Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-fscjk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T17:07:28.821Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Procedural learning across modalities in French-speaking children with specific language impairment

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 January 2014

AUDREY GABRIEL
Affiliation:
University of Liège
THIERRY MEULEMANS
Affiliation:
University of Liège
CHRISTOPHE PARISSE
Affiliation:
University of Paris Ouest Nanterre
CHRISTELLE MAILLART*
Affiliation:
University of Liège
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Christelle Maillart, Department of Psychology: Cognition and Behavior, University of Liège, B38, Rue de l'Aunaie 30, Liège 4000, Belgium. E-mail: christelle.maillart@ulg.ac.be

Abstract

It has been suggested that the language problems encountered in specific language impairment (SLI) arise from basal ganglia abnormalities that lead to impaired procedural memory. However, recent serial reaction time (SRT) studies did not reveal any differences between the SLI and typically developing (TD) groups on the measures of procedural memory linked to visual sequence learning. In this paper, 16 children with and without SLI were compared on two versions of SRT tasks: a visual task and an auditory one. The results showed that children with SLI were as fast as their TD peers in both modalities. All of the children obtained similar specific sequence learning indices, indicating that they were able to detect regularities in both modalities. Although children with SLI were as accurate as their TD peers for the visual SRT task, they made more errors than their TD peers in auditory SRT conditions. The results indicate that, in relation to procedural memory, the core of the impairment in SLI is not linked to difficulties in the detection of regularities. We argue that when children with SLI present some difficulties, the children's weaknesses might depend on the type of processing involved (e.g., tasks involving auditory sequences).

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Adi-Japha, E., Strulovich-Schwartz, O., & Julius, M. (2011). Delayed motor skill acquisition in children with language impairment. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32, 29632971.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Archibald, L. M., & Gathercole, S. E. (2007). The complexities of complex memory span: Storage and processing deficits in specific language impairment. Journal of Memory and Language, 57, 177194.Google Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M. (1989). Test for Reception of Grammar. Manchester: University of Manchester, Age and Cognitive Performance Research Centre.Google Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M. (2002). Motor immaturity and specific speech and language impairment: Evidence for a common genetic basis. American Journal of Medical Genetics (Neuropsychiatric Genetics), 114, 5663.Google Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M. (2006). Developmental cognitive genetics: How psychology can inform genetics and vice versa. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 11531168.Google Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M., Adams, C. V., Nation, K. & Rosen, S. (2005). Perception of transient nonspeech stimuli is normal in specific language impairment: Evidence from glide discrimination. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26, 175194.Google Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M., & Norbury, C. F. (2005). Executive functions in children with communication impairments, in relation to autistic symptomatology: 2. Response inhibition. Autism, 9, 2943.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Botting, N. (2005). Non-verbal cognitive development and language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 317326.Google Scholar
Buchner, A., Steffens, M., Erdfelder, E. & Rothkegel, R. (1997). A multinominal model to assess fluency and recollection in a sequence learning task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50A, 631663.Google Scholar
Buchner, A., Steffens, M., Irmen, L., & Wender, K. (1998). Irrelevant auditory material affects counting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 4867.Google ScholarPubMed
Campbell, W. N., & Skarakis-Doyle, E. (2007). School-aged children with SLI: The ICF as a framework for collaborative service delivery. Journal of Communication Disorders, 40, 513535.Google Scholar
Cohen, A., Ivry, R. I., & Keele, S. W. (1990). Attention and structure in sequence learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 1730.Google Scholar
Cohen, N. J., & Squire, L. R. (1980). Preserved learning and retention of pattern analysing skill in amnesia: Dissociation of knowing how and knowing that. Science, 210, 207210.Google Scholar
Conway, C. M., & Christiansen, M. H. (2006). Statistical learning within and between modalities: Pitting abstract against stimulus specific representations. Psychological Science, 17, 905912.Google Scholar
Dominey, P. F., Hoen, M., Blanc, J. M., & Lelekov-Boissard, T. (2003). Neurological basis of language and sequential cognition: Evidence from simulation, aphasia, and ERP studies. Brain and Language, 86, 207225.Google Scholar
Doyon, J., Korman, M., Morin, A., Dostie, V., Hadj Tahar, A., Benali, H., et al. (2009). Contribution of night and day sleep vs. simple passage of time to the consolidation of motor sequence and visuomotor adaptation learning. Experimental Brain Research, 195, 1526.Google Scholar
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1981). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, revised: Manual for forms L and M. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Dunn, L. M., Thériault-Whalen, C. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1993). Echelle de vocabulaire en images Peabody: Adaptation française du Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test. Toronto: Psycan.Google Scholar
Eichenbaum, H. (2000). A cortical–hippocampal system for declarative memory. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 1, 4150.Google Scholar
Ellis Weismer, S., & Hesketh, L. J. (1996). Lexical learning by children with specific language impairment: Effects of linguistic input presented at varying speaking rates. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39, 177190.Google Scholar
Ellis Weismer, S., Plante, E., Jones, M., & Tomblin, J. B. (2005). A functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation of verbal working memory in adolescents with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 405425.Google Scholar
Evans, J., Saffran, J. R., & Robe-Torres, K. (2009). Statistical learning in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52, 321335.Google Scholar
Fernell, E., Norrelgen, F., Bozkurt, I., Hellberg, G., & Löwing, K. (2002). Developmental profiles and auditory perception in 25 children attending special preschools for language-impaired children. Acta Paediatrica, 91, 11081115.Google Scholar
Frensch, P., Lin, J., & Buchner, A. (1998). Learning versus behavioral expression of the learned: The effects of a secondary tone-counting task on implicit learning in the serial reaction task. Psychology Research, 61, 8398.Google Scholar
Fromm, W., Schöler, H., & Scherer, C. (1998). Jedes vierte Kind sprachgestört? Definition, Verbreitung, Erscheinungsbild, Entwicklungsbedingungen und–voraussetzungen der Spezifischen Sprachentwicklungsstörung. In Schöler, H., Fromm, W., & Kany, W. (Eds.), Spezifische Sprachentwicklungsstörung und Sprachlernen (pp. 2263). Heidelberg: Winter.Google Scholar
Gabriel, A., Maillart, C., Guillaume, M., Stefaniak, N., & Meulemans, T. (2011). Exploration of serial structure procedural learning in children with language impairment. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17, 18.Google Scholar
Gabriel, A., Stefaniak, N., Maillart, C., Schmitz, X., & Meulemans, T. (in press). Procedural visual learning abilities in children with specific language impairment. American Journal of Speech Language Pathology. doi:10.1044/1058–0360Google Scholar
Gabrieli, J. D. E (1998). Cognitive neuroscience of human memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 87115.Google Scholar
Hedenius, M., Persson, J., Tremblay, A., Adi-Japha, E., Verissimo, J., Dye, C. D., et al. (2011). Grammar predicts procedural learning and consolidation deficits in children with specific language impairment. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32, 23622375.Google Scholar
Hill, E. L. (2001). Non-specific nature of specific language impairment: A review of the literature with regard to concomitant motor impairments. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 36, 149171.Google Scholar
Hill, E. L., Bishop, D. V. M., & Nimmo-Smith, I. (1998). Representational gestures in developmental coordination disorder and specific language impairment: Error-types and the reliability of ratings. Human Movement Science, 17, 655678.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoffman, L. M., & Gillam, R. B. (2004). Verbal and spatial information processing constraints in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 114125.Google Scholar
Howard, D. V., Howard, J. H. Jr., Japikse, K. C., DiYani, C., Thompson, A., & Somberg, R. (2004). Implicit sequence learning: Effects of level of structure, adult age, and extended practice. Psychology and Aging, 19, 7992.Google Scholar
Howard, J. H. Jr., & Howard, D. V. (1997). Age differences in implicit learning of higher order dependencies in serial patterns. Psychology and Aging, 12, 634656.Google Scholar
Hsu, H-J., & Bishop, D. V. M (2010). Grammatical difficulties in children with specific language impairment: Is learning deficient? Human Development, 53, 264277.Google Scholar
Hsu, H-J., Christiansen, M. H., Tomblin, J. B., Zhang, X., & Gomez, R. L. (2006). Statistical learning of nonadjacent dependencies in adolescents with and without language impairment. Poster session presented at the 2006 Symposium on Research in Child Language Disorders, Madison, WI.Google Scholar
Im-Bolter, N., Johnson, J., & Pascual-Leone, J. (2006). Processing limitations in children with specific language impairment: The role of executive function. Child Development, 77, 18221841.Google Scholar
Institut national de la statistique et des études économiques. (2003). Insee-Réunion. Paris: INSEE.Google Scholar
Jancke, L., Siegenthaler, S., Preis, S., & Steinmetz, H. (2006). Decreased white-matter density in a left-sided frontotemporal network in children with developmental language disorder: Evidence for anatomical anomalies in a motor language network. Brain and Language, 102, 9198.Google Scholar
Joanisse, M. F., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1998). Specific language impairment in children: An impairment in grammar or processing? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2, 240246.Google Scholar
Joanisse, M. F., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2003). Phonology and syntax in specific language impairment: Evidence from a connectionist model. Brain and Language, 86, 4056.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kemény, F., & Lukács, A. (2010). Impaired procedural learning in language impairment: Results from probabilistic categorization. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 32, 249258.Google Scholar
Khomsi, A. (2001). Evaluation du Langage Oral. Paris: ECPAGoogle Scholar
Knowlton, B. J., Mangels, J. A., & Squire, I. R. (1996). A neostriatal habit learning system in humans. Sciences, 273, 13991402.Google Scholar
Lecocq, P. (1998). Epreuve de compréhension syntaxico-sémantique: Adaptation française du TROG: Reception of Grammar Test. Villeneuve d'Ascq, France: Presses Universitaires du Septentrion.Google Scholar
Leonard, L. B. (1998). Children with specific language impairment. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Leonard, L. B., Bortolini, U., Caselli, M. C., McGregor, K. K., & Sabbadini, L. (1992). Morphological deficits in children with specific language impairment: The status of features in the underlying grammar. Language Acquisition, 2, 151179.Google Scholar
Leonard, L. B., Ellis Weismer, S., Miller, C. A., Francis, D. J., Tomblin, J. B., & Kail, R. V. (2007). Speed of processing, working memory, and language impairment in children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 408428.Google Scholar
Lum, J. & Bleses, D. (2012). Declarative and procedural memory in Danish speaking children with specific language impairment. Journal of Communication Disorders, 45, 4658.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lum, Conti-Ramsden, Page, , & Ullman, (2012). Working, declarative and procedural memory in specific language impairment. Cortex, 48, 11381154.Google Scholar
Lum, J., Gelgec, C., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2010). Procedural and declarative memory in children with and without specific language impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 45, 96107.Google Scholar
Marton, K., Kelmenson, L., & Pinkhasova, M. (2007). Inhibition control and working memory capacity in children with SLI. Psychologia, 50, 110121.Google Scholar
McArthur, G. M., & Bishop, D. (2004). Which people with specific language impairment have auditory processing deficits? Cognitive Neuropsychology, 21, 7994.Google Scholar
Mengler, E. D., Hogben, J. H., Mitchie, P. T., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2005). Poor frequency discrimination is related to oral language disorder in children: A psychoacoustic study. Dyslexia, 11, 155173.Google Scholar
Meulemans, T., Van der Linden, M., & Perruchet, P. (1998). Implicit sequence learning in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 69, 199221.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, C. A., Kail, R., Leonard, L. B., & Tomblin, J. B. (2001). Speed of processing in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 416433.Google Scholar
Nissen, M. J., & Bullemer, P. (1987). Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 132.Google Scholar
Noterdaeme, M., Amorosa, H., Mildenberger, K., Sitter, S., & Minow, F. (2001). Evaluation of attention problems in children with autism and children with a specific language disorder. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 10, 5866.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ors, M. (2002). Commentary: Time to drop “specific” in “specific language impairment.” Acta Paediatrica, 91, 10251030.Google Scholar
Plante, E., Gomez, R., & Gerken, L. (2002). Sensitivity to word order cues by normal and language/learning disabled adults. Journal of Communication Disorders, 35, 453462.Google Scholar
Powell, R. P., & Bishop, D. V. M. (1992). Clumsiness and perceptual problems in children with specific language impairment. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 34, 755765.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saffran, J., Johnson, E., Aslin, R., & Newport, E. (1999). Statistical learning of tone sequences by human infants and adults. Cognition, 70, 2752.Google Scholar
Schvaneveldt, R. W., & Gomez, R. L. (1998). Attention and probabilistic sequence learning. Psychological Research/Psychologische Forschung, 61, 175190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, R. G. (2009). Specific language impairment. In Schwartz, R. G. (Ed.), Handbook of child language disorders (pp. 343). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Spaulding, T. J., Plante, E., & Vance, R. (2008). Sustained selective attention skills of preschool children with specific language impairment: Evidence for separate attentional capacities. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 51, 1634.Google Scholar
Squire, L. R., Knowlton, B., & Musen, G. (1993). The structure and organization of memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 44, 453495.Google Scholar
Tallal, P. (2000). Experimental studies of language learning impairments: From research to remediation. In Bishop, V. D. M. & Leonard, L. B. (Eds.), Speech and language impairments in children: Causes, characteristics, intervention and outcome (pp. 131155). Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Tallal, P., Merzenich, M., Miller, S. & Jenking, W. (1998). Language learning impairments: Integrating basic science, technology and remediation, Experimental Brain Research, 123, 210219.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tallal, P. & Piercy, M. (1974). Developmental aphasia: Rate of auditory processing and selective impairment of consonant perception. Neuropsychologia, 12, 8393.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tallal, P., & Stark, R. E. (1981). Speech acoustic-cue discrimination abilities of normally developing and language-impaired children. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 69, 568574.Google Scholar
Tallal, P., Stark, R., & Mellits, E. D. (1985). Identification of language-impaired children on the basis of rapid perception and production skills. Brain & Language, 25, 314322.Google Scholar
Thomas, K., & Nelson, C. (2001). Serial response time learning in preschool- and school-age children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 79, 364387.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomblin, B., Mainela-Arnold, E., & Zhang, X. (2007). Procedural learning in children with and without specific language impairment. Journal of Child Language Learning and Development, 3, 269293.Google Scholar
Ullman, M. T., & Gopnik, M. (1999). Inflectional morphology in a family with inherited specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 20, 51117.Google Scholar
Ullman, M. T., & Pierpont, E. (2005). Specific language impairment is not specific to language: The procedural deficit hypothesis. Cortex, 41, 399433.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (2005). Echelle d'intelligence de Wechsler pour enfants et adolescents—Quatrième édition—WISC IV. Paris: ECPA.Google Scholar
Willingham, D. B. (1998). A neuropsychological theory of motor skill learning. Psycholinguistic Review, 105, 558584.Google Scholar