979). Organization in Vision. Essay on Gestalt Perception. New York: Praeger Publishers.
Kanizsa, G. (1985). Seeing and thinking. Acta Psychologica, 59, 23–33.
Kant, I. (1783). Prolegomena zu einer Jeden Kunftigen Metaphysic [Prolegomena to any Future Metaphysics]. Available from Project Gutenberg (www.gutenberg.org).
Kaplan, C. A. and Simon, H. A. (1990). In search of insight. Cognitive Psychology, 22, 374–419.
Kellman, P. and Shipley, T. (1991). A theory of visual pattern interpolation in object perception. Cognitive Psychology, 23, 141–221.
Kennedy, J. M. (1978). Illusory contours not due to completion. Perception, 7, 187–9.
Kimchi, R. and Palmer, S. (1982). Form and texture in hierarchically constructed patterns. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 8, 521–35.
Kinchla, R. (1977). The role of structural redundancy in the perception of visual targets. Perception and Psychophysics, 22, 19–30.
Klahr, D. and Wallace, J. G. (1970). The development of serial completion strategies: an information processing analysis. British Journal of Psychology, 61, 243–57.
Koenderink, J. and van Doorn, A. (1976). The singularities of the visual mapping. Biological Cybernetics, 24, 51–9.
Köhler, W. (1920). Die Physischen Gestalten in Ruhe und im station¨aren Zustand [Static and Stationary Physical Shapes]. Braunschweig, Germany: Vieweg.
Koffka, K. (1935). Principles of Gestalt Psychology. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
Kolmogorov, A. N. (1965). Three approaches to the quantitative definition of information. Problems in Information Transmission, 1, 1–7.
Koning, A. and van Lier, R. (2003). Object-based connectedness facilitates matching. Perception and Psychophysics, 65, 1094–102.
Koning, A. and van Lier, R. (2004). Mental rotation depends on the number of objects rather than on the number of image fragments. Acta Psychologica, 117, 65–77.
Kopfermann, H. (1930). Psychologische untersuchungen über die wirkung zweidimensionaler darstellung körperlicher gebilde. Psychologische Forschung, 13, 292–364.
Kosslyn, S. M. (1975). Information representation in visual images. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 341–70.
Kosslyn, S. M. (1981). The medium of the message in mental imagery: a theory. Psychological Review, 88, 46–66.
Kotovsky, K., Hayes, J. R., and Simon, H. (1985). Why are some problems hard? Evidence from tower of Hanoi. Cognitive Psychology, 17, 248–94.
Kruskal, J. B. (1964). Nonmetric multidimensional scaling: a numerical method. Psychometrika, 29, 115–29.
Kurbat, M. A. (1994). Structural description theories: is RBC/JIM a general-purpose theory of human entry-level object recognition? Perception, 23, 1339–68.
Lamme, V. A. F., Supèr, H., and Spekreijse, H. (1998). Feedforward, horizontal, and feedback processing in the visual cortex. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 8, 529–35.
Leeuwenberg, E. (1968). Structural Information of Visual Patterns. The Hague and Paris: Mouton and Co.
Leeuwenberg, E. (1969). Quantitative specification of information in sequential patterns. Psychological Review, 76, 216–20.
Leeuwenberg, E. (1971). A perceptual coding language for visual and auditory patterns. American Journal of Psychology, 84, 307–49.
Leeuwenberg, E. (1973). Meaning of perceptual complexity. In D. E. Berline and O. O. Madsen (eds.), Pleasure, Reward, Preference (pp. 100–14). New York: Academic Press.
Leeuwenberg, E. (1974). Een statisch criterium voor perceptieve tijdrichting [Later-earlier illusion versus the coding of asymmetry]. Nederlands Tijdschrift voor de Psychologie, 28, 533–48.
Leeuwenberg, E. (1976). Figure-ground specification in terms of structural information. The rivalry between different pattern codings. In H. Geissler and Y. Zabrodin (eds.), Advances in Psychophysics (pp. 325–37). Berlin: VEB Deutscher Verlag der Wissenschaften.
Leeuwenberg, E. (1978). Quantification of certain visual pattern properties: salience, transparency, similarity. In E. Leeuwenberg and H. Buffart (eds.), Formal Theories of Visual Perception (pp. 299–314). Chichester, New York, Brisbane, and Toronto: John Wiley.
Leeuwenberg, E. (1982a). Metrical aspects of patterns and structural information theory. In J. Beck (ed.), Organization and Representation in Perception (pp. 57–72). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Leeuwenberg, E. (1982b). The perception of assimilation and brightness contrast as derived from code theory. Perception and Psychophysics, 32, 345–52.
Leeuwenberg, E. (1985). Paradoxale kenmerken van creatieve prestaties [Paradoxical properties of creative achievements]. In Kreativiteit en Wetenschap, Studium Generale [Creativity and Science, General Studies] (pp. 52–71). University paper: University of Delft, Netherlands.
Leeuwenberg, E. (2003a). Miracles of perception. Acta Psychologica (Special Issue), 114, 379–96.
Leeuwenberg, E. (2003b). Structural information theory and visual form. In C. Kaernbach, E. Schroeger, and H. Mueller (eds.), Psychophysics Beyond Sensation: Laws and Invariants of Human Cognition (pp. 481–505). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Leeuwenberg, E. L. J. and Boselie, F. (1988a). Against the likelihood principle in visual form perception. Psychological Review, 95, 485–91.
Leeuwenberg, E. L. J. and Boselie, F. (1988b). How good a bet is the likelihood principle? In B. A. G. Elsendoorn and H. Bouma (eds.), Working Models of Human Perception (pp. 363–79). London: Academic Press.
Leeuwenberg, E. L. J. and Buffart, H. F. J. M. (1983). An outline of coding theory: a summary of related experiments. In H.-G. Geissler, H. F. J. M. Buffart, E. L. J. Leeuwenberg, and V. Sarris (eds.), Modern Issues in Perception (pp. 1–47). Berlin: VEB Deutscher Verlag der Wissenschaften.
Leeuwenberg, E. L. J., Mens, L., and Calis, G. (1985). Knowledge within perception: masking caused by incompatible interpretation. Acta Psychologica, 59, 91–102.
Leeuwenberg, E. L. J. and van der Helm, P. A. (1991). Unity and variety in visual form. Perception, 20, 595–622.
Leeuwenberg, E. L. J. and van der Helm, P. A. (2000). A code-theoretic note on object handedness. Perception, 29, 5–29.
Leeuwenberg, E., van der Helm, P., and van Lier, R. (1994). From geons to structure. A note on object representation. Perception, 23, 505–15.
Leeuwenberg, E. and van Lier, R. (2005). Symmetry cues for matching mirrored objects. Spatial Vision, 18 (1), 1–24.
Leibniz, G. W. (1714/1979). Monadologie [Monadology]. Stuttgart, Germany: Reclam.
Levelt, W. J. M. (1996). Perspective taking and ellipsis in spatial descriptions. In P. Bloom, M. A. Peterson, M. F. Garret, and L. Nadel (eds.), Language and Space (pp. 77–107). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Leyton, M. (1992). Symmetry, Causality, Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT press.
Li, M. and Vitányi, P. (1997). An Introduction to Kolmogorov Complexity and its Applications (2nd edn.). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Locke, J. (1690/1968). An Essay Concerning Human Understanding. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Mach, E. (1886). Beiträge zur Analyse der Empfindungen [Contributions to the Analysis of Sensations]. Jena, Germany: Gustav Fisher.
MacKay, D. (1950). Quantal aspects of scientific information. Philosophical Magazine, 41, 289–301.
MacKay, D. (1969). Information, Mechanism and Meaning. Boston, MA: MIT Press.
Margulies, S. (1977). Principles of beauty. Psychological Reports, 41, 3–11.
Marr, D. (1982). Vision. San Fransisco, CA: Freeman.
Marr, D. and Nishihara, H. K. (1978). Artificial intelligence and the sensorium of sight. Technology Review, 81, 2–23.
Martin, M. (1979). The role of sparsity. Memory and Cognition, 7, 476–84.
McManus, I. C. (1980). The aesthetics of simple figures. British Journal of Psychology, 71, 505–24.
Medin, D. L. and Schaffer, M. M. (1978). Context theory of classification learning. Psychological Review, 85, 207–38.
Medin, D. L. and Smith, E. E. (1984). Concepts and concept formation. Annual Review Psychology, 35, 113–38.
Mens, L. (1988). Primary perception of stimulus structure. PhD thesis, Radboud University, Nijmegen, Netherlands.
Mens, L. and Leeuwenberg, E. L. J. (1988). Hidden figures are ever present. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14, 561–71.
Mens, L. and Leeuwenberg, E. L. J. (1994). Can perceived shape be primed? The autonomy of organization. Giornale Italiano di Psicologia, 20, 821–36.
Metelli, F. (1974). The perception of transparency. Scientific American, 230 (4), 91–8.
Michels, C. F. and Turvey, M. T. (1979). Central sources of visual masking: indexing structures supporting seeing at a single brief glance. Psychological Research, 41, 1–61.
Milner, P. (1974). A model for visual shape recognition. Psychological Review, 81, 521–35.
Monod, J. (1970). Le Hazard et la Nécessité [Chance and Necessity]. Paris: Editions du Seuil, Institut Pasteur.
Moran, T. (1968). The grammar of visual imagery. University paper, Carnegie-Mellon University, Pittsburgh.
Moore, C. M., Mordkoff, J. T., and Enns, J. T. (2007). The path of least persistence: evidence of object-mediated visual updating. Vision Research, 47, 1624–30.
Moyer, R. S. (1973). Comparing objects in memory: evidence suggesting an internal psychophysics. Perception and Psychophysics, 13, 180–4.
Myors, B., Stankov, L., and Oliphant, G. (1989). Competing tasks, working memory, and intelligence. Australian Journal of Psychology, 41, 1–16.
Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: the precedence of global features in visual perception. Cognitive Psychology, 9, 353–83.
Neisser, U. (1967). Cognitive Psychology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Neisser, U. (1976). Cognition and Reality. San Francisco, CA: Freeman.
Newell, A. (1990). Unified Theory of Cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Newton, I. (1687/1960). Philosophiae Naturalis Principia Mathematica [Mathematical Principles of Natural Phylosophy and his System of the World]. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
Nucci, M. and Wagemans, J. (2007). Goodness of regularity in dot patterns: global symmetry, local symmetry, and their interactions. Perception, 36, 1305–19.
Palmer, S. (1977). Hierarchical structure in perceptual representation. Cognitive Psychology, 9, 441–74.
Palmer, S. (1983). The psychology of perceptual organization: a transformational approach. In J. Beck, B. Hope, and A. Rosenfeld (eds.), Human and Machine Vision (pp. 269–339). New York: Academic Press.
Palmer, S. (1999). Vision Science: Photons to Phenomenology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Palmer, S. E. and Bucher, N. M. (1982). Textual effects in perceived pointing of ambiguous triangles. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 8, 693–708.
Palmer, S. E. and Rock, I. (1994). Rethinking perceptual organization: the role of uniform connectedness. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 1, 29–55.
Palmer, S. E., Brooks, J. L., and Nelson, R. (2003). When does grouping happen? Acta Psychologica, 114, 311–30.
Pani, J. R., Jeffres, J. A., Shippey, G. T., and Schwartz, K. J. (1996). Imagining projective transformations: aligned orientations in spatial organization. Cognitive Psychology, 31, 125–67.
Perkins, D. (1976). How good a bet is a good form? Perception, 5, 393–406.
Perkins, D. (1983). Why the human perceiver is a bad machine. In J. Beck, B. Hope, and A. Rosenfeld (eds.), Human and Machine Vision (pp. 341–64). New York: Academic Press.
Petersen, M. J. (1975). The retention of imagined and seen spatial matrices. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 181–93.
Petter, G. (1956). Nuove ricerche sperimentali sulla totalizzazione percettiva [New experimental studies on perceptual organization]. Rivista di Psicologia, 50, 213–27.
Piaget, J. (1961). Les Méchanismes Perceptifs. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.
Pinker, S. and Finke, R. A. (1980). Emergent two-dimensional patterns in images rotated in depth. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Human Performance, 6 (2), 244–64.
Pollack, I. and Klemmer, E. T. (1954). The assimilation of visual information from linear dot patterns. Air Force Cambridge Research Center, Technical Report, July, 54–66.
Pomerantz, J. R. and Kubovy, M. (1986). Theoretical approaches to perceptual organization: simplicity and likelihood principles. In K. R. Boff, L. Kaufman, and J. P. Thomas (eds.), Handbook of Perception and Human Performance. Vol. 12 (pp. 1–46). New York: Wiley.
Pomerantz, J. R., Agrawal, A., Jewell, S. W., Jeong, M., Kahn, H., and Lozano, S. C. (2003). Contour grouping inside and outside of facial contexts. Acta Psychologica (Special Issue), 114, 245–72.
Posner, M. I. (1973). Cognition: An Introduction. Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman and Co.
Prigogine, I. and Stengers, I. (1984). Order Out of Chaos: Man's New Dialogue with Nature. New York: Bantam Books.
Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1973). What the mind's eye tells the mind's brain: a critique of mental imagery. Psychological Bulletin, 80, 1–24.
Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1999). Is vision continuous with cognition? The case of impenetrability of visual perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 341–423.
Quastler, H. (ed.) (1955). Information Theory in Psychology: Problems and Methods. Glencoe, IL: Free Press.
Quereshi, M. Y. and Seitz, R. (1993a). Identical rules do not make letter and number series equivalent. Intelligence, 17 (3), 399–405.
Quereshi, M. Y. and Seitz, R. (1993b). Gender differences in reasoning ability measured by letter series items. Current Psychology Developmental, Learning, Personality, Social, 12, 268–72.
Quinlan, P. T. (1995). Evidence for the use of scene-based frames of reference in two-dimensional shape recognition. Spatial Vision, 9, 101–25.
Reber, A. S., Walkenfeld, F. F., and Hernstadt, R. (1991). Implicit and explicit learning: individual differences and IQ. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 888–96.
Reed, S. K. (1972). Pattern recognition and categorization. Cognitive Psychology, 3, 382–407.
Reed, S. K. and Johnsen, J. A. (1975). Detection of parts in patterns and images. Memory and Cognition, 3, 569–75.
Reichenbach, H. (1956). The Direction of Time. Berkeley, CA: University of Cali-fornia Press.
Restle, F. (1979). Coding theory of the perception of motion configurations. Psychological Review, 86, 1–24.
Restle, F. (1982). Coding theory as an integration of Gestalt psychology and information processing theory. In J. Beck (ed.), Organization and Representation in Perception (pp. 31–56). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Restle, F. and Decker, J. (1977). Size and the Müller-Lyer illusion as a function of its dimensions. Perception and Psychophysics, 21, 489–503.
Rock, I. (1973). Orientation and Form. New York: Academic Press.
Rock, I. (1983). The Logic of Perception. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Rock, I. (1985). Perception and knowledge. Acta Psychologica, 59, 3–22.
Rosch, E. (1975). Cognitive representations of semantic categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104, 192–233.
Runeson, S. and Frickholm, G. (1981). Visual perception of lifted weight. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7, 733–40.
Ruyer, R. (1956). La Cybernetique et l’Origine de l’Information [Cybernetics and Origin of Information]. Paris: Flammarion.
Scharroo, J. and Leeuwenberg, E. (2000). Representation versus process in simplicity of serial pattern completion. Cognitive Psychology, 40, 39–86.
Scharroo, J., Stalmeier, P., and Boselie, F. (1994). Visual search and segregation as a function of display complexity. Journal of General Psychology, 121, 5–18.
Schumann, F. (1900). Beiträge zur analyse der gesichtswahrnemungen: einige beobachtungen über die zusammenfassung von Gesichtseindrücken zu Einheiten [Contributions to visual perception research: some observations on the integration of visual sensations toward perceptual components]. Zeitschrift fur Psychologie, 23, 1–32.
Sekuler, A. and Palmer, S. (1992). Perception of partly occluded objects: a microgenetic analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology, General, 121, 95–111.
Sekuler, A., Palmer, S., and Flinn, C. (1994). Local and global processes in visual completion. Psychological Science, 5, 260–7.
Shannon, C. E. (1948). A mathematical theory of communication. Bell System Technical Journal, 27, 379–423, 623–56.
Shannon, C. E. and Weaver, W. (1949). The Mathematical Theory of Communication. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press.
Shepard, R. N. (1962a). The analysis of proximities: multidimensional scaling with an unknown distance function I. Psychometrika, 27, 125–40.
Shepard, R. N. (1962b). The analysis of proximities: multidimensional scaling with an unknown distance function II. Psychometrika, 27, 219–46.
Shepard, R. N. (1980). Multidimensional scaling, tree-fitting, and clustering. Science, 210, 390–8.
Shepard, R. N. and Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science, 171, 701–3.
Shimaya, A. (1994). A perceptual model of figure segregation and amodal completion. Investigative Ophtalmology and Visual Science, Supplement, 35, 16–44.
Simon, H. A. (1969). The Science of the Artificial. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Simon, H. A. (1972). Complexity and the representation of patterned sequences of symbols. Psychological Review, 79, 369–82.
Simon, H. A. (1990). Invariants of human behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 41, 1–19.
Simon, H. A. and Kotovsky, K. (1963). Human acquisition of concepts for sequential patterns. Psychological Review, 70, 534–46.
Singer, W. and Gray, C. M. (1995). Visual feature integration and the temporal correlation hypothesis. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 18, 555–86.
Smolensky, P. (1988). On the proper treatment of connectionism. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 11, 1–23.
Sober, E. (1975). Simplicity. London: Oxford University Press.
Solomonoff, R. J. (1964a). A formal theory of inductive inference, part 1. Information and Control, 7, 1–22.
Solomonoff, R. J. (1964b). A formal theory of inductive inference, part 2. Information and Control, 7, 224–54.
Spellman, B. A. and Holyoak, K. J. (1996). Pragmatics in analogical mappings. Cognitive Psychology, 31, 307–46.
Stankov, L. and Cregan, A. (1993). Quantitative and qualitative properties of an intelligence test: series completion. Learning and Individual Differences, 5 (2), 137–69.
Sternberg, R. J. and Gardner, M. K. (1983). Unities in inductive reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 112 (1), 80–116.
Sundqvist, F. (2003). Perceptual Dynamics: Theoretical Foundations and Philosophical Implications of Gestalt Psychology. Göteborg, Sweden: Gothenburg University.
Takano, Y. (1989). Perception of rotated forms: a theory of information types. Cognitive Psychology, 21, 1–59.
Tarr, M. J. (1995). Rotating objects to recognize them: a case study on the role of viewpoint dependency in the recognition of three-dimensional objects. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 2, 55–82.
Thurstone, L. L. and Thurstone, T. G. (1941). Factorial Atudies of Intelligence. University of Chicago Press.
Tippett, L. J. (1992). The generation of visual images: a review of neuropsychological research and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 415–32.
Treder, M. S. and van der Helm, P. A. (2007). Symmetry versus repetition in cyclopean vision: a microgenetic analysis. Vision Research, 47, 2956–67.
Treder, M. S., van der Vloed, G., and van der Helm, P. A. (2011). Interactions between constituent single symmetries in multiple symmetry. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 73, 1487–502.
Treisman, A. (1969). Strategies and models of selective attention. Psychologica Review, 76, 282–99.
Treisman, A. (1982). Perceptual grouping and attention in visual search for features and for objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 8, 194–214.
Treisman, A. (1986). Properties, parts, and objects. In K. R. Boff, L. Kaufman, and J. P. Thomas (eds.), Handbook of Perception and Human Performance: Vol. 1. Cognitive Processes and Performance (pp. 1–70). New York: Wiley.
Treisman, A. and Gelade, G. (1980). A feature integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97–136.
Tversky, A. (1977). Features of similarity. Psychological Review, 84, 327–52.
Ungerleider, L. G. and Mishkin, M. (1982). Two cortical visual systems. In D. J. Ingle, M. A. Goodale, and R. J. W. Mansfield (eds.), Analysis of Visual Behavior (pp. 549–86). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
van Bakel, A. (1989). Perceived unity and duality as determined by superstructure components of pattern codes. Masters thesis, Nijmegen Institute for Information and Cognition (NICI), University of Nijmegen, Netherlands.
van der Helm, P. A. (1988). Accessibility and simplicity of visual structures. PhD thesis, Radboud University, Nijmegen, Netherlands.
van der Helm, P. A. (1994). The dynamics of Prägnanz. Psychological Research, 56, 224–36.
van der Helm, P. A. (2000). Simplicity versus likelihood in visual perception: from surprisals to precisals. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 770–800.
van der Helm, P. A. (2004). Transparallel processing by hyperstrings. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 101, 10862–7.
van der Helm, P. A. (2010). Weber-Fechner behaviour in symmetry perception? Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 72, 1854–64.
van der Helm, P. A. (2011). Bayesian confusions surrounding simplicity and likelihood in perceptual organization. Acta Psychologica, 138, 337–46.
van der Helm, P. A. (2012). Cognitive architecture of perceptual organization: from neurons to gnosons. Cognitive Processing, 13, 13–40.
van der Helm, P. A. (2013). Simplicity in Vision: a Multidisciplinary Account of Perceptual Organization. Cambridge University Press (in press).
van der Helm, P. A. and Leeuwenberg, E. L. J. (1986). Avoiding explosive search in automatic selection of simplest pattern codes. Pattern Recognition, 19, 181–91.
van der Helm, P. A. and Leeuwenberg, E. L. J. (1991). Accessibility: a criterion for regularity and hierarchy in visual pattern codes. Journal of Mathematical psychology, 35, 151–213.
van der Helm, P. A. and Leeuwenberg, E. L. J. (1996). Goodness of visual regularities: a nontransformational approach. Psychological Review, 103, 429–56.
van der Helm, P. A. and Leeuwenberg, E. L. J. (1999). A better approach to goodness: reply to Wagemans (1999). Psychological Review, 106, 622–30.
van der Helm, P. A. and Leeuwenberg, E. L. J. (2004). Holographic goodness is not that bad: reply to Olivers, Chater, and Watson (2004). Psychological Review, 111, 261–73.
van der Helm, P. A. and Treder, M. S. (2009). Detection of (anti)symmetry and (anti)repetition: perceptual mechanisms versus cognitive strategies. Vision Research, 49, 2754–63.
van der Helm, P. A., van Lier, R. J., and Leeuwenberg, E. L. J. (1992). Serial pattern complexity: irregularity and hierarchy. Perception, 21, 517–44.
van der Vloed, G. (2005). The structure of visual regularities. PhD thesis, Radboud University, Nijmegen, Netherlands.
van der Vloed, G., Csathó, Á., and van der Helm, P. A. (2005). Symmetry and repetition in perspective. Acta Psychologica, 120, 74–92.
van der Vloed, G., Csathó, Á., and van der Helm, P. A. (2007). Effects of asynchrony on symmetry perception. Psychological Research, 71, 170–7.
van Leeuwen, C. (2007). What needs to emerge to make you conscious? Journal of Consciousness Studies, 14, 115–36.
van Leeuwen, C., Steyvers, M., and Nooter, M. (1997). Stability and intermittency in large-scale coupled oscillator models for perceptual segmentation. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 41, 319–44.
van Lier, R. J. (1996). Simplicity of visual shape: a structural information approach. PhD thesis, Radboud University, Nijmegen, Netherlands.
van Lier, R. J. (1999). Investigating global effects in visual occlusion: from a partly occluded square to a tree-trunk's rear. Acta Psychologica, 102, 203–20.
van Lier, R. J. (2000). Separate features versus one principle: comment on Shimaya (1997). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 412–17.
van Lier, R. J. (2001). Simplicity, regularity, and perceptual interpretations: a structural information approach. In T. Shipley and P. Kellman (eds.), From Fragments to Objects: Segmentation in Vision (pp. 331–52). New York: Elsevier.
van Lier, R. J. (2003). Differential effects of object orientation on imaginary object/viewer transformations. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 10, 455–61.
van Lier, R. and Wagemans, J. (1999). From images to objects: global and local completions of self-occluded parts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1721–41.
van Lier, R. J., Leeuwenberg, E. L. J., and van der Helm, P. A. (1995). Multiple completions primed by occlusion patterns. Perception, 24, 727–40.
van Lier, R. J., Leeuwenberg, E. L. J., and van der Helm, P. A. (1997). In support of structural hierarchy in object representations. Psychological Research, 60, 134–43.
van Lier, R. J., van der Helm, P. A., and Leeuwenberg, E. L. J. (1994). Integrating global and local aspects of visual occlusion. Perception, 23, 883–903.
van Noorden, L. (1975). Temporal coherence in the perception of tone sequences. PhD thesis, Eindhoven University of Technology.
van Soest, J. L. (1952). Informatie en Communicatie Theorie. University paper, Department of Electronics, University of Delft, Netherlands.
van Tuijl, H. (1975). A new visual illusion: neonlike colour spreading and complementary colour induction between subjective contours. Acta Psychologica, 39, 441–5.
van Tuijl, H. (1979). Perceptual interpretation of line patterns. PhD thesis, Radboud University, Nijmegen, Netherlands.
van Tuijl, H. and de Weert, C. (1979). Sensory conditions for the occurence of the neon spreading illusion. Perception, 8, 211–15.
van Tuijl, H. and Leeuwenberg, E. L. J. (1979). Neon color spreading and structural information measures. Perception and Psychophysics, 25, 269–84.
van Tuijl, H. and Leeuwenberg, E. L. J. (1980). Perceptual interpretation of complex line patterns. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 6, 197–221.
van Tuijl, H. and Leeuwenberg, E. L. J. (1982). Peripheral and central determinants of subjective contour strength. In H. G. Geissler, H. F. J. M. Buffart, P. Petzoldt, and Y. M. Zabrodin (eds.), Psychophysical Judgement and the Process of Perception (pp. 114–31). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
Varin, D. (1971). Fenomeni di contrasto e diffusione cromatica nell’ organizzazione spaziale del campo percettivo [Contrast and colour spreading phenomena in the spatial organization of the visual field]. Rivista di Psicologia, 65, 101–28.
von Bezold, W. (1874). Die Farbenlehre im Hinblick auf Kunst und Kunstgewerbe [Colour Theory for Art and Art Products]. Braunschweig, Germany: Westerman.
von der Malsburg, C. (1981). The Correlation Theory of Brain Function. Göttingen, Germany: Max Planck Institute for Biophysical Chemistry, Internal Report 81–2.
von Helmholtz, H. L. F. (1962). Treatise on Physiological Optics (J. P. C. Southall, trans.). New York: Dover (original work published 1909).
Wagemans, J., van Gool, L., and d’Ydewalle, G. (1991). Detection of symmetry in tachistoscopically presented dot patterns: effects of multiple axes and skewing. Perception and Psychophysics, 50, 413–27.
Wagemans, J., van Gool, L., Swinnen, V., and van Horebeek, J. (1993). Higher-order structure in regularity detection. Vision Research, 33, 1067–88.
Watanabe, S. (1969). Methodologies of Pattern Recognition. New York: Academic Press.
Wenderoth, P. and Welsh, S. (1998). Effects of pattern orientation and number of symmetry axes on the detection of mirror symmetry in dot and solid patterns. Perception, 27, 965–76.
Wertheimer, M. (1923). Untersuchungen zur lehre von der Gestalt [On Gestalt theory]. Psychologische Forschung, 4, 301–50.
Weyl, H. (1952). Symmetry. Princeton University Press.
Wiener, N. (1948). Cybernetics. New York: Wiley.
Wouterlood, D. and Boselie, F. (1993). Shape decomposition as a function of concave discontinuity. Report, Nijmegen Institute for Information and Cognition (NICI), University of Nijmegen, Netherlands.
Zusne, L. (1970). Visual Perception of Form. New York: Academic Press.

Metrics

Altmetric attention score

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 0
Total number of PDF views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

Book summary page views

Total views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between #date#. This data will be updated every 24 hours.

Usage data cannot currently be displayed.