Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-7cvxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T17:28:10.992Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Is the relation between phonological memory and foreign language learning accounted for by vocabulary acquisition?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

Elisabet Service*
Affiliation:
University of Helsinki
Viljo Kohonen
Affiliation:
University of Tampere
*
Elisabet Service, Department of Psychology, P.O. Box 11, FIN-00014University of Helsinki, Finland

Abstract

In an earlier study, Service (1992) found that phonological short-term memory, reflected in the ability to repeat English-sounding pseudowords, was a good predictor of learning English as a foreign language over a period of three school years. This study attempts to find out to what extent foreign vocabulary learning is the critical learning process related to pseudoword repetition accuracy. Regression analyses on repetition data and learning measures revealed significant correlations between pseudoword repetition and foreign language learning, even after a measure of general academic achievement had been partialed out. In a fixed-order multiple regression analysis – with predictors entered in the order of general academic achievement, foreign language vocabulary, and pseudoword repetition – no additional variance in any foreign language measure studied could be accounted for by pseudoword repetition in the third step. When vocabulary in the second step was replaced by performance in foreign language essay writing or the correct reproduction of dictated discourse – both highly related to overall foreign language performance – repetition significantly increased the proportion of accounted variance, especially for two foreign language vocabulary tasks and a listening comprehension task. Phonological memory may, therefore, be specifically related to foreign vocabulary learning.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1995

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Baddeley, A. (1993). Short-term phonological memory and long-term learning: A single case study. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 5, 129148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A., Papagno, C., & Vallar, G. (1988). When long-term learning depends on short- term storage. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 586595.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Ellis, N., & Beaton, A. (1993). Factors affecting the learning of foreign language vocabulary: Imagery keyword mediators and phonological short-term memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46 A, 533558.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1989). Evaluation of the role of phonological STM in the development of vocabulary in children: A longitudinal study. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 200213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1990). The role of phonological memory in vocabulary acquisition: A study of young children learning arbitrary names of toys. British Journal of Psychology, 81, 439454.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1993). Working memory and language. Hove: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C. S., Emslie, H., & Baddeley, A. B. (1992). Phonological memory and vocabulary development during the early school years: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 28, 887898.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
loup, G. (1989). Immigrant children who have failed to acquire native English. In Gass, S., Madden, C., Preston, D., & Seinker, L. (Eds.), Variation in language acquisition: Vol. 2. Psycholinguistic issues. Philadelphia: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Kohonen, V. (1987). Towards experiential learning of elementary English 1. Tampere: University of Tampere.Google Scholar
Martin, R. C. (1993). Short-term memory and sentence processing: Evidence from neuropsychology. Memory & Cognition, 21, 176183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, R. C., & Breedin, S. D. (1992). Dissociations between speech perception and phonological short-term memory deficits. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 9, 509534.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Papagno, C., Valentine, T., & Baddeley, A. (1991). Phonological short-term memory and foreign-language vocabulary learning. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 331347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Papagno, C., & Vallar, G. (1992). Phonological short-term memory and the learning of novel words: The effect of phonological similarity and item length. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 44A, 4767.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Service, E. (1992). Phonology, working memory and foreign-language learning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 45A, 2150.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Service, E., & Craik, F. (1993). Differences between young and older adults in learning a foreign vocabulary. Journal of Memory and Language. 32, 608623.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skehan, P. (1989). Language aptitude. London: Edward Arnold.Google Scholar
Snowling, M., Chiat, S., & Hulme, C. (1991). Words, nonwords, and phonological processes: Some comments on Gathercole, Willis, Emslie, and Baddeley. Applied Psycholinguistics, 12, 369373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar