Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-v9fdk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T11:54:03.075Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Finger counting: The missing tool?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 December 2008

Michael Andres
Affiliation:
Cognitive Neuroscience Unit and Laboratory of Neurophysiology, Catholic University of Louvain, 1348 Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgiummichael.andres@uclouvain.besamuel.diluca@uclouvain.bemauro.pesenti@uclouvain.be
Samuel Di Luca
Affiliation:
Cognitive Neuroscience Unit and Laboratory of Neurophysiology, Catholic University of Louvain, 1348 Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgiummichael.andres@uclouvain.besamuel.diluca@uclouvain.bemauro.pesenti@uclouvain.be
Mauro Pesenti
Affiliation:
Cognitive Neuroscience Unit and Laboratory of Neurophysiology, Catholic University of Louvain, 1348 Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgiummichael.andres@uclouvain.besamuel.diluca@uclouvain.bemauro.pesenti@uclouvain.be

Abstract

Rips et al. claim that the principles underlying the structure of natural numbers cannot be inferred from interactions with the physical world. However, in their target article they failed to consider an important source of interaction: finger counting. Here, we show that finger counting satisfies all the conditions required for allowing the concept of numbers to emerge from sensorimotor experience through a bottom-up process.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Andres, M., Olivier, E. & Badets, A. (in press) Actions, words and numbers: A motor contribution to semantic processing? Current Directions in Psychological Science.Google Scholar
Andres, M., Seron, X. & Olivier, E. (2007) Contribution of hand motor circuits to counting. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 19(4):563–76.Google Scholar
Di Luca, S. (2008) Interactions between fingers and numbers: Towards finger numeral representations. Retrieved May 9, 2008, from BICTEL/e Electronic Dissertations. (BelnUcetd-05092008-160734).Google Scholar
Di Luca, S., Grana, A., Semenza, C.Seron, X. & Pesenti, M. (2006) Finger-digit compatibility in Arabic numeral processing. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 59(9):1648–63.Google Scholar
Di Luca, S. & Pesenti, M. (2008) Masked priming effect with canonical finger numeral configurations. Experimental Brain Research 185(1):2739.Google Scholar
Di Luca, S. & Pesenti, M. (under revision) Place coding for canonical finger numeral representations.Google Scholar
Dohmas, F., Krinzinger, H. & Willmes, K. (2008) Mind the gap between both hands: Evidence for internal finger-based number representations in children's mental calculation. Cortex 44:359–67.Google Scholar
Fayol, M., Barrouillet, P. & Marinthe, C. (1998) Predicting arithmetical achievement from neuropsychological performance: A longitudinal study. Cognition 68:6370.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gerstmann, J. (1940) Syndrome of finger agnosia, disorientation for right and left, agraphia and dyscalculia. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry 44:398408.Google Scholar
Gracia-Bafalluy, M. & Noël, M.-P. (2008) Does finger training increased young children's numerical performance? Cortex 44:368–75.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ifrah, G. (1981) Histoire universelle des chiffres. Robert Laffont.Google Scholar
Menninger, K. (1898/1969) Number words and number symbols. MIT Press. (Original work published 1898).Google Scholar
Noël, M-P. (2005) Finger agnosia: A predictor of numerical abilities in children? Child Neuropsychology 11(5):413–30.Google Scholar
Pesenti, M., Thioux, M., Seron, X. & De Volder, A. (2000) Neuroanatomical substrate of Arabic number processing, numerical comparison and simple addition: A PET study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 121(3):461–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pica, P., Lemer, C., Izard, V. & Dehaene, S. (2004) Exact and approximate arithmetic in an Amazonian indigene group. Science 306:499503.Google Scholar
Roggeman, C., Verguts, T. & Fias, W. (2007). Priming reveals differential coding of symbolic and non-symbolic quantities. Cognition 105:380–94.Google Scholar
Rusconi, E., Walsh, V. & Butterworth, B. (2005) Dexterity with numbers: rTMS over left angular gyrus disrupts finger gnosis and number processing. Neuropsychologia 43(11):1609–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sato, M., Cattaneo, L., Rizzolatti, G. & Gallese, V. (2007) Numbers within our hands: Modulation of corticospinal excitability of hand muscles during numerical judgement. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 19(4):684–93.Google Scholar