Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-q99xh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-29T01:21:32.193Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The influence of toys on the behaviour and welfare of kennelled dogs

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 January 2023

DL Wells*
Affiliation:
Canine Behaviour Centre, School of Psychology, Queen's University Belfast, Belfast BT7 1NN, Northern Ireland, UK
*
d.wells@qub.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Core share and HTML view are not available for this content. However, as you have access to this content, a full PDF is available via the ‘Save PDF’ action button.

This study explored the influence of five toys (squeaky ball, non-squeaky ball, Nylabone chew, tug rope and Boomer ball) on the behaviour of 32 adult dogs housed in a rescue shelter. The dogs were exposed to each toy separately for six days, with an intervening period of one day between toys. The dogs' location in their kennels (front or back), activity (moving, standing, sitting or resting) and vocalisation (barking, quiet or other) were recorded over 4 h at 10 min intervals on Days 1, 3 and 5 during a control condition (no toy present) and during five experimental (toy) conditions. Whether or not the dogs were observed playing with the toys during the experimental conditions was also recorded. The dogs spent relatively little (<8%) of the overall observation time playing with the toys. The toys elicited varying degrees of interest, with dogs showing a preference for the Nylabone chew over the other toys. The dogs' interest in the toys waned over time, but the speed of habituation to the Nylabone chew was slower than to any of the other toys. The dogs' activity was significantly related to toy condition: dogs spent more time moving and less time standing during the Nylabone chew, squeaky ball and non-squeaky ball conditions than during any of the other conditions. It is suggested that the welfare of kennelled dogs may be slightly enhanced by the addition of suitable toys to their kennels. It is advised, however, that toys are rotated to encourage exploration and reduce habituation. The provision of other forms of environmental enrichment is also recommended.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 2004 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

References

Broom, DM and Johnson, KG 1993 Stress and Animal Welfare. Chapman and Hall: London, UKCrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeLuca, AM and Kranda, KC 1992 Environmental enrichment in a large animal facility. Lab Animal 21: 3844Google Scholar
Fox, MW 1965 Environmental factors influencing stereotyped and allelomimetic behaviour in animals. Laboratory Animal Care 15: 363370Google ScholarPubMed
Hediger, H 1950 Wild Animals in Captivity. Butterworth: London, UKGoogle Scholar
Hediger, H 1955 Studies of the Psychology and Behaviour of Captive Animals in Zoos and Circuses. Butterworth: London, UKGoogle Scholar
Hetts, S, Clark, JD, Calpin, JP, Arnold, CE and Mateo, JM 1992 Influence of housing conditions on beagle behaviour. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 34: 137155CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howell, DC 1992 Statistical Methods for Psychology. Duxbury Press: California, USAGoogle Scholar
Hubrecht, RC 1993 A comparison of social and laboratory environmental enrichment methods for laboratory housed dogs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 37: 345361CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hubrecht, RC 1995 Enrichment in puppyhood and its effects on later behaviour of dogs. Laboratory Animal Science 45: 7075Google ScholarPubMed
Ladd, JK, Albright, JL, Beck, AM and Ladd, BT 1992 Behavioral and physiological studies on the effect of music on animals. Journal of Animal Science 70: 170 (Suppl)Google Scholar
Larsson, F, Winblad, B and Mohammed, AH 2002 Psychological stress and environmental adaptation in enriched vs impoverished housed rats. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior 73: 193207CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, P and Bateson, P 1986 Measuring Behaviour. Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UKGoogle Scholar
Mertens, PA and Unshelm, J 1996 Effects of group and individual housing on the behavior of kennelled dogs in animal shelters. Anthrozoos 9: 4051CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morris, D 1964 The response of animals to a restricted environment. Symposium of the Zoological Society of London 13: 99118Google Scholar
Newberry, RC 1995 Environmental enrichment: increasing the biological relevance of captive environments. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 44: 229243CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poole, TB 1992 The nature and evolution of behavioural needs in mammals. Animal Welfare 1: 203220Google Scholar
Renner, MJ and Lussier, JP 2002 Environmental enrichment for the captive spectacled bear (Tremarctos ornatus). Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior 73: 279283CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rooney, NJ, Bradshaw, JWS and Robinson, IH 2000 A comparison of dog–dog and dog–human play behaviour. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 66: 235248CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shepherdson, DJ, Mellen, JD and Hutchins, M 1998 Second Nature: Environmental Enrichment for Captive Animals. Smithsonian Institution Press: London, UKGoogle Scholar
Wells, DL 1996 The welfare of dogs in an animal rescue shelter. PhD Thesis, Queen's University, Belfast, UKGoogle Scholar
Wells, DL, Graham, L and Hepper, PG 2002a The influence of length of time in a rescue shelter on the behaviour of kennelled dogs. Animal Welfare 11: 317325Google Scholar
Wells, DL, Graham, L and Hepper, PG 2002b The influence of auditory stimulation on the behaviour of dogs housed in a rescue shelter. Animal Welfare 11: 385393Google Scholar
Wells, DL and Hepper, PG 1992 The behaviour of dogs in a rescue shelter. Animal Welfare 1: 171186Google Scholar
Wells, DL and Hepper, PG 1998 A note on the influence of visual conspecific contact on the behaviour of sheltered dogs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 60: 8388CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wells, DL and Hepper, PG 2000 The influence of environmental change on the behaviour of sheltered dogs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 68: 151162CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed