Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-7cvxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T04:09:52.996Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The role of cognitive control in anaphor resolution in children with specific language impairment

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 May 2017

ENIKŐ LADÁNYI*
Affiliation:
Université Paris Descartes and Budapest University of Technology and Economics
BENCE KAS
Affiliation:
Hungarian Academy of Sciences and Eötvös Loránd University
ÁGNES LUKÁCS
Affiliation:
Budapest University of Technology and Economics
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Enikő Ladányi, Laboratoire Psychologie de la Perception, Université Paris Descartes, Paris, France. E-mail: eniko.ladanyi@parisdescartes.fr

Abstract

We studied anaphor resolution and its relationship with cognitive control abilities in children with specific language impairment (SLI) and typically developing (TD) children. In a sentence–picture verification task assessing anaphor interpretation, the SLI group was less successful than age-matched TD peers, and displayed similar performance patterns as younger TD children in previous studies. The SLI group showed weaknesses in nonlinguistic cognitive control tasks, which were associated with anaphor interpretation results. These findings are in contrast with the view that proposes a grammar-specific deficit behind anaphor resolution problems in SLI. We suggest that anaphor interpretation in this population is delayed but not atypical, and this delay can be partly explained by weaker cognitive control abilities.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Archibald, L., & Gathercole, S. (2006). Short-term and working memory in specific language impairment. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 41, 675693.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memory. In Bower, G. H. (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (pp. 4789). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Badecker, W., & Straub, K. (2002). The processing role of structural constraints on the interpretation of pronouns and anaphors. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 28, 748769.Google Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M. (1983). Test for Reception of Grammar. Manchester: Medical Research Council.Google Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M., Bright, P., James, C., Bishop, S. J., & van der Lely, H. K. J. (2000). Grammatical SLI: A distinct subtype of developmental language impairment? Applied Psycholinguistics, 21, 159181.Google Scholar
Chien, Y.-C. & Wexler, K. (1990). Children's knowledge of locality conditions in binding as evidence for the modularity of syntax and pragmatics. Language Acquisition, 1, 225295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1981). Lectures on government and binding. Dordrecht: Foris.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1986). Knowledge of language: Its nature, origin and use. New York: Praeger.Google Scholar
Clackson, K., Felser, C., & Clahsen, H. (2011). Children's processing of reflexives and pronouns in English: Evidence from eye-movements during listening. Journal of Memory and Language, 65, 128144.Google Scholar
Clackson, K., & Heyer, V. (2014). Reflexive anaphor resolution in spoken language comprehension: Structural constraints and beyond. Frontiers in Psychology: Language Sciences, 5, 904.Google Scholar
Cowan, N. (1995). Attention and memory: An integrated framework. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cowan, N. (1999). An embedded-processes model of working memory. In Miyake, A. & Shah, P. (Eds.), Models of working memory (pp. 62101). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Csányi, I. (1974). Peabody Szókincs-Teszt [Peabody Receptive Vocabulary Test]. Budapest: Bárczi Gusztáv Gyógypedagógiai Főiskola.Google Scholar
Cunnings, I., & Felser, C. (2013). The role of working memory in the processing of reflexives. Language and Cognitive Processes, 28, 188219.Google Scholar
del Río, D., Maestú, F., López-Higes, R., Moratti, S., Gutiérrez, R., Maestú, C., & del-Pozo, F. (2011). Conflict and cognitive control during sentence comprehension: Recruitment of a frontal network during the processing of Spanish object-first sentences. Neuropsychologia, 49, 382391.Google Scholar
Ellis Weismer, S., Evans, J., & Hesketh, L. (1999). An examination of verbal working memory capacity in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 12491260.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellis Weismer, S., & Thordardottir, E. T. (2002). Cognition and language. In Accardo, P., Capute, A., & Rogers, B. (Eds.), Disorders of language development (pp. 2137). Timonium, MD: York Press.Google Scholar
Engle, R. W. (2002). Working memory capacity as executive attention. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 1923.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Finneran, D. A., Francis, A. L., & Leonard, L. B. (2009). Sustained attention in preschool-age children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 52, 915929.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S., & Baddeley, A. (1990). Phonological memory deficits in language disordered children: Is there a causal connection? Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 336360.Google Scholar
Gaulin, C., & Campbell, T. (1994). Procedure for assessing verbal working memory in normal school-age children: Some preliminary data. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 79, 5564.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grodzinsky, Y., & Reinhart, T. (1993). The innateness of binding and coreference. Linguistic Inquiry, 24, 69101.Google Scholar
Guasti, M. T. (2002). Language acquisition: The growth of grammar. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Henry, L. A., Messer, D., & Nash, G. (2012). Executive functioning in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53, 3745.Google Scholar
Hesketh, A., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2013). Memory and language in middle childhood for individuals with a history of specific language impairment. PLOS ONE, 8, e56314.Google Scholar
Im-Bolter, N., Johnson, J., & Pascual-Leone, J. (2006) Processing limitations in children with specific language impairment: The role of executive function. Child Development, 77, 18221841.Google Scholar
January, D., Trueswell, J. C., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2009). Co-localization of Stroop and syntactic ambiguity resolution in Broca's area: Implications for the neural basis of sentence processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 24342444.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joanisse, M. F., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1998). Specific language impairment in children: An impairment in grammar or processing? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 240246.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Just, M., & Carpenter, P. (1992). A capacity theory of comprehension: Individual differences in working memory. Psychological Review, 99, 122149.Google Scholar
Kail, R. (1994). A method for studying the generalized slowing hypothesis in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 37, 418421.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kas, B., & Lukács, Á. (2016). Magyar Mondatutánmondási Teszt [Hungarian Sentence Repetition Test]. Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
Kas, B., Lukács, Á., & Szentkuti-Kiss, K. (2016) A szórend és az esetjelölés szerepe specifikus nyelvi zavart mutató gyerekek mondatfeldolgozásában. In Kas, B. (Ed.), Szavad ne feledd! Tanulmányok Bánréti Zoltán tiszteletére. Budapest: MTA Nyelvtudományi Intézet.Google Scholar
Leonard, L. B. (2014). Children with specific language impairment. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (Original work published 1998)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leonard, L. B., Ellis Weismer, S., Miller, C. A., Francis, D. J., Tomblin, J. B., & Kail, R. V. (2007). Speed of processing, working memory, and language impairment in children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 408428.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lukács, Á., Győri, M., & Rózsa, S. (2012). A TROG pszichometriai jellemzőinek magyar vizsgálata, a normák kialakítása [The psychometric analysis of Hungarian data from the TROG]. In Bishop, D. V. M. (Ed.), TROG—Test for Reception of Grammar. Handbook (pp. 4786). Budapest: OS Hungary.Google Scholar
Lukács, Á., Kas, B., & Leonard, L. B. (2013). Case marking in Hungarian children with specific language impairment. First Language, 33, 331353.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lukács, Á., Ladányi, E., Fazekas, K., & Kemény, F. (2016). Executive functions and the contribution of short-term memory span in children with specific language impairment. Neuropsychology, 30, 296303.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lukács, A., Leonard, L. B., Kas, B., & Pléh, C. (2009). The use of tense and agreement by Hungarian-speaking children with language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52, 98117.Google Scholar
Marton, K. (2008). Visuo-spatial processing and executive functions in children with specific language impairment. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 43, 181200.Google Scholar
Marton, K., Campanelli, L., Eichorn, N., Scheuer, J., & Yoon, J. (2014). Information processing and proactive interference in children with and without specific language impairment (SLI). Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 57, 106119.Google Scholar
Marton, K., Kelmenson, L., & Pinkhasova, M. (2007). Inhibition control and working memory capacity in children with SLI. Psychologia, 50, 110121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marton, K., & Schwartz, R. (2003). Working memory capacity and language processes in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46, 11381153.Google Scholar
Miller, C. A., Kail, R., Leonard, L. B., & Tomblin, B. (2001). Speed of processing in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 416433.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24, 167202.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miyake, A., Carpenter, P. A., & Just, M. A. (1994). A capacity approach to syntactic comprehension disorders: Making normal adults perform like aphasic patients. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 11, 671717.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montgomery, J. (1995). Sentence comprehension in children with specific language impairment: The role of phonological working memory. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 187199.Google Scholar
Montgomery, J. (2000). Verbal working memory and sentence comprehension in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43, 293308.Google Scholar
Montgomery, J. (2004). Sentence comprehension in children with specific language impairment: Effects of input rate and phonological working memory. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 39, 115134.Google Scholar
Montgomery, J. W., & Evans, J. L. (2009). Complex sentence comprehension and working memory in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 52, 269288.Google Scholar
Montgomery, J. W., Magimairaj, B. M., & Finney, M. C. (2010). Working memory and specific language impairment: An update on the relation and perspectives on assessment and treatment. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 19, 7894.Google Scholar
Nicol, J. (1988). Coreference processing during sentence comprehension (PhD thesis, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences).Google Scholar
Nicol, J., & Swinney, D. (1989). The role of structure in coreference assignment during sentence comprehension. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 18, 519.Google Scholar
Norbury, C., Bishop, D. V. M., & Briscoe, J. (2002). Does impaired grammatical comprehension provide evidence for an innate grammar module? Applied Psycholinguistics, 23, 247268.Google Scholar
Noterdaeme, M., Amorosa, H., Mildenberger, K., Sitter, S., & Minow, F. (2001). Evaluation of attention in children with autism and children with a specific language disorder. European Journal of Child Psychology, 10, 5866.Google Scholar
Novick, J. M., Trueswell, J. C., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2005). Cognitive control and parsing: Reexamining the role of Broca's area in sentence comprehension. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 5, 263281.Google Scholar
Novick, J. M., Trueswell, J. C., & Thomspon-Schill, S. L. (2010). Broca's area and language processing: Evidence for the cognitive control connection. Language and Linguistics Compass, 4, 906924.Google Scholar
Novogrodsky, R., & Friedmann, N. (2010). Not all dependencies are impaired in syntactic-SLI: Binding in children with a deficit in Wh-movement. In Franich, K., Keil, L., Iserman, K., & Chandlee, J. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 34th Boston University Conference on Language Development (BUCLD). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Perovic, A., Modyanova, N., & Wexler, K. (2013). Comparison of grammar in neurodevelopmental disorders: The case of binding in Williams syndrome and autism with and without language impairment. Language Acquisition, 20, 133154.Google Scholar
Racsmány, M., Lukács, Á., Németh, D., & Pléh, C. (2005). A verbális munkamemória magyar nyelvű vizsgálóeljárásai [Hungarian methods for studying verbal working memory]. Magyar Pszichológiai Szemle, 60, 479505.Google Scholar
Rákosi, G., & Tóth, E. (2016). The pronoun interpretation problem in child Hungarian. Acta Linguistica Hungarica, 63, 6396.Google Scholar
Raven, J., Court, J. H., & Raven, J. C. (1987). Raven's Progressive Matrices. London: H. K. Lewis.Google Scholar
Rice, M. L., Wexler, K., & Redmond, S. (1999) Grammaticality judgments of an extended optional infinitive grammar: Evidence from English-speaking children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 943961.Google Scholar
Runner, J. T., Sussman, R. S., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2003). Assignment of reference to reflexives and pronouns in picture noun phrases: Evidence from eye movements. Cognition, 89, B1–B13.Google Scholar
Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2012). E-Prime user's guide. Pittsburgh, PA: Psychology Software Tools.Google Scholar
Spaulding, T. J. (2010). Investigating mechanisms of suppression in preschool children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53, 725738.Google Scholar
Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643662.Google Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H., & Cooper, J. (1999). Present and future possibilities for defining a phenotype for specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 42, 12751278.Google Scholar
Tallal, P., & Piercy, M. (1973). Defects of non-verbal auditory perception in children with developmental aphasia. Nature, 241, 468469.Google Scholar
van der Lely, H. K. (1996). Specifically language impaired and normally developing children: Verbal passive vs adjectival passive sentence interpretation. Lingua, 98, 243272.Google Scholar
van der Lely, H. K. (2005). Domain-specific cognitive systems: Insight from grammatical specific language impairment. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 5359.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van der Lely, H. K. (1998). SLI in children: Movement, economy and deficits in the computational syntactic system. Language Acquisition, 72, 161192.Google Scholar
van der Lely, H. K., & Harris, M. (1990). Comprehension of reversible sentences in specifically language-impaired children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 55, 101117.Google Scholar
van der Lely, H. K., & Stollwerck, L. (1997). Binding theory and grammatical specific language impairment in children. Cognition, 62, 245290.Google Scholar
Waters, G., & Caplan, D. (1996). Processing resource capacity and the comprehension of garden path sentences. Memory and Cognition, 24, 2455.Google Scholar
Windsor, J., & Hwang, M. (1999). Testing the generalized slowing hypothesis in specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 12051218.Google Scholar
Xiang, M., Dillon, B., & Phillips, C. (2009). Illusory licensing effects across dependency types: ERP evidence. Brain and Language, 108, 4055.Google Scholar
Ye, Z., & Zhou, X. (2009). Executive control in language processing. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 33, 11681177.Google Scholar