Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-8kt4b Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-07T23:25:11.248Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

5 - Modeling the hierarchical structure of learning

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 January 2010

Get access

Summary

Introduction

In the social and behavioral sciences, theories abound that view phenomena as hierarchically organized and decomposable into entities that can be ordered in terms of their complexity. For example, learning has frequently been conceptualized as a hierarchy of processes or outcomes of increasing complexity. Of particular importance to educators because of the impact they have had on curriculum and test development are a number of educational taxonomies – taxonomies being, of course, a type of theory – that embody the notion of a hierarchy of processes or outcomes (Bloom et al. 1956; Gagne 1975; Ausubel & Robinson 1969; Merrill 1971; Biggs & Collis 1981).

This chapter illustrates how theories involving the notion of a hierarchy of variables of increasing complexity can be expressed in mathematical form as a series of structural models and how the analysis of empirical data using these models can be employed to reformulate the original theories.

Modeling hierarchical structure: the Guttman simplex model

One of the most significant attempts to express the concept of a hierarchy of variables of increasing complexity in mathematical form was that of Guttman (1954), who proposed a theory to explain the pattern of intercorrelations to be observed among mental test scores. Guttman proposed (pp. 260–1) that, if content is held constant, a set of tests will differ only in terms of their complexity and are said to belong to a “simplex,” since they can be arranged in rank order from least to most complex.

In translating the concept of the simplex into mathematical terms, Guttman considered the case of continuous variables.

Type
Chapter
Information
Structural Modeling by Example
Applications in Educational, Sociological, and Behavioral Research
, pp. 65 - 85
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 1988

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×