Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-j824f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-18T09:36:21.783Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Narrative competence and underlying mechanisms in children with pragmatic language impairment

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 June 2011

MIEKE PAULINE KETELAARS*
Affiliation:
Radboud University Nijmegen
KINO JANSONIUS
Affiliation:
Sint Marie Eindhoven
JULIANE CUPERUS
Affiliation:
Sint Marie Eindhoven
LUDO VERHOEVEN
Affiliation:
Radboud University Nijmegen
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Mieke Ketelaars, Department of Special Education, Behavioural Science Institute, Radboud University Nijmegen, P.O. Box 9104, Nijmegen 6500 HE, The Netherlands. E-mail: M.Ketelaars@pwo.ru.nl

Abstract

This study investigated narrative competence in children with pragmatic language impairment (PLI) and the extent to which it is related to impairments in theory of mind and executive functioning (EF). Narrative competence was assessed using a retelling design in a group of 77 children with PLI and a control group of 77 typically developing children, aged 5. The children with PLI showed an overall poorer narrative competence as apparent in measures of narrative productivity, organization of content, and cohesion. Some of these differences could be attributed to language impairments. The remaining differences could be partly interpreted as pragmatic deficits. In typically developing children, narrative productivity skills were related to both theory of mind and EF, but only theory of mind explained unique variance once language ability was added to the model. In the PLI group, however, narrative productivity skills were solely related to EF, over and above language abilities. Organization of story content and cohesion were not related to any of the cognitive measures for either group. The results indicate that children with PLI show narrative deficits and that these deficits are related to EF.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Berman, R. A. (1988). On the ability to relate events in narrative. Discourse Processes, 11, 469497.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M. (1998). Development of the Children's Communication Checklist (CCC): A method for assessing qualitative aspects of communicative impairment in children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 879891.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bishop, D. V. M. (2000). Pragmatic language impairment: A correlate of SLI, a distinct subgroup, or part of the autistic continuum? In Leonard, L. B. & Bishop, D. V. M. (Eds.), Speech language impairments in children: Causes, characteristics, intervention and outcome (p. 305). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M., & Baird, G. (2001). Parent and teacher report of pragmatic aspects of communication: Use of the Children's Communication Checklist in a clinical setting. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 43, 809818.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bishop, D. V. M., & Edmundson, A. (1987). Language impaired 4-year-olds: Distinguishing transient from persistent impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 52, 156173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bishop, D. V. M., & Norbury, C. F. (2005a). Executive functions in children with communication impairments, in relation to autistic symptomatology 1: Generativity. Autism, 9, 727.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bishop, D. V. M., & Norbury, C. F. (2005b). Executive functions in children with communication impairments, in relation to autistic symptomatology 2: Response inhibition. Autism, 9, 2943.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Botting, N. (2002). Narrative as a tool for the assessment of linguistic and pragmatic impairments. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 18, 121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brook, S. L., & Bowler, D. M. (1992). Autism by another name? Semantic and pragmatic impairments in children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 22, 6181.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Capps, L., Losh, M., & Thurber, C. (2000). “The frog ate the bug and made his mouth sad”: Narrative competence in children with autism. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28, 193204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coelho, C. A. (2002). Story narratives of adults with closed head injury and non brain-injured adults: Influence of Socioeconomic status, elicitation task, and executive functioning. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45, 12321248.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conti-Ramsden, G., Crutchley, A., & Botting, N. (1997). The extent to which psychometric tests differentiate subgroups of children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 765777.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Villiers, J. G., & Pyers, J. E. (2002). Complements to cognition: A longitudinal study of the relationship between complex syntax and false-belief-understanding. Cognitive Development, 17, 10371060.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diehl, J. J., Bennetto, L., & Young, E. C. (2006). Story recall and narrative coherence of high-functioning children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 34, 87102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dodwell, K., & Bavin, E. L. (2007). Children with specific language impairment: An investigation of their narratives and memory. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 43, 201218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feagans, L., & Applebaum, M. I. (1986). Validation of language subtypes in learning disabled children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 78, 358364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flory, K., Milich, R., Lorch, E. P., Hayden, A. N., Strange, C., & Welsh, R. (2006). Online story comprehension among children with ADHD: Which core deficits are involved? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 34, 853865.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geurts, H. M., Verte, S., Oosterlaan, J., Roeyers, H., Hartman, C. A., Mulder, E. J., et al. (2004). Can the Children's Communication Checklist differentiate between children with autism, children with ADHD, and normal controls? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 14371453.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldberg, M. C., Mostofsky, S. H., Cutting, L. E., Mahone, E. M., Astor, B. C., Denckla, M. B., et al. (2005). Subtle executive impairment in children with autism and children with ADHD. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35, 279293.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hale, C. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2003). The influence of language on theory of mind: A training study. Developmental Science, 6, 346359.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hunt, K. W. (1970). Syntactic maturity in schoolchildren and adults. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 35, iii67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jansonius-Schultheiss, K., Borgers, M., DeBruin, B., & Stumpel, H. (2006). Renfrew's language scales Dutch adaptation. Amsterdam: Pro-education, HvA.Google Scholar
Kaufman, A. S., & Kaufman, N. L. (2004). Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children (2nd ed.). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Ketelaars, M. P., Cuperus, J. M., Van Daal, J., Jansonius, K., & Verhoeven, L. (2009). Screening for pragmatic language impairment: The potential of the Children's Communication Checklist. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 30, 952960.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Korkman, M., Kirk, U., & Kemp, S. (1998). NEPSY: A developmental neuropsychological assessment. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Liles, B. Z., Duffy, R. J., Merritt, D. D., & Purcell, S. L. (1995). Measurement of narrative discourse ability in children with language disorders. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 415425.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Losh, M., & Capps, L. (2003). Narrative ability in high-functioning children with autism or Asperger's syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33, 239251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mar, R. A. (2004). The neuropsychology of narrative: Story comprehension, story production and their interrelation. Neuropsychologia, 42, 14141434.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Merritt, D. D., & Liles, B. Z. (1989). Narrative analysis: Clinical applications of story generation and story retelling. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 54, 438447.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miniscalco, C., Hagberg, B., Kadesjö, B., Westerlund, M., & Gillberg, C. (2007). Narrative skills, cognitive profiles and neuropsychiatric disorders in 7-8-year-old children with late developing language. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 42, 665681.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Montgomery, J. W. (2003). Working memory and comprehension in children with specific language impairment: What we know so far. Journal of Communication Disorders, 36, 221231.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Montgomery, J. W., Polunenko, A., & Marinellie, S. A. (2009). Role of working memory in children's understanding spoken narrative: A preliminary investigation. Applied Psycholinguistics, 30, 485509.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norbury, C. F., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2003). Narrative skills of children with communication impairments. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 38, 287313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pankratz, M. E., Plante, E., Vance, R., & Insalaco, D. M. (2007). The diagnostic and predictive validity of the Renfrew bus story. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 38, 390399.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paul, R. (1995). Language disorders from infancy through adolescence: Assessment and intervention. St. Louis, MO: Mosby.Google Scholar
Paul, R., & Smith, R. L. (1993). Narrative skills in 4-year-olds with normal, impaired, and late-developing language. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 592598.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (1983). Developmental psycholinguistics: Three ways of looking at a child's narrative. New York: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peterson, C. C., & Slaughter, V. P. (2006). Telling the story of theory of mind: Deaf and hearing children's narratives and mental state understanding. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24, 151179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rapin, I., & Allen, A. (1983). Developmental language disorders: Nosological considerations. In Kirk, U. (Ed.), Neuropsychology of language reading and spelling. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Raven, J. C. (1956). Guide to using the Coloured Progressive Matrices. London: H. K. Lewis.Google Scholar
Reilly, J., Losh, M., Bellugi, U., & Wulfeck, B. (2004). “Frog, where are you?” Narratives in children with specific language impairment, early focal brain injury, and Williams syndrome. Brain and Language, 88, 229247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reuterskiold Wagner, C., Sahlen, B., & Nettelbladt, U. (1999). What's the story? Narration and comprehension in Swedish preschool children with language impairment. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 15, 113137.Google Scholar
Roth, F. P., Speece, D. L., Cooper, D. H., & De La Paz, S. (1996). Unresolved mysteries: How do metalinguistic and narrative skills connect with early reading. Journal of Special Education, 30, 257277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shields, J., Varley, R., Broks, P., & Simpson, A. (1996). Social cognition in developmental language disorders and high-level autism. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 38, 487495.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silva, P. A. (1980). The prevalence, stability and significance of developmental language delay in preschool children. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 22, 768777.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singer, B. D., & Bashir, A. S. (1999). What are executive functions and self-regulation and what do they have to do with langauge-learning disorders? Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 30, 265273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sinzig, J. M., D., Bruning, N., Schmidt, M. H., & Lehmkuhl, G. (2008). Inhibition, flexibility, working memory and planning in autism spectrum disorders with and without comorbid ADHD-symptoms. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health, 2, 4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stein, N. L., & Glenn, C. G. (1979). An analysis of story comprehension in elementary children (Vol. 2). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H., & Sullivan, K. (1995). Attributing mental states to story characters: A comparison of narratives produced by autistic and mentally retarded individuals. Applied Psycholinguistics, 16, 241256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tannock, R., Purvis, K. L., & Schachar, R. J. (1993). Narrative abilities in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and normal peers. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 21, 103117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomblin, J. B. (1997). Prevalence of specific language impairment in kindergarten children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 12451260.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Bon, W. H. J. (1986). Raven's Coloured Progressive Matrices: Manual of Dutch norms. Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.Google Scholar
Verhoeven, L., & Vermeer, A. (2001). Taaltest Alle Kinderen (TAK) (Language Test for All Children). Arnhem, The Netherlands: CITO.Google Scholar
Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, D. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72, 655684.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wetherell, D., Botting, N., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2007). Narrative skills in adolescents with a history of SLI in relation to non-verbal IQ scores. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 23, 95113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar