Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-gb8f7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-29T11:01:10.619Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Episodic memory solves both social and nonsocial problems, and evolved to fulfill many different functions

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 January 2018

Raymond A. Mar
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, York University, Toronto, ON, M3J 1P3Canada. mar@yorku.cayorku.ca/mar
R. Nathan Spreng
Affiliation:
Montreal Neurological Institute, Department of Neurology and Neurosurgery, McGill University, Montreal, QC, H3A 2B4, Canada. nathan.spreng@gmail.com Department of Human Development, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY 14853. http://lbc.human.cornell.edu

Abstract

The episodic memory system is flexible and complex, and likely evolved in response to a wide range of survival-relevant problems in our evolutionary past, both social and nonsocial. Episodic memory allows us to recollect and infer details that may have seemed trivial on encoding, but are now known to be relevant. This memory aids humans in navigating their uncertain environment.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2018 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Smallwood, J. & Spreng, R. N. (2014) The default network and self-generated thought: Component processes, dynamic control, and clinical relevance. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1316:2952.Google Scholar
Beadle, J. N., Tranel, D., Cohen, N. J. & Duff, M. C. (2013) Empathy in hippocampal amnesia. Frontiers in Psychology 4:69.Google Scholar
Davidson, P. S., Drouin, H., Kwan, D., Moscovitch, M. & Rosenbaum, R. S. (2012) Memory as social glue: Close interpersonal relationships in amnesic patients. Frontiers in Psychology 3:531.Google Scholar
Kumaran, D., Banino, A., Blundell, C., Hassabis, D. & Dayan, P. (2016) Computations underlying social hierarchy learning: Distinct mechanisms for updating and representing self-relevant information. Neuron 76:653–66.Google Scholar
Raichle, M (2010) Two views of brain function. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 14:180–90.Google Scholar
Spreng, R. N. & Andrews-Hanna, J. R. (2015) The default network and social cognition. In: Brain mapping: An encyclopedic reference, ed. Toga, A. W., pp. 165–69. Academic Press/Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spreng, R. N., Mar, R. A. & Kim, A. S. N. (2009) The common neural basis of autobiographical memory, prospection, navigation, theory of mind and the default mode: A quantitative meta-analysis. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 21(3):489510. Available at: http://www.mitpressjournals.org/doi/abs/10.1162/jocn.2008.21029.Google Scholar
Tavares, R., William, C., Grossman, Y., Mendelsohn, A., Shapiro, M., Trope, Y. & Schiller, D. (2015) A map for social navigation in the human brain. Neuron 87:231–43.Google Scholar
Yates, F. A. (1966) The art of memory. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar