Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-gq7q9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-21T15:36:14.399Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

References

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 November 2018

Lisa Osbeck
Affiliation:
University of West Georgia
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2018

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adler, A. (1964). The individual psychology of Alfred Adler. Ansbacher, H. L. & Ansbacher, R. R. (eds.). New York: Harper Torchbooks.Google Scholar
Angell, J. R. (1904). Psychology: An introductory study of the structure and function of human consciousness. New York: Henry Holt.Google Scholar
Angell, J. R. (1907). The province of functional psychology. Psychological Review, 14, 6191.Google Scholar
Angell, J. R. (1915). Chapters from modern psychology. New York: Longmans, Green, & Co.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ansbacher, H. L., & Ansbacher, R. R. (eds.). (1956). The individual psychology of Alfred Adler. Oxford: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Araujo, S. F. (2016). Wundt and the philosophical foundations of psychology. London: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Araujo, S. F. (2017). Psychology between science and common sense: William James and the problems of psychological language in the PrinciplesNew Ideas in Psychology,  46, 3945.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atkinson, P., & Hammersley, M. (1994). Ethnography and participant observation. In Denzin, N. K. & Lincoln, Y. S. (eds.), Handbook of qualitative research (pp. 248261). Thousand Oaks: SAGE.Google Scholar
Austin, J. L. (1962). How to do things with words. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Baars, B. (1986). The cognitive revolution in psychology. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Bacon, F. (1937). In Jones, R. F. (ed.), Novum Organum.New York: Odyssey Press, Inc., Originally published 1620.Google Scholar
Bacon, F. (1996). Sylva Sylvarum: Or a natural history in ten centuries. Whitefish, MT: Kessinger. Originally published posthumously 1627.Google Scholar
Baker, D. B., & Benjamin, L. T. Jr. (2000). The affirmation of the scientist–practitioner: A look back at Boulder. American Psychologist, 55(2), 241247.Google Scholar
Bender, A., & Beller, S. (2016). Current perspectives on cognitive diversity. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 509. http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00509Google Scholar
Bentley, M. (1929). “Observer” and “subject.” American Journal of Psychology, 41, 682.Google Scholar
Bergner, R. M. (2017). What is a person? What is the self? Formulations for a science of psychology. Journal of Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology, 37(2), 7790.Google Scholar
Berlin, I., & Hardy, H. (1999). The Roots of Romanticism: The AW Mellon Lectures in the Fine Arts 1965. Washington, DC: The National Gallery of Art.Google Scholar
Black, M. (1955, June). XII.–Metaphor, Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society, Vol. 55, 1, pp. 273294.Google Scholar
Black, M. (1962). Models and metaphors: Studies in language and philosophy. Ithaca: Cornell University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloor, D. (1984). The strengths of the strong programme. In Brown, J. R. (ed.), Scientific rationality: The sociological turn (pp. 7594). Dordrecht: Reidel.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base: Clinical applications of attachment theory. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Brentano, F. (1874). Psychologie vom empirischen sandpunkte. Leipzig: Duncker & Humblot.Google Scholar
Breuer, J., & Freud, S. (1957). Studies on hysteria. Strachey, J. (ed.). Oxford: Basic Books, Inc. Originally published 1895, Studien über Hysterie.Google Scholar
Bridgman, P. (1959). The way things are. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Brightman, E. S. (1943). Personality as a metaphysical principle. In Brightman, E. S. (ed.), Personalism in theology (pp. 4063). Boston: Boston University Press.Google Scholar
Brockmeier, J. (2009). Reaching for meaning: Human agency and the narrative imagination. Theory & Psychology, 19(2), 213233.Google Scholar
Bronowski, J. (1961). Science and human values. New York: Harper & Row. Originally published 1956.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. (1973. The conditions of creativity. In Bruner (ed.), J., Beyond the information given (pp. 208217). New York: Norton & Co. Originally published 1962.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. (1973). Going beyond the information given. In Bruner (ed.), J., Beyond the information given (pp. 218238). New York: Norton & Co. Originally published 1957.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. (1990). Acts of meaning. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Buford, T. (2006). Persons in the tradition of Boston Personalism. The Journal of Speculative Philosophy, 20(3), 214218.Google Scholar
Burrow, R. (1999). Personalism: A critical introduction. St. Louis: Chalice Press.Google Scholar
Byford, J., & Tileagă, C. (2014). Social psychology, history, and the study of the Holocaust: The perils of interdisciplinary “borrowing”. Peace and Conflict: Journal of Peace Psychology,  20(4), 349.Google Scholar
Camp, E. (2009). Two varieties of literary imagination: Metaphor, fiction, and thought experiments. Midwest Studies in Philosophy, 33(1), 107130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cartwright, N. (1997). Models: The blueprints for laws. Philosophy of Science, 64, S292S303.Google Scholar
Chalmers, A. (1999). What is this thing called science? 3rd ed. Indianapolis: Hackett. Originally published 1976.Google Scholar
Chalmers, A. (2013). What is this thing called science? 4th ed. Indianapolis: Hackett. Originally published 1976.Google Scholar
Ciarrochi, J., Morin, A. J. S., Sahdra, B. K., Litalien, D., & Parker, P. D. (2017). A longitudinal person-centered perspective on youth social support: Relations with psychological wellbeing. Developmental Psychology, 53(6), 11541169.Google Scholar
Cokely, E. T., & Feltz, A. (2014). Expert intuition. In Osbeck, L. & Held, B. (eds.), Rational intuition (pp. 213238). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Comte, A. (1957). A general view of positivism. J. H. Bridges, trans. New York: Speller. Original work published 1849.Google Scholar
Coplan, A., & Goldie, P. (eds.). (2011). Empathy: Philosophical and psychological perspectives. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Crenshaw, K. (1989). Demarginalizing the intersection of race and sex: A black feminist critique of antidiscrimination doctrine, feminist theory, and antiracist politics. University of Chicago Legal Forum: special issue, Feminism in the Law: Theory, Practice, and Criticism, University of Chicago Law School, 139168.Google Scholar
Cronbach, L. J. (1957). The two disciplines of scientific psychology. American Psychologist, 12(11), 671684.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Danziger, K. (1990). Constructing the subject: Historical origins of psychological research. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Danziger, K. (1997). Naming the mind. London: SAGE.Google Scholar
Danziger, K. (2013). Historical psychology of persons: Categories and practice. In Martin, J. & Bickhard, M. (eds.), The psychology of personhood: Philosophical, historical, social-developmental, and narrative perspectives (pp. 5180). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Dashiell, J. F. (1929). Note on use of term “observer.” Psychological Review, 36, 550551.Google Scholar
Daston, L. (1998). Fear and loathing of the imagination in science. Daedalus, 127(1), 7395.Google Scholar
Daston, L. (2011). The empire of observation. In Daston, L. & Lunbeck, E. (eds.), Histories of scientific observation (pp. 81114). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Date, G., & Chandrasekharan, S. (2017): Beyond efficiency: Engineering for sustainability requires solving for pattern. Engineering Studies. https://doi.org/10.1080/19378629.2017.1410160Google Scholar
De Jaegher, H., & Di Paolo, E. (2007). Participatory sense-making: An enactive approach to social cognitionPhenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 6, 485507.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Derry, S., & Schunn, C. (2014). Interdisciplinarity: A beautiful but dangerous beast. In Derry, S., Schunn, C., & Gernsbacher, M. (eds.), Interdisciplinary collaboration: An emerging cognitive science (pp. xiiixx). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Derry, S., Schunn, C., & Gernsbacher, M. (eds.). (2014). Interdisciplinary collaboration: An emerging cognitive science. New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Dewey, J. (1910). How we think. New York: Heath.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dewey, J. (1938). Logic: The theory of inquiry. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Dewey, J. (2008). Experience and nature. In Boydston, J. A. (ed.), The later works of John Dewey, Vol. 1. Carbondale: Southern Illinois University Press.Google Scholar
Dewey, J., & Bentley, A. (1949). The knower and the known. The Later Works of John Dewey, 16. Boston: Beacon.Google Scholar
Dilthey, W. (1977). Ideas concerning a descriptive and analytic psychology. In R. Zaner & K. Heiges (trans.), Descriptive psychology and historical understanding (pp. 21120). Dordrecht: Springer. Originally published 1894.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Donoghue, D. (2014). Metaphor. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Douglas, H. (2009). Science, policy, and the value-free ideal. Pittsburgh: University of Pittsburgh Press.Google Scholar
Dupré, J. (1993). The disorder of things. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Elgin, C. Z. (2014). Fiction as thought experiment. Perspectives on Science, 22(2), 221241.Google Scholar
Elliott, K. C. (2017). A tapestry of values: An introduction to values in science. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Elms, A. C. (1975). The crisis of confidence in social psychology. American Psychologist, 30(10), 967976.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Emerson, R. M., Fretz, R. I., & Shaw, L. L. (2001). Participant observation and fieldnotes. In Atkinson, P., Coffey, A., Delamont, S., Lofland, J., & Lofland, L. (eds.), Handbook of ethnography (pp. 352368). London: SAGE Publications.Google Scholar
Emirbayer, M., & Goldberg, C. A. (2005). Pragmatism, Bourdieu, and collective emotions in contentious politics. Theory and Society, 34(5), 469518.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Erikson, E. H. (1968). Identity. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Feist, G. J. (2008). The psychology of science and the origins of the scientific mind. New Haven: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Feyerabend, P. (1975). Against method. Scranton: Verso.Google Scholar
Fisher, H. (2017) Person, psychologist, and psychology suspended in a phase 1 science. Theory & Psychology, 27(4), 524549.Google Scholar
Flanagan, O. J. (1981). Psychology, progress, and the problem of reflexivity: A study in the epistemological foundations of psychology. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 17(3), 375386.Google Scholar
Fox Keller, E. (1995). Reflections on gender and science. New Haven: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Frappier, M., Meynell, L., & Brown, J. R. (eds.). (2012). Thought experiments in philosophy, science, and the arts (Vol. 11). Abingdon and New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Freeman, M. (2014). The priority of the other: Thinking and living beyond the self. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Frigg, R., & Hunter, M. (eds.). (2010). Beyond mimesis and convention: Representation in art and science (Vol. 262). New York: Springer Science & Business Media.Google Scholar
Gendler, T. S. (2004). Thought experiments rethought – and reperceived. Philosophy of Science, 71, 11521163.Google Scholar
Gentner, D. (1983). Structure‐mapping: A theoretical framework for analogy. Cognitive Science, 7(2), 155170.Google Scholar
Gergen, K. J. (1973). Social psychology as history. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 26(2), 309320.Google Scholar
Gergen, K. J. (1985). The social constructionist movement in modern psychology. American Psychologist, 40(3), 266275.Google Scholar
Gergen, K. J. (1990). Metaphor, metatheory, and the social world. In Leary, D. (ed.), Metaphor in the history of psychology (pp. 267299). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gergen, K. J. (2001). Preface. In Gergen, K. (ed.), Social construction in context (pp. ixx). London: SAGE.Google Scholar
Gesn, P. R., & Ickes, W. (1999). The development of meaning contexts for empathic accuracy: Channel and sequence effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(4), 746761.Google Scholar
Giere, R. N. (1999). >Using models to represent reality. In Magnani, L., Nersessian, N., & Thagard, P. (eds.), Model-based reasoning in scientific discovery (pp. 4157). New York: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Giere, R. N. (2010). Scientific perspectivism. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Giorgi, A. (1970). Psychology as a human science: A phenomenologically based approach. Oxford: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Goertzen, J. R. (2008). On the possibility of unification: The reality and nature of the crisis in psychology. Theory & Psychology, 18(6), 829852.Google Scholar
Gooding, D. (1990). Experiment and the making of meaning. Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Gorman, S., & Gorman, J. (2017). Denying to the grave: Why we ignore the facts that will save us. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gould, S. (ed.). (2001). The value of science: Essential writings of Henri Poincaré. New York: Modern Library.Google Scholar
Greenwood, J. D. (2015). A conceptual history of psychology: Exploring the tangled web. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Grice, J., Barrett, P., Cota, L., Felix, C., Taylor, Z., Garner, S., & Vest, A. (2017). Four bad habits of modern psychologists. Behavioral Sciences, 7(3), 53.Google Scholar
Gruber, H. (1974). Darwin on man: A psychological study of scientific creativity. New York: Dutton.Google Scholar
Guba, E. G. (ed.). (1990a). The paradigm dialog. Thousand Oaks: SAGE.Google Scholar
Guba, E. G. (1990b). The alternative paradigm dialog. In Guba, E. G. (ed.), The paradigm dialog (pp. 1730). Thousand Oaks: SAGE.Google Scholar
Hacking, I. (1983). Representing and intervening: Introductory topics in the philosophy of natural science. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hadamard, J. (1945). The mathematician's mind: The psychology of invention in the mathematical field. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Haig, B. D. (2014). Investigating the psychological world: Scientific method in the behavioral sciences. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hanson, N. R. (1958). Patterns of discovery. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Haraway, D. (1988). Situated knowledges: The science question in feminism and the privilege of partial perspectiveFeminist Studies,  14(3), 575599.Google Scholar
Harding, S. G. (1986). The science question in feminism. Ithaca: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Harding, S. G. (2015). Objectivity and diversity: Another logic of scientific research. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Harré, R. (1992). What is real in psychology: A plea for persons. Theory & Psychology, 2(2), 153158.Google Scholar
Harré, R. (2012). Positioning theory: Moral dimensions of social-cultural psychology. In Valsiner, J. (ed.), The Oxford handbook of culture and psychology (pp. 196206). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Harré, R., & Krausz, M. (1995). Varieties of relativism. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Hartley, D. (2013). Observations on man: His frame, his duty, and his expectations.Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Originally published 1749.Google Scholar
Heidbreder, E. (1933). Seven psychologies. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Henriques, G. (2003). The tree of knowledge system and the theoretical unification of psychology. Review of General Psychology, 7(2), 150182.Google Scholar
Hesse, M. (1963). Models and analogies in science. London: Sheed and Ward.Google Scholar
Higgins, A. (1991).The just community approach to moral education: Evolution of the idea and recent findings. In Kurtines, W. M. & Gewirtz, J. L. (eds.), Handbook of moral behavior and development, Vol.3 (pp. 111141). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hockney, D. (1975). The bifurcation of scientific theories and indeterminacy of translation. Philosophy of Science, 42(4), 411427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoffman, M. L. (2001). Empathy and moral development: Implications for caring and justice. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hoffman, R. (1980). Metaphor in science. In Honeck, R. P. & Hoffman, R. R. (eds.), Cognitive psychology and figurative language. Hillsdale: L. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hoffman, E., Myerberg, N. R., & Morawski, J. G. (2015). Acting otherwise: Resistance, agency, and subjectivities in Milgram’s studies of obedienceTheory & Psychology,  25(5), 670689.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofstadter, D. (1995). A review of Mental Leaps: Analogy in Creative Thought. AI Magazine, 16(3), 75.Google Scholar
Hofstadter, D., & Sander, E. (2013). Surfaces and essences: Analogy as the fuel and fire of thinking. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Hudson, R. 2014. Seeing things: The philosophy of reliable observation. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hutto, D. D., & Myin, E. (2017). Evolving enactivism: Basic minds meet content. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Isaacson, W. (2017). Leonardo Da Vinci. New York: Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
James, H. (1934). The art of the novel. New York: Charles Scribner & Sons. Originally published 1909.Google Scholar
James, H. (2000). The Bostonians. London: Penguin Books. Originally published 1886, Macmillan & Co.Google Scholar
James, H. (2009). The golden bowl. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Originally published 1904; author’s preface published 1909.Google Scholar
James, H. (2010). What Maisie knew. London: Penguin Classics. Originally published 1897.Google Scholar
James, H. (2016). A small boy and others. In Horne, P. (ed.), Henry James: Autobiographies (pp. 1250). New York: Library of America.Google Scholar
James, W. (1878). Remarks on Spencer’s definition of mind as correspondence. Journal of Speculative Philosophy, XII (1), 118.Google Scholar
James, W. (1879). The sentiment of rationality. Mind, 4(15), 317346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology, Vols. 1 & 2. New York: Henry Holt.Google Scholar
James, W. (1892). Psychology, the briefer course. New York: Henry Holt.Google Scholar
James, W. (1987). The varieties of religious experience: A study in human nature. Being the Gifford Lectures on Natural Religion Delivered at Edinburgh in 1901–1902. In William, James, Writings 1902–1910, pp. 1478. New York: Library of America. Originally published 1902.Google Scholar
James, W. (2003). Pragmatism: A new name for some old ways of thinking. Vescio, B. (ed.). New York: Barnes & Noble. Originally published 1907.Google Scholar
James, W. (2007). The principles of psychology, Vol. 1. New York: Cosimo Classics. Originally published 1890.Google Scholar
Johnson, M. (2007). The meaning of the body. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1983). Mental models: Towards a cognitive science of language, inference, and consciousness (No. 6). Boston: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Josselson, R., & Hopkins, B. (2015). Narrative psychology and life stories. In Martin, J., Sugarman, J., & Slaney, K. (eds.), The Wiley handbook of theoretical and philosophical psychology (pp. 219233). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Kant, I. (1960). Observations on the feeling of the beautiful and sublime. J. Goldthwait (trans.). Berkeley: University of California Press. Originally published 1764.Google Scholar
Kellert, S. H., Longino, H. E., & Waters, C. K. (eds.). (2006). Scientific pluralism [electronic resource] (Vol. 19). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Kelly, G. A. (1955). The psychology of personal constructs. Volume 1: A theory of personality. New York: W.W. Norton.Google Scholar
Kim, U. (1990). Indigenous psychology: Science and applications. In Brislin, R. W. (ed.), Cross-cultural research and methodology series, Vol. 14. Applied cross-cultural psychology (pp. 142160). Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications, Inc.Google Scholar
Kind, A. (ed.). (2016). The Routledge handbook of philosophy of imagination. Abingdon and New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Kirschner, S. R. (2006). Psychology and pluralism: Toward the psychological studies. Journal of Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology, 26(1–2), 117.Google Scholar
Kitcher, P. (1993). The advancement of science: Science without legend, objectivity without illusions. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kitcher, P. (2001). Science, truth, and democracy. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Knorr-Cetina, K. D. (2013). The manufacture of knowledge: An essay on the constructivist and contextual nature of science. Oxford: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Koch, S. (1981). The nature and limits of psychological knowledge: Lessons of a century qua “science.” American Psychologist, 36(3), 257269.Google Scholar
Koch, S. (1993). “Psychology” or “the psychological studies”? American Psychologist, 48(8), 902904.Google Scholar
Köhler, W. (1938). The place of value in a world of facts. Oxford: Liveright.Google Scholar
Köhler, W. (1973). The mentality of apes. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. Originally published 1925.Google Scholar
Kounios, J., & Beeman, M. (2015). The eureka factor: Aha moments, creative insight, and the brain. New York: Random House.Google Scholar
Kraus, M. (2017). Voice-only communication enhances empathic accuracy. American Psychologist, 72(7), 644654.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhn, T. (1962). Structure of scientific revolutions. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Kuhn, T. (1977). The essential tension. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lacey, H. (1999a). Is science value free? Values and scientific understanding. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Lacey, H. (1999b). On cognitive and social values: A reply to my critics. Science & Education, 8(1), 89103.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (2003). Metaphors we live by. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lamiell, J. T. (2009). Reviving person-centered inquiry in psychology: Why it’s erstwhile dormancy? In Valsiner, J., Molenaar, P. C. M., Lyra, M. C. D. P., & Chaudhary, N. (eds.), Dynamic process methodology in the social and developmental sciences (pp. 3144). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Latour, B., & Woolgar, S. (1979). Laboratory life: The construction of scientific facts. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Lauden, L. (1984). Science and values. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Leary, D. E. (1990). Psyche’s muse: The role of metaphor in the history of psychology. In Leary, D. E. (ed.), Metaphors in the history of psychology (pp. 178). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Leary, D. E. (1992). William, James and the art of human understandingAmerican Psychologist, 47(2), 152160.Google Scholar
Leonelli, S. (2016). Data-centric biology: A philosophical study. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Levitt, H. M., Motulsky, S. L., Wertz, F. J., Morrow, S. L., & Ponterotto, J. G. (2017). Recommendations for designing and reviewing qualitative research in psychology: Promoting methodological integrityQualitative Psychology,  4(1), 222.Google Scholar
Lewin, K. (1946). Behavior and development as a function of the total situation. Reprinted in Cartwright, D. (ed.), Field theory in social science: Selected papers by Kurt Lewin (pp. 238303). New York: Harper Torch Books, 1951.Google Scholar
Lincoln, Y. S. (1990). The making of a constructivist: A remembrance of transformations past. In Guba, E. (ed.), The paradigm dialog (pp. 4666). Newbury Park: Sage.Google Scholar
Lincoln, Y. S., & Guba, E. G. (1985). Naturalistic inquiry (Vol. 75). Beverly Hills: Sage.Google Scholar
Longino, H. (1983). Beyond “bad science”: Skeptical reflections on the value-freedom of scientific inquiry. Science, Technology, & Human Values, 8(1), 717.Google Scholar
Longino, H. (1990). Science as social knowledge: Value and objectivity in scientific inquiry. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Longino, H. E. (1996). Cognitive and non-cognitive values in science: Rethinking the dichotomy. In Hankinson, L. & Nelson, J. (eds.), Feminism, science, and the philosophy of science (pp. 3958). Dordrecht: Springer.Google Scholar
Longino, H. E. (2002). The fate of knowledge. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Longino, H. E. (2004). How values can be good for science. In Machamer, P. K. & Wolters, G. (eds.), Science, values, and objectivity (pp. 127142).  Pittsburgh: University of Pittsburgh Press.Google Scholar
Mach, E. (1976). Knowledge and error: Sketches on the psychology of enquiry. T. J. McCormack & P. Filches, trans. Dordrecht: D. Reidel. Original work published 1905.Google Scholar
Machamer, P., & Osbeck, L. (2004). The social in the epistemic. In Machamer, P. & Wolters, G. (eds.), Science, values, and objectivity (pp. 113). Pittsburgh: University of Pittsburgh Press.Google Scholar
Machamer, P., & Wolters, G. (2004). Introduction. In Machamer, P. & Wolters, G. (eds.), Science, values, and objectivity (pp. 7889). Pittsburgh: University of Pittsburgh Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacLeod, M., & Nersessian, N. J. (2015). Modeling systems-level dynamics: Understanding without mechanistic explanation in integrative systems biology. Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part C: Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences, 49, 111.Google Scholar
Magnani, L., Nersessian, N., & Thagard, P. (eds.). (1999). Model-based reasoning in scientific discovery. Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Mahoney, M. (2004). Scientist as subject: The psychological imperative. Cambridge: Ballinger. Originally published 1976.Google Scholar
Malone, K., & Barbarino, G. (2009). Narrations of race in STEM settings: Identity formation and its discontents. Science Education, 93(3), 485510.Google Scholar
Martin, J. (2017). Studying persons in context: Taking social psychological reality seriously. New Ideas in Psychology, 44: 2833.Google Scholar
Martin, J., & Bickhard, M. (2013). Introducing persons and the psychology of personhood. In Martin, J. & Bickhard, M. (eds.), The psychology of personhood: Philosophical, historical, social-developmental, and narrative perspectives (pp. 117). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Martin, J., Sugarman, J., & Hickenbottom, S. (eds.). (2010). Persons: Understanding psychological selfhood and agency. New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masterman, M. (1970). The nature of a paradigm. In Lakatos, I. & Musgrave, A. (eds.), Criticism and the growth of knowledge (pp. 5989). London: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Matravers, D. (2010). Why we should give up on the imagination. Midwest Studies in Philosophy, 34(1), 190199.Google Scholar
Maxwell, S. E., Lau, M. Y., & Howard, G. S. (2015). Is psychology suffering from a replication crisis? What does “failure to replicate” really mean? American Psychologist, 70(6), 487498.Google Scholar
McAdams, D. P. (2014). The life narrative at midlife. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 2014 (145), 5769.Google Scholar
McAllister, J. (2012). Thought experiment and the exercise of imagination in science. In Frappier, M., Meynell, L., & Brown, J. R. (eds.), Thought experiments in philosophy, science, and the arts (pp. 1129). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
McDaniel, K. (2014). Edith Stein: On the problem of empathy. In Schliesser, E. (ed.), Ten neglected philosophical classics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, self and society (Vol. 111). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N. (2002). Imitation as a mechanism of social cognition: Origins of empathy, theory of mind, and the representation of action. In Goswami, U. (ed.), Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development (pp. 625). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Menand, L. (2001). The metaphysical club: A story of ideas in America. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.Google Scholar
Menary, R. (2007). Cognitive integration: Mind and cognition unbounded. New York: Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
Mill, J. S. (1882). August Comte and positivism, 3rd ed. London: Trübner.Google Scholar
Miller, R. B. (2004). Facing human suffering: Psychology and psychotherapy as moral engagement. Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Mills, J. A. (2010). Incorporating realist fiction into dialogical theories of the self. Theory & Psychology, 20(5), 621640.Google Scholar
Mitchell, S. D. (2002). Integrative pluralism. Biology and Philosophy, 17(1), 5570.Google Scholar
Mitroff, I. (1974). The subjective side of science: Philosophical inquiry into the psychology of the Apollo Moon Scientists. Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Moghaddam, F.M. (2004). From “psychology in literature” to “psychology is literature.” Theory & Psychology, 14(4), 505525.Google Scholar
Morawski, J. G. (1988). The rise of experimentation in American psychology. New Haven: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Morawski, J. G. (2005). Reflexivity and the psychologist. History of the Human Sciences, 18(4), 77105.Google Scholar
Morrow, S. L. (2005). Quality and trustworthiness in qualitative research in counseling psychology. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 52(2), 250260.Google Scholar
Moser, K. S. (2000). Metaphor analysis in psychology – Method, theory, and fields of application. In Forum Qualitative Sozialforschung/Forum: Qualitative Social Research, 1(2), Art 21.Google Scholar
Muelder, W. G. (1998). Foreword. In Burrow, R., Personalism: A critical introduction (pp. xxiii). St. Louis: Chalice Press.Google Scholar
Nemetz, A. (1958). Metaphor: The Daedalus of discourse. Thought, 33(3), 417442.Google Scholar
Nersessian, N. (1984). Faraday to Einstein: Constructing meaning in scientific theories. Dordrecht: Martinus Nijhoff/Kluwer.Google Scholar
Nersessian, N. J. (1992). How do scientists think? Capturing the dynamics of conceptual change in science. Cognitive Models of Science, 15, 344.Google Scholar
Nersessian, N. J. (1999). Model-based reasoning in conceptual change. In Magnani, L., Nersessian, N., & Thagard, P. (eds.), Model-based reasoning in scientific discovery (pp. 522). New York: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Nersessian, N. J. (2002). The cognitive basis of model-based reasoning in science. In Carruthers, P., Stitch, S., & Siegal, M. (eds.), The cognitive basis of science (pp. 133153). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Nersessian, N. (2005). Interpreting scientific and engineering practices: Integrating the cognitive, social, and cultural dimensions. In Gorman, M. E., Tweney, R. D., Gooding, D. C., & Kincannon, A. P. (eds.), Scientific and technological thinking (pp. 1756). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Nersessian, N. J. (2008). Creating scientific concepts. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Nersessian, N. J. (2012). Engineering concepts: The interplay between concept formation and modeling practices in bioengineering sciences. Mind, Culture, and Activity, 19(3), 222239.Google Scholar
Ortner, S. (1995). Resistance and the problem of ethnographic refusal. Comparative Studies in Society and History, 37(1), 173193.Google Scholar
Osbeck, L. M. (1993). Social constructionism and the pragmatic standard. Theory & Psychology, 3(3), 337349.Google Scholar
Osbeck, L. M. (1995). Social constructionism and the pragmatic standard revisited: A reply to Botschner. Theory & Psychology, 5(1), 153157.Google Scholar
Osbeck, L. M. (2005). Method and theoretical psychology. Theory & Psychology, 15(1), 526.Google Scholar
Osbeck, L. M., & Held, B. S. (eds.). (2014). Rational intuition: Philosophical roots, scientific investigations. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Osbeck, L. M., & Nersessian, N. J. (2006). The Distribution of RepresentationJournal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 36(2), 141160.Google Scholar
Osbeck, L. M., & Nersessian, N. J. (2010). Forms of positioning in interdisciplinary science practice and their epistemic effects. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 40(2), 136161.Google Scholar
Osbeck, L. M., & Nersessian, N. J. (2012). The acting person in scientific practice. In Proctor, R. & Capaldi, J. (eds.), Psychology of science: Implicit and explicit reasoning (pp. 89111). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Osbeck, L. M., & Nersessian, N. J. (2013). Beyond motivation and metaphor: “Scientific passions” and anthropomorphism. In EPSA11 Perspectives and Foundational Problems in Philosophy of Science (pp. 455466). Cham: Springer International.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Osbeck, L. M., & Nersessian, N. J. (2015). Prolegomena to an empirical philosophy of science. In Wagenknecht, S., Nersessian, N. J., & Andersen, H. (eds.), Empirical philosophy of science: Introducing qualitative methods into philosophy of science (pp. 1335). Cham: Springer International.Google Scholar
Osbeck, L. M., & Nersessian, N. J. (2017). Epistemic identities in interdisciplinary science. Perspectives on Science, 25(2), 226260.Google Scholar
Osbeck, L. M., Nersessian, N. J., Malone, K. R., & Newstetter, W. C. (2011). Science as psychology: Sense-making and identity in science practice. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Pandit, G. L., & Dosch, H. G. (2013). The frontiers of theory development in physics: A methodological study in its dynamical complexity. Los Angeles: Trebol Press.Google Scholar
Park, K. (2011). Observation in the margins, 500–1500. In Daston, L. & Lunbeck, E. (eds.), Histories of scientific observation (pp. 1544). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Parker, I. (2015). Critical discursive psychology, 2nd ed. London: Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
Peirce, C. S. (1868). Questions concerning certain faculties claimed for man. The Journal of Speculative Philosophy, 2(2), 103114.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1962). Play, dreams and imitation in children. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Plotkin, H. C. (2002). The imagined world made real: Towards a natural science of culture. New Brunswick: Rutgers University Press.Google Scholar
Poincaré, H. (2001a). The value of science. In Gould, S. J. (ed.), The value of science: Essential writings of Henri Poincaré (pp. 181337). New York: Modern Library. Originally published 1913.Google Scholar
Poincaré, H. (2001b). Science and method. In Gould, S. J. (ed.), The value of science: Essential writings of Henri Poincaré (pp. 357558). New York: Modern Library. Originally published 1914.Google Scholar
Polanyi, M. (1964). Science, faith, and society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Originally published 1946.Google Scholar
Polanyi, M. (1974). Personal knowledge: Towards a post-critical philosophy. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Originally published 1958.Google Scholar
Polya, G. (1945). How to solve it. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Pomata, G. (2011). Observation rising: Birth of an epistemic genre, 1500–1650. In Daston, L. & Lunbeck, E. (eds.), Histories of scientific observation (pp. 4580). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Ponterotto, Joseph G. (2005). Qualitative research in counseling psychology: A primer on research paradigms and philosophy of science. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 52( 2), 126136.Google Scholar
Power, C., Higgins, A., & Kohlberg, L. (1989). Lawrence Kohlberg’s approach to moral education: A study of three democratic high schools. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Pribram, K. H. (1990). From metaphors to models: The use of analogy in neuropsychology. In Leary, David E. (ed.), Metaphors in the history of psychology (pp. 79103). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Price, D. D., & Barrell, J. J. (2012). Inner experience and neuroscience: Merging both perspectives. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Proctor, R. (1991). Value-free science? Purity and power in modern knowledge. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Proctor, R. W., & Capaldi, E. J. (2006). Why science matters: Understanding the methods of psychological research. Malden: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Putnam, H. (1981). Reason, truth and history. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Rabinow, P., & Bennett, G. (2012). Designing human practices: An experiment with synthetic biology. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Rabinow, P., & Stavrianakis, A. (2013). Demands of the day: On the logic of anthropological inquiry. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Robinson, D. N. (2014). Science, scientism, and explanation. In Williams, D. & Robinson, D. N., Scientism: The new orthodoxy (pp. 2340). London: Bloomsbury.Google Scholar
Rogers, C. R. (1963). Toward a science of the personJournal of Humanistic Psychology, 3(2), 7292.Google Scholar
Ross, W. D. (1928). Complete works of Aristotle. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Sarbin, T. R. (1986). Narrative psychology: The storied nature of human conduct. Westport: Praeger/Greenwood.Google Scholar
Sarbin, T. R. (1993). The narrative as the root metaphor for contextualism. In Hayes, S. C., Hayes, L. J., Reese, H. W., & Sarbin, T. R. (eds.), Varieties of scientific contextualism (pp. 5165). Reno: Context Press.Google Scholar
Schatzman, L., & Strauss, A. (1973). Field research: Strategies for a natural sociology. London: Pearson.Google Scholar
Schön, D. (1963). Displacement of concepts. London: Tavistock Publications.Google Scholar
Scudder, H. E. (1886). The Bostonians, by Henry James. The Atlantic Monthly. Retrieved from https://www.theatlantic.com/past/docs/unbound/classrev/thebosto.htmGoogle Scholar
Shapere, D. (1982). The concept of observation in science and philosophy. Philosophy of Science, 49(4), 485525.Google Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (2004). Creativity in science: Chance, logic, genius, and zeitgeist. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York: Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
Smith, L. D. (1990). Metaphors of knowledge and behavior in the behaviorist tradition. In Leary, D. (ed.), Metaphors in the history of psychology (pp. 239265). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Smythe, W. E. (ed.). (1998). Toward a psychology of persons. New York: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Staats, A. W. (1991). Unified positivism and unification psychology: Fad or new field? American Psychologist, 46(9), 899912.Google Scholar
Staats, A. W. (1999). Unifying psychology requires new infrastructure, theory, method, and a research agenda. Review of General Psychology, 3, 313.Google Scholar
Stafford, B. M. (2001). Visual analogy: Consciousness as the art of connecting. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Stam, H. (1998). The dispersal of subjectivity and the problem of persons in psychology. In Smythe, W. (ed.), Toward a psychology of persons (pp. 221244). New York: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Stein, E. (1964). On the problem of empathy. W. Stein, trans. Dordrecht: Springer Science and Business. Originally published 1917.Google Scholar
Stroebe, W., & Strack, F. (2014). The alleged crisis and the illusion of exact replication. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9(1), 5971.Google Scholar
Stuart, M. (2018). How thought experiments increase understanding. In Stuart, Michael T., Yiftach, J. H. Fehige, & Brown, James Robert (eds.), The Routledge companion to thought experiments (pp. 526544). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Sundararajan, L. (2015). Indigenous psychology: Grounding science in culture, why and how? Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 45(1), 6481.Google Scholar
Teo, T. (2015). Critical psychology: A geography of intellectual engagement and resistance. American Psychologist, 70(3), 243254.Google Scholar
Teo, T. (2017a). From psychological science to the psychological humanities: Building a general theory of subjectivity. Review of General Psychology, 21(4), 281291.Google Scholar
Teo, T. (2017b, March). The epistemic, practical, and emancipatory interests of the psychological humanities in theoretical refection. In T. Teo (Chair), Reenvisioning theoretical psychology: Rebels with(out) a cause? Symposium (Presidential Forum) conducted at the Midwinter Meeting of the Society for Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology, Richmond, VA.Google Scholar
Teo, T. (2018).The role of values, power, and money in the psydisciplines. In Outline of theoretical psychology: Palgrave studies in the theory and history of psychology (pp. 179199). London: Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
Thagard, P. (2014). Creative intuition: How EUREKA results from three neural mechanisms. In Osbeck, L. & Held, B. (eds.), Rational intuition: Philosophical roots, scientific investigations (pp. 287306). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Thorndike, R. L., & Hagen, E. P. (1969). Measurement and evaluation in psychology and education. New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Tissaw, M. A., & Osbeck, L. M. (2007). On critical engagement with the mainstream: Introduction. Theory & Psychology, 17(2), 155168.Google Scholar
Titchener, E. B. (1899). Structural and functional psychology. Philosophical Review, 8, 290299.Google Scholar
Titchener, E. B. (1912). The schema of introspection. American Journal of Psychology, 23, 485508.Google Scholar
Tjeltveit, A. (1999). Ethics and values in psychotherapy. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1948). Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychological Review, 55(4), 189208.Google Scholar
Toon, A. (2012). Models as make-believe: Imagination, fiction and scientific representation. London: Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
Tweney, R. D. (1985). Faraday’s discovery of induction: A cognitive approach. In Gooding, D. (ed.), Faraday rediscovered (pp. 189209). London: Palgrave.Google Scholar
Varela, F. J., Thompson, E., & Rosch, E. (2017). The embodied mind: Cognitive science and human experience. Cambridge: MIT Press. Originally published 1991.Google Scholar
Vescio, B. (2003). Introduction. In James, W., Pragmatism: A new name for some old ways of thinking (pp. ix–xiiii) New York: Barnes & Noble.Google Scholar
Vosniadou, S., & Ortony, A. (eds.). (1989). Similarity and analogical reasoning. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher mental process. M. Cole, trans. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Wagenknecht, S., Nersessian, N. J., & Andersen, H. (eds.). (2015). Empirical philosophy of science: Introducing qualitative methods into philosophy of science. Cham: Springer International.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, J. B. (1913). Psychology as the behaviorist views it. Psychological Review, 20(2), 158177.Google Scholar
Weber, M. (1946). In Gerth, H. H. & Wright Mills, C. (trans. and ed.), Max Weber: Essays in Sociology (pp. 129156). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Weick, K. E. (1995). Sensemaking in organizations. Thousand Oaks: SAGE.Google Scholar
Weinfurt, K. (2003). Book review: Experience, grammar, and the stuff of persons. Theory & Psychology, 15(3), 407409.Google Scholar
Wertheimer, M. (1981). Einstein: The thinking that led to the theory of relativity. In Tweney, R., Doherty, M., & Mynatt, C. (eds.), On scientific thinking (pp. 192211). New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Wertz, F. J. (2014). Qualitative inquiry in the history of psychologyQualitative Psychology, 1(1), 416.Google Scholar
Wertz, F. J. (2016). Outline of the relationship among transcendental phenomenology, phenomenological psychology, and the sciences of persons. Schutzian Research: A Yearbook in Lifeworldly Phenomenology and Qualitative Social Sciences, 8, 139162.Google Scholar
Wertz, F. J., Charmaz, K., McMullen, L. M., Josselson, R., Anderson, R., & McSpadden, E. (2011). Five ways of doing qualitative analysis. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Wheelwright, P. (1962). Metaphor and reality. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
Whewell, W. (1844). The philosophy of the inductive sciences (2 vols., 2nd ed.). London: J. W. Parker.Google Scholar
Williams, R., & Robinson, D. (2015). Scientism: The new orthodoxy. Sydney: Bloomsbury Academic.Google Scholar
Wittgenstein, L. (1953). Philosophical investigations. G. E. M. Anscombe, trans. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Woolhouse, C. (2017). Multimodal life history narrative. Narrative Inquiry, 27(1), 109131.Google Scholar
Wundt, W. (1882). Die Aufgaben der experimentellen psychologie. Unsere Zeit, 18, 389406.Google Scholar
Zachar, P. (2014). A metaphysics of psychopathology. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • References
  • Lisa Osbeck, University of West Georgia
  • Book: Values in Psychological Science
  • Online publication: 23 November 2018
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316471302.008
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • References
  • Lisa Osbeck, University of West Georgia
  • Book: Values in Psychological Science
  • Online publication: 23 November 2018
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316471302.008
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • References
  • Lisa Osbeck, University of West Georgia
  • Book: Values in Psychological Science
  • Online publication: 23 November 2018
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316471302.008
Available formats
×