Hostname: page-component-7bb8b95d7b-qxsvm Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-09-27T01:38:02.269Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The Impact of Cognitive Restructuring on Post-Event Rumination and Its Situational Effect on Socially Anxious Adolescents

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 August 2023

Meng Yu
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, School of Public Health, Southern Medical University, Guangzhou 510515, P.R. China Department of Psychiatry, Zhujiang Hospital, Southern Medical University, Guangzhou 510282, P.R. China
Yawen Zhu
Affiliation:
Key Laboratory of Behavioral and Mental Health of Gansu Province, School of Psychology, Northwest Normal University, Lanzhou 730070, P.R. China
Dingguo Gao
Affiliation:
Guangdong Provincial Key Laboratory of Social Cognitive Neuroscience and Mental Health, Department of Psychology, Sun Yat-Sen University, Guangzhou 510006, P.R. China
Qian Xu
Affiliation:
Mental Health Education Center, Southwestern University of Finance and Economics, Chengdu 611130, P.R. China
Ye Wang
Affiliation:
Changzhou ART Vocational College of Jiangsu Province, Changzhou 213147, P.R. China
Jianping Wang*
Affiliation:
Beijing Key Laboratory of Applied Experimental Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, Beijing Normal University, Beijing 100875, P.R. China
*
Corresponding author: Jianping Wang; Email: wjphh@bnu.edu.cn

Abstract

Post-event rumination (PER) has been seen as a key element in the persistence of social anxiety (disorder). Studies on PER-targeted intervention, e.g., cognitive restructuring (CR), has, however, received little attention in adults, not yet in youth. In addition, previous research showed that, compared to interaction, participants reported higher levels of PER after speech task. The main aim of the present study was to investigate the effect of CR targeting PER among socially anxious (Chinese) adolescents and also to compare the intervention effect between speech and interaction situations. The present study recruited a sample of 73 high socially anxious adolescents aged 12–16 years and then randomly assigned them into speech (n = 37) or interaction (n = 36) group, without control group. PER and social anxiety (SA) were measured before and after CR. Analysis of Covariance (ANCOVA) results showed that adolescents’ PER and SA symptoms were significantly improved with intervention with moderate to high effect size. Furthermore, the decrease in PER could significantly predict the improvement of SA. However, the intervention effect showed no difference between groups. Although no control group was included, one-session CR still showed its potential to improve participants’ PER and SA. Limitations and future directions were discussed.

Type
Standard Paper
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of the Australian Association for Cognitive and Behaviour Therapy

Introduction

Social anxiety disorder (SAD) is characterised by being fearful of negative evaluations or judgements from others in social situations (Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM)-5; American Psychiatric Association, 2013; Hirsch, Meeten, Krahé, & Reeder, Reference Hirsch, Meeten, Krahé and Reeder2016; Westenberg, Gullone, Bokhorst, Heyne, & King, Reference Westenberg, Gullone, Bokhorst, Heyne and King2007). It is one of the most epidemic psychological disorders in children and adolescents (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Mcgonagle, Zhao, Nelson, Hughes, Eshleman and Kendler1994) with a prevalence rate of about 10% (e.g., Beesdo et al., Reference Beesdo, Bittner, Pine, Stein, Hofler, Lieb and Wittchen2007; Wittchen, Stein, & Kessler, Reference Wittchen, Stein and Kessler1999). The existence of SAD in the period of adolescence would raise the risk of comorbid other mental disorders (e.g., Costello, Mustillo, Erkanli, Keeler, & Angold, Reference Costello, Mustillo, Erkanli, Keeler and Angold2003). Moreover, SAD during adolescence is relevant to impairments in academic and social functioning, such as, lower peer acceptance (e.g., Blöte & Westenberg, Reference Blöte and Westenberg2007; Greco & Morris, Reference Greco and Morris2005), negative interaction with peers (e.g., Blöte, Miers, Heyne, & Westenberg, Reference Blöte, Miers, Heyne, Westenberg, Ranta, La Greca, Garcia-Lopez and Marttunen2015), and victimisation by peers (e.g., Ranta, Kaltiala-Heino, Fröjd, & Marttunen, Reference Ranta, Kaltiala-Heino, Fröjd and Marttunen2013).

Clark and Wells (Reference Clark, Wells, Heimberg, Liebowitz, Hope and Schneier1995) proposed that the post-event rumination (PER) following an anxiety-provoking social situation is a key factor in the maintenance of SAD, including in youth sample (Hodson, McManus, Clark, & Doll, Reference Hodson, McManus, Clark and Doll2008). PER is a repetitive cognitive process involving intrusive images, negative thoughts and self-perception related to social situations, and similar past social failures (Abbott & Rapee, Reference Abbott and Rapee2004; Clark & Wells, Reference Clark, Wells, Heimberg, Liebowitz, Hope and Schneier1995; Hofmann, Reference Hofmann2007). Previous research has consistently indicated that socially anxious individuals registered high levels of PER after experiencing social occasions (e.g., Kiko et al., Reference Kiko, Stevens, Mall, Steil, Bohus and Hermann2012; Makkar & Grisham, Reference Makkar and Grisham2011); however, relatively few empirical studies have examined the role of PER in the maintenance of social anxiety (disorder) in youth samples (e.g., Blöte, Miers, Van den Bos, & Westenberg, Reference Blöte, Miers, Van den Bos and Westenberg2019; Hodson et al., Reference Hodson, McManus, Clark and Doll2008).

One cross-sectional study by Hodson et al. (Reference Hodson, McManus, Clark and Doll2008) demonstrated that socially anxious adolescents aged 11–14 years old showed greater PER than participants with low levels of social fear. The result of further regression analysis showed that after controlling for depression, PER could significantly predict social anxiety (SA) (Hodson et al., Reference Hodson, McManus, Clark and Doll2008). Although no longitudinal design was adopted in Hodson et al. (Reference Hodson, McManus, Clark and Doll2008)'s study, the authors still claimed that the cross-sectional research design was able to infer the applicability of Clark and Well's (Reference Clark, Wells, Heimberg, Liebowitz, Hope and Schneier1995) model to young people, which the conclusion that PER could be significantly predictive of adolescents’ SA was convincible as the model primarily concerns the maintenance, not the aetiology, of social anxiety (disorder) (Hodson et al., Reference Hodson, McManus, Clark and Doll2008). In another study, Blöte et al. (Reference Blöte, Miers, Van den Bos and Westenberg2019) used an experimental social event — a 5-min Leiden Public Speaking Task (Westenberg et al., Reference Westenberg, Bokhorst, Miers, Sumter, Kallen, van Pelt and Blöte2009) — with a community sample of 229 adolescents, aged 11–18 years, whose PER was measured with Thoughts Questionnaire (Edwards, Rapee, & Franklin, Reference Edwards, Rapee and Franklin2003) 1 week after the social task. Results demonstrated that after partially out of depression, adolescents’ SA measured at baseline significantly predicted PER after the speech task (Blöte et al., Reference Blöte, Miers, Van den Bos and Westenberg2019). Thus, for socially anxious adolescents, a vicious and an interactive circle exists; in other words, the original SA raises their PER level after experiencing social occasions; then the induced PER further increases their fears of social events.

Cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) has been empirically proven efficacious in alleviating SA in the samples of youth (e.g., Albano, Reference Albano1995; Hayward et al., Reference Hayward, Varady, Albano, Thienemann, Henderson and Schatzberg2000) and also beneficial in reducing cognitive bias, such as PER (e.g., Kocovski & Rector, Reference Kocovski and Rector2008; Shikatani, Antony, Kuo, & Cassin, Reference Shikatani, Antony, Kuo and Cassin2014) but only in adult populations. McEvoy, Mahoney, Perini, and Kingsep (Reference McEvoy, Mahoney, Perini and Kingsep2009) recruited 61 participants diagnosed with SAD to conduct group CBT. Results showed that after the 7-week group intervention participants’ PER and SA levels were significantly improved with a moderate effect size (McEvoy et al., Reference McEvoy, Mahoney, Perini and Kingsep2009). Further analyses suggested a significant correlation between the reduction of both PER and SA (McEvoy et al., Reference McEvoy, Mahoney, Perini and Kingsep2009). Shikatani et al. (Reference Shikatani, Antony, Kuo and Cassin2014) recruited 56 participants with high levels of SA. In this study, after delivering a 3-min impromptu speech, participants were randomly distributed to one of two intervention groups: cognitive restructuring (CR) or mindfulness; a control group was also formed. Participants were provided with psychoeducation and were taught by the experimenter how to identify and challenge their negative and unrealistic thoughts about the speech performance. Findings showed that both the CR and the mindfulness groups scored lower on PER than the control group immediately after the intervention (Shikatani et al., Reference Shikatani, Antony, Kuo and Cassin2014).

Furthermore, in consideration of the potentially negative consequences of PER, Hofmann and Otto (Reference Hofmann and Otto2008) proposed PER-targeted techniques including aspects of CBT, for example, CR, in their treatment model. As far as we know, only two studies have examined the amelioration of PER with CR. One is from Shikatani et al. (Reference Shikatani, Antony, Kuo and Cassin2014), and the other is from Modini and Abbott (Reference Modini and Abbott2017). In the latter study, researchers recruited 47 adults diagnosed with SAD and randomly assigned them to an intervention or control group. Participants were asked to finish a speech presentation immediately followed by a brief 30-min intervention, including the rationale on CR and guidance on challenging negative thoughts about the speech task (Modini & Abbott, Reference Modini and Abbott2017). Results nevertheless demonstrated that the intervention effect did not differentiate between the intervention and the control group with regard to negative rumination following the speech task (Modini & Abbott, Reference Modini and Abbott2017). A possible explanation was that the cognitive intervention was not specific to PER but was instead applicable to broader cognitive bias, for example, threat appraisal and self-performance evaluation (Modini & Abbott, Reference Modini and Abbott2017), resembling the CR steps used by Shikatani et al. (Reference Shikatani, Antony, Kuo and Cassin2014). The unsupportive evidence left unclear whether the PER-targeted intervention was effective in improving the negative rumination of socially anxious individuals, including adolescents. Additionally, in the present study, one important manipulation which is unlike with Modini and Abbott (Reference Modini and Abbott2017)'s design was that, we would first trigger participants’ original anxiety with hypothetical social situation text, and then intervene their PER and SA by providing social situation-specified realistic thoughts. Our most concern was to target PER, not a range of cognitive processes as mentioned in Modini and Abbott (Reference Modini and Abbott2017), by CR.

CR is a technique designed to alleviate negative emotions by adjusting individuals’ biased cognitions (Ellis, Prather, Grenen, & Ferrer, Reference Ellis, Prather, Grenen and Ferrer2019; Goldin, Morrison, Jazaieri, Heimberg, & Gross, Reference Goldin, Morrison, Jazaieri, Heimberg and Gross2017). More specifically, CR can help individuals replace their distorted thoughts with more adaptive ones by using realistic thinking (McLellan, Alfano, & Hudson, Reference McLellan, Alfano, Hudson, Ranta, La Greca, Garcia-Lopez and Marttunen2015). Namely, adaptative thoughts might be for or against original thoughts (McLellan et al., Reference McLellan, Alfano, Hudson, Ranta, La Greca, Garcia-Lopez and Marttunen2015), e.g., a socially anxious adolescent scared to give a speech, as she/he feared to be observed as being nervous and hand-shaking; however, the adaptive fact might be that her/his hands were a little shaking but might not be found out by others. Another problem in conducting PER-targeted CR for socially anxious individuals, however, is how to develop the intervention procedure experimentally. Morgan and Banerjee (Reference Morgan and Banerjee2008) experimentally manipulated PER by providing a hypothetical social situation text and types of post-event thoughts, for example, ruminative as opposed to reflective; however, their study emphasised the influence of PER on autobiographical memories, not SA symptoms. Moreover, the operation of manipulating reflective post-event processing occurred by providing positive rather than realistic thoughts, for example, ‘I believe I can change and improve my feelings about my new job’ (Morgan & Banerjee, Reference Morgan and Banerjee2008). Despite a lack of CR in Morgan and Banerjee's (Reference Morgan and Banerjee2008) research, the manipulation procedure aiming at PER was a helpful reference. Hence, the main objective of the present study was to intervene PER by developing PER-orientated realistic thoughts suitable for (Chinese) socially anxious adolescents. Furthermore, whether the alleviation of PER could also reduce the distress resulting from SA was our concern. Another important reason to develop our own realistic thoughts was that some concerns about the social situation might be true, for instance, for an adolescent with SA who would blush during a speech task, she/he is possibly teased by others, as our eastern culture does not so encourage people to openly express themselves; but, the result would not be catastraphised as she/he concerns. Hence, unlike positive thoughts from Morgan and Banerjee (Reference Morgan and Banerjee2008), developing culturally appropriate realistic thoughts in the current was necessary.

Moreover, previous findings demonstrated a situational difference phenomenon in PER among participants after undergoing various social events, for example, a speech task as opposed to interaction with others. For instance, Fehm, Schneider, and Hoyer (Reference Fehm, Schneider and Hoyer2007) showed that participants reported higher levels of PER following social interaction as opposed to social performance (e.g., speech task). Conversely, Makkar and Grisham (Reference Makkar and Grisham2011) pointed out that socially anxious individuals are inclined to engage in PER during the speech task. A possible explanation was that, compared to interaction, the speech task was more likely to elicit negative performance appraisals, attracting attention from others and making the social situation ambiguous (Brozovich & Heimberg, Reference Brozovich and Heimberg2008; Makkar & Grisham, Reference Makkar and Grisham2011). By contrast, we preferred the idea proposed by Brozovich and Heimberg (Reference Brozovich and Heimberg2008): The deficiency of instant feedback from audiences in the process of a speech would indeed cause more negative appraisal, increasing the difficulty to verify the assumption about self-performance.

In general, PER may be changed after social situations, but for socially anxious individuals experiencing social occasions, such as a speech task as opposed to interaction with others, does the PER-targeted intervention effect show difference? When Price and Anderson (Reference Price and Anderson2011) conducted group CBT among participants diagnosed with SAD, their results showed a significant reduction in negative PER and SA symptoms. Nevertheless, further multilevel analyses results signified that participants with higher levels of PER at the baseline would relatively benefit less from the intervention. Taken together, we hypothesised that participants experiencing the speech task would benefit less from the PER-targeted intervention.

To summarise, by recruiting socially anxious (Chinese) adolescents, the present study focused on two research questions. First, does PER-targeted CR lead to the decrease in adolescents’ PER and SA level? Second, does the intervention effect show a situational effect?

Materials and Methods

Participants

Two stages were included for recruiting participants in the present study. During the first stage, 17 socially anxious Chinese adolescents who exceeded 50Footnote 1 on the Social Anxiety Scale for Adolescents (SAS-A) (La Greca & Lopez, Reference La Greca and Lopez1998) were recruited to evaluate the realism and emotional arousal of self-developed hypothetical social situation text. Their age range was from 13 to 17 years (M ± SD age = 15.24 ± 1.44). The average SAS-A score was 58.89 (SD = 10.05). During the second phase, another original sample of 907 Chinese adolescents from three urban public middle schools in the Beijing district completed an initial screening procedure using SAS-A. As there was no cut-off value for the Chinese version of SAS-A (Zhou et al., Reference Zhou, Xu, Inglés, Hidalgo and La Greca2008), among 907 participants, adolescents with SAS-A score in the top 10% (Miers, Blöte, Bögels, & Westenberg, Reference Miers, Blöte, Bögels and Westenberg2008; Yu, Westenberg, Li, Wang, & Miers, Reference Yu, Westenberg, Li, Wang and Miers2019) were invited to participate the intervention; finally, 73 high socially anxious Chinese adolescents (30 boys and 43 girls) (and their parents) in total voluntarily accepted our invitation and were then randomly allocated to the speech (n = 37) or interaction (n = 36) group, without control group. They ranged from 12 to 16 years old with the average of 13.93 (SD age = 1.51) and a score of 70.80 on the SAS-A (SD SAS-A = 6.88).

The majority of adolescents’ parents (n = 64; 87.7%) were married, 5.5% were divorced (n = 4), 1.4% (n = 1) were separated, 4.1% (n = 3) were widowed, and 1.4% (n = 1) were remarried. Among which, the most of the participants (95.9%) reported their place of residence as city, 2.7% as county/town, and the rest (1.40%) was from the countryside. Most participants (74.0%) were the only child of the family. In addition, adolescents in the speech or interaction group showed no significant difference in terms of demographic information, including gender, age, place of residence, or parental marriage status. In the two stages, exclusion criteria were as follows: (1) intellectual disability or history of substance abuse, (2) diagnosis with a psychotic disorder, (3) reported suicidal ideation or tendency, or (4) currently receiving psychological or medical therapy. After receiving the intervention, adolescents also were given a small gift for their participation.

Experimental MaterialsFootnote 2

Hypothetical social situation text

The preparation process was conducted in the following procedures. First, we reviewed several intervention handbooks targeting SA (e.g., Hope, Heimberg, Juster, & Turk, Reference Hope, Heimberg, Juster and Turk2000; Rapee et al., Reference Rapee, Lyneham, Schniering, Wuthrich, Abbott, Hudson and Wignall2006) and related literature and drew upon our clinical experiences. Hereby, anxiety-provoking texts for the speech (i.e., give a self-introduction talk in front of classmates at the beginning of a new semester) and interaction (i.e., chat with classmates in the 10-min break in the corridor) situations typically for high socially anxious adolescents were drafted. Second, three research experts in the adolescent SA field and one professor certified by the Academy of Cognitive Therapy were invited to discuss the social encounters.Footnote 3 Third, a psychological practitioner working with adolescents in middle school for more than 3 years was invited to adjust the drafted text. Fourth, to confirm the final version, 17 socially anxious Chinese adolescents were invited to score the text for its realism and emotional arousal.Footnote 4 Finally, the hypothetical social text contained four social encounters and 405 Chinese characters for speech and interaction situations, respectively. For example, ‘… You stand on the platform and introduce yourself. Your classmates are looking at you. You notice that your hands are sort of shaking. You have no idea where to put them and are also concerned that your shaking hands will be noticed by others … ’ (excerpt from speech situation text). An excerpt from interaction situation is given as follow, ‘… You wanted to join them, but you hesitated, wondering if they would like you to join. In the end, you walked over and participated in the discussion with everyone, but found that your voice was shaking when you spoke, and you were not sure whether people really wanted to listen to you …’.

Social situation-specified realistic thoughts

First, the original version of the social situation-specified realistic thoughts was mostly derived from several therapeutic handbooks for SADs (e.g., Hope et al., Reference Hope, Heimberg, Juster and Turk2000; Rapee et al., Reference Rapee, Lyneham, Schniering, Wuthrich, Abbott, Hudson and Wignall2006) but adapted to be specific to the social encounters in the hypothetical social situation. The rationale for developing realistic thoughts relied on the important approaches of cognitive therapy, for instance, de-catastrophisation, looking at the positive side, breaking the emotional reasoning (McLellan et al., Reference McLellan, Alfano, Hudson, Ranta, La Greca, Garcia-Lopez and Marttunen2015). Then, 14 realistic thoughts targeting SA symptoms were drafted for speech and interaction situation, separately.Footnote 5 Second, to modify the wording, we mainly adopted expert evaluation method as those thoughts are more likely to be clinical-experience-independent, i.e., we consulted two Ph.D. candidates in clinical and consulting psychology with more than 5 years’ experience with CBT and one clinical psychology professor certified as CBT therapist and fellow by the Academy of Cognitive Therapy. Finally, 12 realistic thoughts were confirmed for the speech and interaction situations (cf. Morgan & Banerjee, Reference Morgan and Banerjee2008), separately. Example is given from the speech situation: ‘I was undeniably nervous; however, I stood on the platform anyway.’, and from interaction situation: ‘Although chatting with classmates may seem a little nervous, it will not affect my completion of the entire chat process.’, separately.

Measurements

Social anxiety scale for adolescents (SAS-A)

La Greca and Lopez (Reference La Greca and Lopez1998) contained 18 items and three subscales: Fear of Negative Evaluation (SAS-A-FNE), Social Avoidance Specific to New Situations or Unfamiliar Peers (SAS-A-New), and Social Avoidance and Distress in General (SAS-A-G). According to the degree to which the item ‘is true for you’, SAS-A was rated on a 5-point Likert scale from 1 (not at all) to 5 (all the time). The Chinese version of the SAS-A was revised by Zhou et al. (Reference Zhou, Xu, Inglés, Hidalgo and La Greca2008), showing a satisfactory reliability and validity. Cronbach's coefficient for the SAS-A in the current study was 0.59, and the alpha coefficients for three subscales were 0.76, 0.66, and 0.60, separately (N = 907).Footnote 6

Short mood and feelings questionnaire (SMFQ)

Angold, Costello, Messer and Pickles (Reference Angold, Costello, Messer and Winder1995) were used to measure depressive emotion in children and adolescents aged 8–16 years. The SMFQ included 13 items and was rated on a 3-point Likert from 0 (not true) to 2 (true). The Chinese version of the SMFQ was culturally revised by Cheng, Cao, and Su (Reference Cheng, Cao and Su2009) and showed reliable psychometric properties. The internal consistency coefficient in the current study was 0.84 (N = 907).

Post-event processing inventory-trait (PEPI-T)

Blackie and Kocovski (Reference Blackie and Kocovski2017) were to measure PER following all types of social situations and showed an excellent psychometric property. PEPI-T contained 12 items asking participants to rate on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = ‘strongly disagree’, 5 = ‘strongly agree’). The Chinse version of the PEPI-T was revised for adolescents and showed an acceptable reliability and validity (Yu, Pan, Xu, Zhu, & Wang, Reference Yu, Pan, Xu, Zhu and Wang2020). PEPI-T was administered during the screening stage in the present study, and its internal consistency coefficient was 0.85 (N = 907).

Post-Event Processing Inventory-Trait-Revised (PEPI-T-R) was adapted by authors from the PEPI-T to measure rumination pre- and post-intervention. The main revisions focused on the instructions and the verb tense in items. The PEPI-T-R was administered twice in the current study. During the first measurement, participants were asked to rate the degree of agreement (agree or disagree) with statements after reading the hypothetical social situation text. The verb tense was changed from the present to future, for example, ‘I will think about how poorly the situation will go’. During the second measurement, after the reading realistic thoughts, that is, receiving cognitive appraisal intervention, the socially anxious adolescents were asked to rate again how they will think when encountering similar social situations in the future. The Cronbach's coefficient for the PEPI-T-R pre- and post-intervention in the current study was 0.87 and 0.91, respectively (n = 73).

Emotional thermometer

The emotional thermometer was adapted from Higa and Daleiden (Reference Higa and Daleiden2008) to measure SA pre- and post-intervention. As the protagonist in the hypothetical text, participants were asked to rate from 0 (not at all) to 10 (extremely) how worried they would be about looking foolish to others in a particular situation. SA was surveyed twice, immediately after reading the hypothetical social situation text and immediately after reading the realistic thoughts.

Experimental Procedure

This research was approved by the ethics committee of the corresponding author's university. The research was completed in two sessions. At the baseline measurement, adolescents were invited to fill out a packet of questionnaires, including the SAS-A, the SMFQ, the PEPI-T, and the demographic information questionnaire. Then, those participants whose SAS-A score ranked in the top 10% (Miers et al., Reference Miers, Blöte, Bögels and Westenberg2008; Yu et al., Reference Yu, Westenberg, Li, Wang and Miers2019) were invited to participate in the intervention experiment 1 week later. Once informed written consents were acquired from participants and at least one parent, the adolescents were randomly assigned to the speech or interaction group.

At the appointed time participants attended the second session. Sitting before a laptop, each participant was instructed to read the hypothetical social situation (speech or interaction) text displayed on the screen with no time limit. They were requested to try their best to visualise them locating in the hypothetical social situations. This manipulation was expected to induce participants’ SA (Morgan & Banerjee, Reference Morgan and Banerjee2008). Once participants had finished reading the text, they reported their PER and SA levels (T1) by filling in the PEPI-T-R and the emotional thermometer. Then, they were instructed to read the situation-specified realistic thoughts displayed on the laptop screen; thus, they received CR intervention. Each realistic thought statement was presented for 10 s. When all 12 realistic thought sentences had been presented in sequence, adolescents were asked to make a choice which two statements were the most believable. The choice of the statement was not included in the analyses but to ensure that the adolescents were indeed engaged in reading the sentences (Morgan & Banerjee, Reference Morgan and Banerjee2008).

Following the above-mentioned procedure, via completing the PEPI-T-R and using the emotional thermometer, participants were asked to report again their PER and SA level (T2). Finally, participants were asked to choose the most fearful social situation among the given choices. This task was designed to check whether the group — speech and interaction — to which the participants were randomly assigned matched the social situation they feared in reality. All participants took part in the current study individually and were thanked for their participation with a small gift.

Data Analyses

Participants who dropped out of the study (n = 1) at any stage were excluded from the data analyses. Hence, the final sample comprised 73 socially anxious adolescent participants. First, the Pearson chi-squared test was used to examine whether the group to which the participants were randomly assigned matched the social situation they actually were in fear of. The Pearson correlation analyses were adopted for investigating the correlation coefficients between variables at the baseline and pre- and post-intervention. Tests of heterogeneity of variance were conducted before ANOVAs for analysing the group differences between variables. ANCOVAs, with depression as a covariate to control the comorbidity effect, were used for analysing the intervention effect and the effect between groups. Hierarchical regression analysis was adopted to investigate whether the intervention targeting PER could also effectively alleviate SA symptoms. All the analyses were conducted with SPSS 22.0. In addition, Cohen's d was adopted to demonstrate the intervention effect and was interpreted as follows: 0.20, 0.05, and 0.80 for small, medium, and large effect, respectively (Cohen, Reference Cohen1988).

Results

Descriptive Statistics and Bivariate Relations

The Pearson chi-squared test showed no significant difference (x 2 (1) = 0.01, p = 0.922), suggesting a random grouping. Further Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) results demonstrated no significant difference between speech and interaction participants on the SAS-A, SMFQ, and PEPI-T (F(1,71) = 0.003–1.67, ps > 0.200), suggesting the homogeneity of the participants. As shown in Table 1, Pearson correlation analyses results demonstrated that PEPI-T significantly correlated with PER_T1 and PER_T2. Score on the SMFQ was significantly (rs = 0.24–0.35, ps < 0.039) associated with SA_T1 and PER pre- and post-intervention. The SMFQ also significantly associated with SA (r = 32, p = 0.007) and the PER level measured at the baseline (r = 31, p = 0.008). Associations between SA and PER scores measured at pre- and post-intervention displayed at significantly moderate to high level (rs = 0.44–0.80, ps < 0.001).

Table 1. Bivariate Relations Among Variables

Note: *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001. SAS-A = Social Anxiety Scale for Adolescents measured at the baseline; PEPI-T = Post-Event Processing Inventory-Trait measured at the baseline; SMFQ = Short Mood and Feelings Questionnaire measured at the baseline; PER_T1 = the PER score measured immediately after reading the hypothetical social situation text; PER_T2 = the PER score measured immediately after receiving the CR intervention; SA_T1 = the social anxiety score measured at Time 1; SA_T2 = the social anxiety score measured at Time 2, that is, immediately after receiving CR intervention.

The Intervention Effect of CR

As shown in Table 2 and Figure 1, after controlling for depression, participants’ PER and SA levels significantly decreased after the intervention (ps < 0.001) with moderate to high effect size (d = 0.71–0.84). Hierarchical regression analysis further demonstrated that, after inputting depression in the first step as covariate ((β = 0.11, ΔR 2 = 0.01, p = 0.356), the decrease in PER could significantly predict the improvement of SA symptoms immediately after receiving CR intervention (β = 0.55, ΔR 2 = 0.30, p < 0.001).

Table 2. ANCOVA Results of PER and SA Pre- and Post-Intervention

Note: ***p < 0.001. PER = post-event rumination; SA = social anxiety.

Figure 1. Score change of PER and SA pre- and post-intervention. Note: PER — the average item score; SA — the sum score.

The Situational Effect of CR Intervention

As shown in Table 3, unexpected in the hypothesis, the change in PER and SA, that is, the intervention effect, showed no difference among participants from speech and interaction groups. From the perspective of PER and SA levels pre- and post-intervention, no significant difference between groups was shown even after controlling for depression (F(1,70) = 1.20–1.38, ps > 0.132). Additional ANCOVA results showed no significant difference between PEPI-T and PER_T1 (F(1) = 2.91, p = 0.090, $\eta _p^2$ = 0.02) after controlling for depression; however, a significant difference was found between the PEPI-T and PER_T2 (F(1) = 39.98, p < 0.001, $\eta _p^2$ = 0.22) after controlling for depressive emotion. Thus, this finding also demonstrated the effectiveness of the one-session intervention for targeting PER.

Table 3. Analysis of Variance Results Among State PER and Social Anxiety

Note: aChange = the score of post-intervention minus pre-intervention; bSMFQ score was controlled as a covariate when analysing the state of PER and social anxiety. PER_T1 = the PER score measured immediately after reading the hypothetical social situation text; PER_T2 = the PER score measured immediately after receiving the CR intervention; SA_T1 = the social anxiety score measured at Time 1; SA_T2 = the social anxiety score measured at Time 2 (i.e., immediately after receiving CR intervention).

Discussion and conclusions

To our best knowledge, the present study was the first attempt to target PER with the rationale of CBT among socially anxious adolescents and yielded encouraging findings. On the basis of the cognitive model of SAD proposed by Clark and Wells (Reference Clark, Wells, Heimberg, Liebowitz, Hope and Schneier1995), we developed PER-targeted realistic thoughts to examine their intervention effect. Results showed that, albeit no control group was included, one-session CR still showed its potential to improve rumination and SA symptoms among adolescents with high levels of SA, with moderate to high effect size. This is parallel with previous results with CBT among adults (e.g., McEvoy et al., Reference McEvoy, Mahoney, Perini and Kingsep2009; Modini & Abbott, Reference Modini and Abbott2017). Furthermore, regression analysis result showed that the reduction of PER could be significantly predictive of SA, which also parallels the previous findings in an adult population (e.g., Price & Anderson, Reference Price and Anderson2011). Although a randomised control group was not adopted in the current study, the significant difference between the rumination score measured at the baseline and post-intervention still suggested the intervention effect of PER-targeted method. Unexpectedly, the intervention did not show situational effect.

The improvement in individuals’ PER often benefits from the change of negative self-perception and catastrophic estimation to social costs (Hofmann & Otto, Reference Hofmann and Otto2008). The current study provided realistic thoughts specific to social encounters to reduce negative self-perceptions in socially anxious adolescents (e.g., ‘My classmates might notice my embarrassment, but most of them also dislike giving speeches, so this is not a big deal.’) and to decatastrophise the social costs (e.g., ‘Although I was a little awkward during the conversation, they'll soon forget about it.’). Notably, participants were primed with realistic thoughts (e.g., ‘I was undeniably nervous; however, I stood on the platform anyway.’) to notice positive cues. This assisted them in expanding their scope of attention (Price & Anderson, Reference Price and Anderson2011) and in more objectively judging their social outcomes. Once less obsessed with focusing on negative details, they became less anxious. From the perspective of intervention, the hypothesis proposed in the cognitive model — that PER is key to the persistence of SAD (Clark & Wells, Reference Clark, Wells, Heimberg, Liebowitz, Hope and Schneier1995) — was tested again in the present study.

Previous research conducted with adults showed that CBT can moderately to highly improve PER and SA symptoms (e.g., 7-week intervention) (McEvoy et al., Reference McEvoy, Mahoney, Perini and Kingsep2009). Notably, in the current study, the PER-targeted invention also yielded a moderate to high effect size among highly anxious adolescents, illustrating that one-session CR demonstrated its potential value in improving the social-related distorted cognition. Adolescence is characterised by changes in social — effective and — cognitive abilities (Haller, Kadosh, Scerif, & Lau, Reference Haller, Kadosh, Scerif and Lau2015); therefore, adolescents’ emotion and ratiocination about outside social world and others’ mental states would easily fluctuate in response to social cues (Haller et al., Reference Haller, Kadosh, Scerif and Lau2015). Because of its nonfixed nature, targeted intervention can loosen their biased cognitions and make the alleviation of symptoms of anxiety and distress possible; moreover, compared to children, adolescents have the advantage of better understanding and ability to learn (Crone & Dahl, Reference Crone and Dahl2012). These age characteristics favour the application of one-session PER-targeted CR for adolescents.

Unexpectedly, the intervention effect did not show group differences in the current study. Combining results from sections The Intervention Effect of Cognitive Restructuring and The Situational Effect of Cognitive Restructuring Intervention, the level of PER was indeed significantly lower right after receiving CR than that both at the baseline and after reading hypothetical social situation; however, after reading the PER-targeted realistic thoughts, socially anxious adolescents from two groups did not differentiate on PER and SA scores. In addition, the amount of reduction from pre- to post-intervention also did not differ between the two groups. A possible explanation would be that the hypothetical text provoking SA, especially for interaction, was still imaginary, despite the high score on realism and emotional arousal. This might limit the participants’ engagement in real conversational situations and not receiving feedback from partners with no chance to affirm self-image in others’ eyes; therefore, participants visualising themselves in the hypothetical interaction situation still reported similar levels of rumination and anxiety by contrast to those in the speech task group. Nevertheless, in the consideration of convenience, the hypothetical social situation texts still effectively provoked SA.

Albeit the present study yielded notable findings on intervening PER and SA among Chinese socially anxious adolescents, several important limitations require consideration. First, highly socially anxious adolescents, instead of a group with a diagnosis of SAD, were recruited to conduct a preventive intervention in the current study; so the findings cannot be generalised to clinical patients. Second, because of the changing characteristics of adolescents and the special requirements of the school, no randomised control group was adopted; however, compared to the rumination score measured at baseline, the rumination level immediately after the intervention still showed a significant decrease. Future research should incorporate control group to re-examine the effect of this one-session CR intervention for socially anxious adolescents. Third, developing PER-targeted CR sentences specific to social situations (e.g., social encounters) requires researchers armed with CBT clinical practice, which to some extent would constrain the wide application of this method. Fourth, to examine the stability of the intervention effect over time, a follow-up measurement should have been required. Fifth, because the hypothetical texts were visualised, more vivid methods, such as Apps with interactive functions would be useful in future research. Finally, as the sample size in the current study was not-large, results should be cautiously explained and future research should re-examine the situation effect in a larger population of socially anxious adolescents.

Despite the limitations noted above, the current study still was the first effort to examine the intervention effect of targeting PER using aspects of CBT among socially anxious (Chinese) adolescents and yield clinical implications. Several cognitive models of SAD (Clark & Wells, Reference Clark, Wells, Heimberg, Liebowitz, Hope and Schneier1995; Hofmann, Reference Hofmann2007; Rapee & Heimberg, Reference Rapee and Heimberg1997) have shown that after engaging in social events, highly anxious individuals often repetitively ruminate on their mistakes and negative performance. With the sense of shame, and their levels of self-confidence were then gradually reduced. To relieve the distress, individuals might avoid social situations and become more isolated from others. Clinically, PER may further increase individuals’ suicide rate (Clark & Wells, Reference Clark, Wells, Heimberg, Liebowitz, Hope and Schneier1995); consequently, the intervention aiming at PER, especially in adolescents’ population, would effectively relieve rumination and anxious symptoms. More importantly, the standardised intervention procedure may have made the operation process easier to follow and the duration shorter. In addition, the one-session PER-targeted method presented on a computer screen is easy to conduct, suggesting the inclusion of this method in psychology classes for (Chinese) adolescents as a preventive measure is possible and perhaps essential in future research and practice. To sum up, the present study endeavoured its first effort to conduct the PER-oriented intervention with CR, and proved it in socially anxious Chinese adolescents’ sample. Future research should be concerned with the cultural examination of the intervention effect's diversity and uniformity.

Acknowledgements

None.

Funding

This work is funded by Social Science Fund of Ministry of Education of the People's Republic of China (grant No. 21YJC190020) for the first author.

Declaration of interest

None.

Footnotes

1 A cut-off value of 50 was adopted. The SAS-A has not had a cut-off value for Chinese adolescents yet (Yu et al., Reference Yu, Westenberg, Li, Wang and Miers2019; Zhou, Xu, Inglés, Hidalgo, & La Greca, Reference Zhou, Xu, Inglés, Hidalgo and La Greca2008); hence, the cut-off value for U.S. adolescents was used in the current study.

2 The experimental materials will be available for researchers when in contact with the first or corresponding author.

3 A social encounter is defined as a social occasion that provokes social anxiety (Morgan & Banerjee, Reference Morgan and Banerjee2008).

4 Evaluation results demonstrated that the average for realism was 8.06 out of 10 (SD = 1.44) and for emotional arousal was 7.06 out of 10 (SD = 1.52), suggesting that the hypothetical social situation would effectively trigger socially anxious adolescents’ anxiety.

5 For the speech, among the drafted 14 realistic thought sentences, 6 of them were reworked by Hope et al. (Reference Hope, Heimberg, Juster and Turk2000), two were revised by Morgan and Banerjee (Reference Morgan and Banerjee2008), 2 adapted from Hope, Heimberg, and Turk (Reference Hope, Heimberg and Turk2010), 1 adapted from Miers et al. (Reference Miers, Blöte, Bögels and Westenberg2008), 1 from McLellan et al. (Reference McLellan, Alfano, Hudson, Ranta, La Greca, Garcia-Lopez and Marttunen2015), and the rest were self-develped depending on own clinical experience. With respect to interaction situation, six of which were adapted from Hope et al. (Reference Hope, Heimberg, Juster and Turk2000), two revised from Hope et al. (Reference Hope, Heimberg and Turk2010), two recomposed from Rapee et al. (Reference Rapee, Lyneham, Schniering, Wuthrich, Abbott, Hudson and Wignall2006), and the rest were developed by the first author relying on own experience.

6 Although the internal consistency coefficient of the SAS-A in the present study was not ideal, the alpha coefficient of subscale SAS-A-FNE was acceptable. A possible explanation was that the participants in the current study were without a diagnosis of SAD, other than subclinical adolescents screened by SAS-A. The negative cognitions about others’ evaluation characterize their main clinical feature; however, the fear or anxiety did not cause clinical distress or impairment in social, learning, or other important functioning. Given this, the Cronbach's coefficient for the SAS-A was still acceptable.

References

Abbott, MJ and Rapee, RM (2004). Post-event rumination and negative self-appraisal in social phobia before and after treatment. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113, 136144. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.113.1.136CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Albano, AM (1995). Treatment of social anxiety in adolescents. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 2, 271298. doi:10.1016/S1077-7229(95)80014-X.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing.Google Scholar
Angold, A, Costello, EJ, Messer, SC and Winder, F (1995). Development of a short questionnaire for use in epidemiological studies of depression in children and adolescents. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 5, 237249. doi:1049-8931.95/040237-13.Google Scholar
Beesdo, K, Bittner, A, Pine, DS, Stein, MB, Hofler, M, Lieb, R and Wittchen, H-U (2007). Incidence of social anxiety disorder and the consistent risk for secondary depression in the first three decades of life. Archives of General Psychiatry, 64, 903912. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.64.8.903.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blackie, RA and Kocovski, NL (2017). Development and validation of the trait and state versions of the post-event processing inventory. Anxiety, Stress and Coping, 30(2), 202218. doi:10.1080/10615806.2016.1230668CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blöte, AW and Westenberg, PM (2007). Socially anxious adolescents’ perception of treatment by classmates. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 189198. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2006.02.002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blöte, AW, Miers, AC, Heyne, DA and Westenberg, PM (2015). Social anxiety and the school environment of adolescents. In Ranta, K, La Greca, AM, Garcia-Lopez, L-J and Marttunen, M (Eds.), Social anxiety and phobia in adolescents. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-16703-9Google Scholar
Blöte, AW, Miers, AC, Van den Bos, E and Westenberg, PM (2019). The role of performance quality in adolescents’ self-evaluation and Rumination after a speech: Is it contingent on social anxiety level? Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 47(2),148163. doi:10.1017/S1352465818000310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brozovich, F and Heimberg, RG (2008). An analysis of post-event processing in social anxiety disorder. Clinical Psychology Review, 28, 891903. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2008.01.002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheng, P, Cao, F and Su, L (2009). Reliability and validity of the short mood and feelings questionnaire in Chinese adolescent. Chinese Mental Health Journal, 23, 6063. doi:10.3969/j.issn.1000-6729.2009.01.016.Google Scholar
Clark, DM and Wells, A (1995). A cognitive model of social phobia. In Heimberg, RG, Liebowitz, MR, Hope, DA and Schneier, FR (Eds.), Social phobia: Diagnosis, assessment, and treatment (pp. 6390). New York, NY: The Guildford Press.Google Scholar
Cohen, J (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Costello, EJ, Mustillo, S, Erkanli, A, Keeler, G and Angold, A (2003). Prevalence and development of psychiatric disorders in childhood and adolescence. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60, 837844. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.60.8.837.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crone, EA and Dahl, RE (2012). Understanding adolescence as a period of social–affective engagement and goal flexibility. Nature Reviews Neurosciences, 13, 636650. doi:10.1038/nrn3313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edwards, SL, Rapee, RM and Franklin, J (2003). Postevent rumination and recall bias for a social performance event in high and low socially anxious individuals. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27, 603617. doi:10.1023/A:1026395526858.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, EM, Prather, AA, Grenen, EG and Ferrer, RA (2019). Direct and indirect associations of cognitive reappraisal and suppression with disease biomarkers. Psychology & Health, 34, 336354. doi:10.1080/08870446.2018.1529313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fehm, L, Schneider, G and Hoyer, J (2007). Is post-event processing specific for social anxiety? Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 38, 1122. doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2006.02.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldin, PR, Morrison, AS, Jazaieri, H, Heimberg, RG and Gross, JJ (2017). Trajectories of social anxiety, cognitive reappraisal, and mindfulness during an RCT of CBGT versus MBSR for social anxiety disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 97, 113. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2017.06.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greco, LA and Morris, TL (2005). Factors influencing the link between social anxiety and peer acceptance : Contributions of social skills and close friendships during middle childhood. Behavior Therapy, 36, 197205. doi:005-7894/05/0197-0205$1.00/0.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haller, SPW, Kadosh, KC, Scerif, G and Lau, JYF (2015). Social anxiety disorder in adolescence: How developmental cognitive neuroscience findings may shape understanding and interventions for psychopathology. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 1120. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2015.02.002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayward, C, Varady, S, Albano, AM, Thienemann, M, Henderson, L and Schatzberg, AF (2000). Cognitive-behavioral group therapy for social phobia in female adolescents : Results of a pilot study. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 721726. doi:10.1097/00004583-200006000-00010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Higa, CK and Daleiden, EL (2008). Social anxiety and cognitive biases in non-referred children: The interaction of self-focused attention and threat interpretation biases. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 22, 441452. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2007.05.005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hirsch, CR, Meeten, F, Krahé, C and Reeder, C (2016). Resolving ambiguity in emotional disorders: The nature and role of interpretation biases. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 12, 281305. doi:10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-021815-093436.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hodson, KJ, McManus, FV, Clark, DM and Doll, H (2008). Can Clark and Wells’ (1995) cognitive model of social phobia be applied to young people? Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 36, 449461. doi:10.1017/S1352465808004487CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofmann, SG (2007). Cognitive factors that maintain social anxiety disorder: A comprehensive model and its treatment implications. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 36, 193209. doi:10.1080/16506070701421313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hofmann, SG and Otto, MW (2008). Cognitive behavioral therapy for social anxiety disorder: evidenced-based and disorder-specific treatment techniques. New York: Routledge Taylor & Francis Group.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hope, DA, Heimberg, RG, Juster, HR and Turk, CL (2000). Managing social anxiety: A cognitive-behavioral therapy approach (client workbook) (D. Barlow, ed.). New York, NY: Graywind Publications Incorporated.Google Scholar
Hope, DA, Heimberg, RG and Turk, CL (2010). Managing social anxiety: A cognitive-behavioral therapy approach (therapist guide) (2nd ed.) (D. Barlow, ed.). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kessler, RC, Mcgonagle, KA, Zhao, S, Nelson, CB, Hughes, M, Eshleman, S, … Kendler, KS (1994). Lifetime and 12-month prevalence of DSM-III-R psychiatric disorders in the United States. Archives of General Psychiatry, 51, 819. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1994.03950010008002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kiko, S, Stevens, S, Mall, AK, Steil, R, Bohus, M and Hermann, C (2012). Predicting post-event processing in social anxiety disorder following two prototypical social situations: State variables and dispositional determinants. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 50, 617626. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2012.06.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kocovski, NL and Rector, NA (2008). Post-event processing in social anxiety disorder: Idiosyncratic priming in the course of CBT. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 32, 2336. doi:10.1007/s10608-007-9152-z.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
La Greca, AM and Lopez, N (1998). Social anxiety among adolescents: Linkages with peer relations and friendships. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 26, 8394. doi:0091-0627/98/0400-0083$15.00/0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Makkar, SR and Grisham, JR (2011). The predictors and contents of post-event processing in social anxiety. Cognitive Therapy Research, 35, 118133. doi:10.1007/s10608-011-9357-z.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McEvoy, PM, Mahoney, A, Perini, SJ and Kingsep, P (2009). Changes in post-event processing and metacognitions during cognitive behavioral group therapy for social phobia. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 23, 617623. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2009.01.011.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLellan, LF, Alfano, CA and Hudson, JL (2015). Cognition-focused interventions for social anxiety disorder among adolescents. In Ranta, K, La Greca, AM, Garcia-Lopez, L-J and Marttunen, M (Eds.), Social anxiety and phobia in adolescents: development, manifestation and intervention strategies (pp. 225250). Switzerland: Springer International Publishing. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-16703-9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miers, AC, Blöte, AW, Bögels, SM and Westenberg, PM (2008). Interpretation bias and social anxiety in adolescents. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 22, 14621471. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2008.02.010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Modini, M and Abbott, MJ (2017). Negative rumination in social anxiety: A randomised trial investigating the effects of a brief intervention on cognitive processes before, during and after a social situation. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 55, 7380. doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2016.12.002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morgan, J and Banerjee, R (2008). Post-event processing and autobiographical memory in social anxiety: The influence of negative feedback and rumination. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 22, 11901204. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2008.01.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Price, M and Anderson, PL (2011). The impact of cognitive behavioral therapy on post event processing among those with social anxiety disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 49, 132137. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2010.11.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ranta, K, Kaltiala-Heino, R, Fröjd, S and Marttunen, M (2013). Peer victimization and social phobia : A follow-up study among adolescents. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 48, 533544. doi:10.1007/s00127-012-0583-9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rapee, RM and Heimberg, RG (1997). A cognitive-behavioral model of anxiety in soical phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35, 741756.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rapee, RM, Lyneham, HJ, Schniering, CA, Wuthrich, V, Abbott, MA, Hudson, JL and Wignall, A (2006). Cool kids: Child & adolescent anxiety program (client workbook). Sydney, Australia: Centre for Emotional Health, Macquarie University.Google Scholar
Shikatani, B, Antony, MM, Kuo, JR and Cassin, SE (2014). The impact of cognitive restructuring and mindfulness strategies on postevent processing and affect in social anxiety disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 28, 570579. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2014.05.012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Westenberg, PM, Gullone, E, Bokhorst, CL, Heyne, DA and King, NJ (2007). Social evaluation fear in childhood and adolescence: Normative developmental course and continuity of individual differences. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 25, 471483. doi:10.1348/026151006X173099.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Westenberg, PM, Bokhorst, CL, Miers, AC, Sumter, SR, Kallen, VL, van Pelt, J and Blöte, AW (2009). A prepared speech in front of a pre-recorded audience: Subjective, physiological, and neuroendocrine responses to the Leiden Public Speaking Task. Biological Psychology, 82, 116124. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.06.005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wittchen, HU, Stein, MB and Kessler, RC (1999). Social fears and social phobia in a community sample of adolescents and young adults : prevalence, risk factors and co-morbidity. Psychological Medicine, 29, 309323. doi:10.1017/S0033291798008174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yu, M, Westenberg, PM, Li, W, Wang, J and Miers, AC (2019). Cultural evidence for interpretation bias as a feature of social anxiety in Chinese adolescents. Anxiety, Stress, & Coping, 32, 376386. doi:10.1080/10615806.2019.1598556.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yu, M, Pan, J, Xu, Q, Zhu, Y and Wang, J (2020). The psychometric properties of post-event processing inventory-trait (PEPI-T) among Chinese adolescents. Chinese Journal of Behavioral Medicine and Brain Science, 29, 943947. doi:10.3760/cma.j.cn371468-20200215-01030.Google Scholar
Zhou, X, Xu, Q, Inglés, CJ, Hidalgo, MD and La Greca, AM (2008). Reliability and validity of the Chinese version of the Social Anxiety Scale for Adolescents. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 39, 185200. doi:10.1007/s10578-007-0079-0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Bivariate Relations Among Variables

Figure 1

Table 2. ANCOVA Results of PER and SA Pre- and Post-Intervention

Figure 2

Figure 1. Score change of PER and SA pre- and post-intervention. Note: PER — the average item score; SA — the sum score.

Figure 3

Table 3. Analysis of Variance Results Among State PER and Social Anxiety