Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-8bhkd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-17T16:25:20.176Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Towards the elucidation of evolution of out-group aggression

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 August 2019

Nobuhiro Mifune
Affiliation:
School of Economics & Management, Kochi University of Technology, Eikokuji, Kochi City, Kochi 780-8515, Japann.mifune@gmail.com
Dora Simunovic
Affiliation:
Field C, Bremen International Graduate School of Social Sciences, 28759 Bremen, Germany. doras@bigsss-bremen.de

Abstract

We focus on the implications of De Dreu and Gross's findings for the evolutionary perspective on out-group aggression and in-group cooperation. Although their experimental protocols are potentially useful in determining the origins of out-group aggression in humans, they so far provide inconclusive evidence only. We suggest ways of furthering our understanding of the connection between parochial cooperation and intergroup conflict.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Balliet, D. P., Wu, J. & De Dreu, C. K. W. (2014) In-group favoritism and cooperation: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin 140(6):1556–81.Google Scholar
Bernhard, H., Fischbacher, U. & Fehr, E. (2006) Parochial altruism in humans. Nature 442(7105):912–15. Available at: https://www.nature.com/articles/nature04981.Google Scholar
Choi, J. K. & Bowles, S. (2007) The coevolution of parochial altruism and war. Science 318(5850):636–40. Available at: http://science.sciencemag.org/content/318/5850/636.full.Google Scholar
Halevy, N., Bornstein, G. & Sagiv, L. (2008) “In-group love” and “out-group hate” as motives for individual participation in intergroup conflict: A new game paradigm. Psychological Science 19(4):405–11.Google Scholar
Herrmann, B., Thöni, C. & Gächter, S. (2008) Antisocial punishment across societies. Science 319(5868):1362–67.Google Scholar
McLoughlin, N. & Over, H. (2018) The developmental origins of dehumanization. Advances in Child Development and Behavior 54:153–78. doi: 10.1016/bs.acdb.2017.10.006.Google Scholar
Mifune, N., Hizen, Y., Kamijo, Y. & Okano, Y. (2016) Preemptive striking in individual and group conflict. PLoS One 11(5):e0154859.Google Scholar
Mifune, N., Simunovic, D. & Yamagishi, T. (2017) Intergroup biases in fear-induced aggression. Frontiers in Psychology 8:49.Google Scholar
Pruitt, D. G. & Kimmel, M. J. (1977) Twenty years of experimental gaming: Critique, synthesis, and suggestions for the future. Annual Review of Psychology 28(1):363–92.Google Scholar
Simunovic, D., Mifune, N. & Yamagishi, T. (2013) Preemptive strike: An experimental study of fear-based aggression. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 49(6):1120–3.Google Scholar
Yamagishi, T. & Mifune, N. (2008) Does shared group membership promote altruism? Fear, greed, and reputation. Rationality and Society 20(1):530.Google Scholar
Yamagishi, T. & Mifune, N. (2016) Parochial altruism: Does it explain modern human group psychology? Current Opinion in Psychology 7:3943.Google Scholar
Yamagishi, T., Mifune, N., Li, Y., Shinada, M., Hashimoto, H., Horita, Y., Miura, A., Inukai, K., Tanida, S., Kiyonari, T., Takagishi, H. & Simunovic, D. (2013) Is behavioral pro-sociality game-specific? Pro-social preference and expectations of pro-sociality. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 120(2):260–71.Google Scholar