Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-hvd4g Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-10T10:32:32.198Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The role of simulation in imagery rescripting for post-traumatic stress disorder: a single case series

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 November 2020

Kathy Looney*
Affiliation:
Royal Holloway University, London, UK
Sharif El-Leithy
Affiliation:
Traumatic Stress Service, Springfield Hospital, London, UK
Gary Brown
Affiliation:
Royal Holloway University, London, UK
*
*Corresponding author. Email: kathy.looney@ucd.ie
Get access
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background:

Imagery rescripting (ImRs) is an experiential therapy technique used to change the content and meaning of intrusive imagery in post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) by imagining alternative endings to traumatic events. There is growing evidence that ImRs is an effective treatment for PTSD; however, little is known about how it brings about change.

Aims:

This study aimed to explore the role of mental simulation as a candidate mechanism of action in ImRs, and, specifically, whether well-simulated imagery rescripts are associated with greater change in symptom severity during ImRs.

Method:

Using a single-case experimental design, seven participants receiving cognitive therapy for PTSD were assessed before, during and after sessions of imagery rescripting for one intrusive image. Participants completed continuous symptom severity measures. Sessions were recorded, then coded for goodness of simulation (GOS) as well as additional factors (e.g. rescript believability, vividness).

Results:

Participants were divided into high- and low-responders and coding was compared across groups. Correlational analyses were supported by descriptive analysis of individual sessions. High-responders’ rescripts tended to be rated as well-simulated compared with those of low-responders. Specific factors (e.g. intensity of thoughts/emotions related to original and new imagery elements, level of cognitive and emotional shift and belief in the resultant rescript) were also associated with reductions in symptom severity.

Conclusions:

There was tentative evidence that well-simulated rescripted images tended to be associated with greater reductions in symptom severity of the target image. Clinical implications and avenues for further research are discussed.

Type
Main
Copyright
© British Association for Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapies 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Introduction

Imagery rescripting (ImRs) is a therapeutic intervention that involves imagining alternative, hypothetical endings to traumatic events. Used to target intrusive images in post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (Rusch et al., Reference Rusch, Grunert, Mendelsohn and Smucker2000), ImRs contrasts with imaginal exposure/reliving techniques in trauma-focused cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT), which typically involve vividly and realistically reliving the trauma memory as it actually happened (Ehlers and Clark, Reference Ehlers and Clark2000; Foa and Rothbaum, Reference Foa and Rothbaum1998).

While there is a growing body of evidence supporting the effectiveness of ImRs in reducing PTSD symptomology (e.g. Arntz et al., Reference Arntz, Tiesema and Kindt2007; Ehlers et al., Reference Ehlers, Clark, Hackmann, McManus and Fennell2005; Grunert et al., Reference Grunert, Weis, Smucker and Christianson2007), there is also a recognition of the need for further systematic research into its underlying change mechanisms (Arntz, Reference Arntz2012) and the aspects of therapy that bear upon these mechanisms. Two competing ideas as to how ImRs might produce change have emerged. Applying a retrieval competition account (Brewin, Reference Brewin2006; Brewin et al., Reference Brewin, Gregory, Lipton and Burgess2010), ImRs may create a competing, less distressing memory representation of the traumatic event, which is more accessible than the original memory in response to retrieval cues and therefore preferentially retrieved, inhibiting the old image (Brewin et al., Reference Brewin, Wheatley, Patel, Fearon, Hackmann and Wells2009). Others have argued that ImRs changes the underlying meaning of the original memory image such that when triggers are encountered, an altered, less-distressing memory representation is recalled (Arntz, Reference Arntz, Thoma and McKay2015; Arntz and Weertman, Reference Arntz and Weertman1999).

There is considerable variation in approaches to conducting ImRs sessions (e.g. Arntz and Weertman Reference Arntz and Weertman1999; Smucker and Niederee Reference Smucker and Niederee1995; Wild and Clarke, Reference Wilde and Clarke2011), so improving our understanding of how ImRs elicits change in the memory representation could help inform how to best deliver the technique (Arntz, Reference Arntz2012). To provide a basis for gaining further knowledge in this domain, Salter et al. (Reference Salter, Parker, Brown, El Leithy and Wheatly2015) developed a comprehensive coding scheme based on close analysis of the content of ImRs sessions during PTSD treatment. Textual analysis of session transcripts using the coding framework suggested various discernible processes, including the activation of thoughts and feelings, cognitive and emotional shifts, and believability of the rescript. Preliminary evidence for the association of these factors with ImRs efficacy was also demonstrated in a series of cases (Salter et al., Reference Salter, Parker, Brown, El Leithy and Wheatly2015).

The present study builds on this research by moving beyond ‘bottom up’ processes that are directly observable within therapy sessions. Instead, it considers a conceptual ‘top down’ property of rescripted images, namely the quality of their mental simulation. According to the simulation heuristic (Kahneman and Tversky, Reference Kahneman, Tversky, Kahneman, Slovik and Tversky1982; Tversky and Kahneman, Reference Tversky and Kahneman1973), the properties of imagined scenarios that lend them believability relate to the ease with which a mental model of a hypothetical situation can be created. In people with clinical levels of anxiety, this ‘goodness of simulation’ (GOS; how well they could simulate a hypothetical event in imagination) has been associated with higher subjective probability ratings for hypothetical, negative events as well as increased access to these simulations (Raune et al., Reference Raune, MacLeod and Holmes2005). Similarly, when simulating imagined positive outcomes to future stressful events, better simulations have been linked to higher subjective probability ratings and less worry (Brown et al., Reference Brown, MacLeod, Tata and Goddard2002). GOS may therefore contribute to an understanding of how ImRs might work. Specifically, both ImRs and GOS relate to the mental construction of imagined events that did not, or could not, happen. It may be that rescripted images that are better simulated, and so more accessible, are therefore more compelling (believable), and so more effective in competing with or modifying PTSD intrusive imagery. In this way the present study considers whether effective ImRs targets intrusive imagery by enhancing the simulation, and hence believability, of new, less distressing images and their associated meanings (Arntz, Reference Arntz2012).

Current study

The broad aim of this study was to examine aspects of ImRs that may account for therapeutic change. Explanations of how and why effective therapies work are crucial for optimising therapy to produce change (Kazdin, Reference Kazdin2007). In the face of the inherent complexity of this type of research, and the large number of potential factors interacting dynamically, one potential strategy would be to focus on a small, manageable set of potential ImRs change factors across a large number of participants while attempting to keep potential confounding factors constant. However, it has been argued that research questions that focus on investigating functional relationships between variables and the processes underlying particular phenomena do not lend themselves to traditional null-hypothesis significance testing (Smith and Little, Reference Smith and Little2018). Furthermore, conducting a larger scale confirmatory study poses two main difficulties: (1) how to identify those ImRs factors that should be targeted for investigation and (2) running the risk of excluding variables that could be central to the process of therapeutic change in ImRs. Sidman (Reference Sidman1960) has highlighted the importance of identifying the relevant controlling variables for the phenomenon under study and argues that small-n designs are the most appropriate approach for this purpose.

Studies of small samples of cases in greater detail is considered preferable when the research question relates to the processes and conditions that give rise to a particular effect, rather than demonstrating the existence of a particular phenomenon (Normand, Reference Normand2016). In this way, a wider range of variables can be encompassed within the study to determine which factors seem to be the most promising in terms of accounting for change during ImRs, paving the way for subsequent, larger scale confirmatory studies (Smith and Little, Reference Smith and Little2018).

In line with these considerations, the present study employed a single case experimental design (SCED). SCEDs involve comparing performance under different conditions within an individual, rather than either within or between groups (Kazdin, Reference Kazdin1978). A number of single cases can be tracked, allowing for patterns to emerge across a series of individual cases. Rather than using a control group, SCED relies on repeated measurement, following participants for a period of time before and during treatment (Turpin, Reference Turpin, Mace, Moorey and Roberts2001).

Seven participants were followed before, during and after rescripting of one image during treatment for PTSD. Participants completed continuous measures of the frequency and distress of images across sessions. To capture the dynamic processes at play in ImRs, recordings of sessions were coded using two separate coding schemes. The first was a refined version of the Session Content scheme of Salter et al. (Reference Salter, Parker, Brown, El Leithy and Wheatly2015), designed to capture those aspects of ImRs that are directly observable. The GOS coding scheme (Rose et al., Reference Rose, Ellett, Huddy and Brown2019) was also used to capture less readily observable characteristics such as logical flow, ease of imagining and sequencing. It was hypothesised that reductions in frequency and distress would be associated with higher GOS ratings and that aspects of the ImRs process previously shown to be associated with improvement (e.g. cognitive and emotional shift, rescript believability) would also be associated with a better outcome.

Method

Participants

All participants had a PTSD diagnosis, experienced intrusive traumatic memories and were undertaking trauma-focused CBT treatment with an ImRs component in a routine clinical setting. Of ten participants initially recruited, one withdrew from the study before the first rescripting session. ImRs treatment was delayed for two, meaning they could not participate within the study time frame. The final sample of seven (five female, two male) had a mean age of 30.7 years (SD = 9; range = 20–45) and were from a range of ethnic backgrounds. All but one had experienced more than one traumatic event. See Table 1 for individual participant details. To protect participant anonymity, some details have been changed.

Table 1. Individual participant demographic information and symptom severity scores before and after ImRs

Measures

Within SCED research, idiographic measures of the construct of interest that are sensitive to small changes over a short time period are preferable to standardised diagnostic measures designed to detect broader clinical change at a group level over longer periods (Morley, Reference Morley2018). The set of measures described below reflects these considerations.

Frequency, distress and symptom severity

Frequency and distress of the target image were measured using two of Brewin et al.’s (Reference Brewin, Wheatley, Patel, Fearon, Hackmann and Wells2009) self-report visual analogue scales (VASs), requiring participants to rate the target image on a 0–100 scale. Frequency and distress were also combined to form a pre-/post-measure of symptom severity. This conceptualisation of PTSD symptom severity, combining frequency and distress, is consistent with the approach taken by the Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale (CAPS-5), the gold standard clinical interview for PTSD assessment (National Centre for PTSD, Reference National2016; Weathers et al., Reference Weathers, Keane and Davidson2001).

GOS coding scheme

The current version of the GOS coding scheme was adapted from that of Rose et al. (Reference Rose, Ellett, Huddy and Brown2019) and consists of six codes (logical sequencing, temporal ordering, minimisation of uncertainty, level of detail, easy of imagining, smooth flow). Each code was rated on a 3-point scale (1 = not true or mostly not true, 2 = partially true, and 3 = mostly true). Inter-rater reliability was assessed by two raters on ImRs recordings from an archival sample comparable to the current sample. Discrepancies were resolved through discussion and the coding scheme adjusted accordingly.

ImRs session content coding scheme

Each session was coded using nine codes adapted from the Salter et al. (2015) coding scheme. Based on previous experience with the coding scheme, ‘imagery activation’ for original and new imagery was coded separately, instead of a single overall code, leading to a total of ten codes. Codes are summarised in Table 2 and each was rated on a scale of 0–3. The full coding scheme can be found in the Supplementary material.

Table 2. Summary of session content codes

Design and procedure

The aim was to implement a SCED design naturalistically so as not to disrupt the course of routine treatment. Participants were enrolled at any point prior to the week before the imagery rescripting component of their therapy, and followed for the duration of the therapeutic work on a single intrusive image. All had undertaken imaginal exposure/reliving of traumatic memories prior to undertaking ImRs sessions. Data collection began 1 week prior to the first rescripting of the target image. During this session, patient and therapist collaboratively identified the target image for subsequent rescripting. No therapeutic work in relation to the image took place during this session. VAS measures were completed during this session, once during the following week and again at the start of the following session (first rescripting session), giving a total of three baseline points. Participants then commenced re-scripting sessions of the target image. While a specific ImRs protocol was not stipulated, the process generally followed those previously used by Arntz and Weertman (Reference Arntz and Weertman1999), Wheatley and Hackmann (Reference Wheatley and Hackmann2011), Smucker et al. (Reference Smucker, Dancu, Foa and Niederee1995) and Smucker and Dancu (Reference Smucker and Dancu2005). Treatment was carried out in a specialist, trauma-focused CBT service by clinical psychologists also trained in ImRs by Arnold Arntz. Having identified a target image and specified the associated cognitions and emotions, patient and therapist collaboratively agreed how the image might be rescripted before engaging in the rescripting process. A wide range of rescripting methods were employed (e.g. bringing other people into the rescript, changing the behaviour of oneself or others, altering the perpetrator or the surroundings in some way, etc.), and additional elements added iteratively as the rescript was elaborated. Rescripts varied widely in the amount of therapist direction, between the more directive, multi-stage methods of Arntz and Weertman (Reference Arntz and Weertman1999), and the more Socratic imagery method of Wheatley and Hackman (Reference Wheatley and Hackmann2011). Additonal ImRs sessions were added as needed to consolidate and enhance rescripts, or to address new cognitions and emotions that had emerged through the ImRs process.

The image distress VAS was administered at the start and end of each rescripting session. The image frequency VAS was administered only at the start of each session (as the frequency VAS asks the participant to rate ‘frequency over the past 3 days’, it did not make sense to administer this twice in an hour). The ImRs phase lasted for the number of sessions required to rescript one image, but in the interest of consistency across cases, participants were followed for a maximum of three sessions. Thus, each participant was followed for the duration of one image, up to a maximum of three sessions. Given that the aim of this study was to explore processes within ImRs, rather than assessing overall efficacy, this level of variation in session numbers was deemed acceptable. In keeping with the naturalistic design of this study, clinical judgement and patient choice were used to determine when sufficient rescripting had been completed for the target image. All sessions were recorded. When rescripting of the target image was complete (or after three ImRs sessions), participants completed the VAS measures at the beginning of the following session as a follow-up data point. In cases where this follow-up session involved no therapeutic work (e.g. discussions about therapy direction, practical tasks such as assistance with paperwork) VAS measures were collected again the following week as a second follow-up point. All sessions were coded individually using both the GOS and session content coding schemes. The session content coding scheme produced ten separate codes rated on an anchored scale between 0 and 3. The GOS coding scheme provided six codes rated between 1 and 3. Internal consistency of GOS codes has been found to be high (α > .9) in previous studies (e.g. Huddy et al., Reference Huddy, Brown, Boyd and Wykes2012; Keen et al., Reference Keen, Brown and Wheatley2008), thus an overall GOS score for each participant was obtained by summing the six individual scores. In order to create a GOS scale range of 0–12 (rather than 6–18 which resulted from each item being scored on a scale of 1–3), each total score was reduced by 6 points. GOS ratings can be considered as high (9–12), medium (5–8) or low (1–4).

Results

This section outlines associations between codes and outcomes for participants. There is debate in the literature about the use of conventional parametric statistics in SCED, which are thought to increase threats to validity (Shadish et al., Reference Shadish, Rindskopf, Hedges and Sullivan2013). Thus data were analysed using both parametric statistics and descriptive observations commonly used in SCED. Firstly, participants were divided into high- and low-responders using the reliable change index (RCI) (Jacobson and Truax, Reference Jacobson and Truax1991). Following this, links between codes and outcome were explored using three approaches: individual SCED analysis, descriptive statistics at group level and, finally, GOS and individual session content codes were correlated with symptom severity change.

Reliable change

Six cases encompassing 18 sessions were coded by two researchers using the session content coding scheme. Discrepancies greater than 1 rating point were only found in 2.1% of the ratings. Intra-class correlation between the raters was 0.81, indicating acceptable agreement (>0.7). For the GOS coding scheme, one session from three cases (50% sample, 17% of sessions) was coded by two researchers. Ratings were never discrepant by more than 1 point. The intra-class correlation (0.78) was acceptable.

Symptom severity was used to calculate reliable and clinically significant change following rescripting (Jacobson and Truax, Reference Jacobson and Truax1991). Table 1 summarises pre- and post- symptom severity scores. The RCI (RCI = (M1 – M2)/SEdiff) was calculated for the difference between pre- and post-ImRs scores. SEdiff was calculated using the test–retest reliability based on the first two baseline scores. RCI > 1.96 (+2SD), equating to a difference score of 38 in symptom severity, were considered to reflect reliable change. Pre-ImRs data was based on the average of each participant’s three baseline scores. Post-ImRs scores were based on participants’ follow-up score (or average of follow-up scores where available). Four participants (P1, P2, P4 and P5) met criteria for reliable change and are referred to as high-responders. The latter three also met criteria for clinically significant change. P3, P6 and P7 did not meet criteria for reliable or clinically significant change and are referred to as low-responders. The Leeds Reliable Change Indicator (Morley and Dowzer, Reference Morley and Dowzer2014) was used to graph the results (see Fig. 1).

Figure 1. Scatter plot of pre- and post-symptom severity scores.

Individual SCED analysis

Visual SCED analysis involves determining baseline stabilityFootnote 1 , assessing level, trend and variability of data within each phase, and comparing phases to consider whether patterns of level, trend and variability change across phases (Kratochwill et al., Reference Kratochwill, Hitchcock, Horner, Levin, Odom, Rindskopf and Shadish2010). Overlap of data between phases can also be assessed. (A detailed plan for SCED analysis, along with frequency and distress graphs and individual coding across sessions for all participants can be seen in the Supplementary material.) Patterns of level, trend and variability within and across phases for high- and low-responders are summarised below followed by a synthesis of coding patterns across sessions and how these appeared to differ for high- and low-responders.

Overall, high-responders (P1, P2, P4, P5) experienced small to moderate changes in frequency and large decreases in distress during the treatment phase. Small to medium changes tended to be stable and sustained at follow-up. While some larger changes seen during treatment were sustained, others were more variable and would often increase at follow-up but still remain below baseline levels. Low-responders experienced no change or small, but reliable, changes (P3, P7) in distress and frequency. There was one exception to this where P6 experienced large changes during treatment (possibly facilitated by high levels of therapist prompting) that returned to baseline at follow-up.

Table 3 describes apparent different configurations of codes for high- and low-responders across sessions. Higher GOS, cognitive/emotional shift and rescript believability, and lower levels of therapist guidance appeared to be associated with larger and more consistent changes in frequency and distress. Introducing change during (rather than before) the most distressing part of the trauma image/memory also appeared to be associated with better outcomes.

Table 3. Patterns of coding within and between high- and low-responders

When examined in isolation, there were no clear patterns within or between high- and low-responders for vividness of original and new imagery elements. However, when these codes were considered relative to each other, three of the four high-responders’ new imagery was rated as more vivid or equally vivid (P2) compared with the original imagery. In contrast, two of the three low-responders’ original imagery was more vivid than the new imagery (P3, P7). Original and new internal processes showed similar but clearer patterns. Larger, more stable treatment gains were experienced by those whose original processes were less intense than new processes (P1, P4, P5). Smaller/no treatment gains were experienced by those whose original, trauma-related processes were either minimal/absent (P3, P6) or very intense (P7). Furthermore, original processes for low-responders tended to be rated more intensely than new processes (P6, P7). The other element of interest to note from the SCED observations is that high-responders often progressed to optimal levels of various processes across sessions (e.g. P4 and P5 show increasing GOS).

Descriptive analysis at group level

Descriptive statistics were used to compare codes for high- and low-responders. For participants with more than one rescripting session, summary scores were computed by averaging codes across sessions (summarised in Table 4). Darker shading represents higher ratings. Participants are listed in order of symptom severity change. Mean high-responder and low-responder scores for each code are also presented in Table 4.

Table 4. Participant GOS and session content ratings across all ImRs sessions

High-responders are above the thick horizontal line and low-responders are below. Mean codes for high- and low-responders are represented at the bottom of the table. GOS coding ranges from 0 to 12 and session content codes range from 0 to 3. Darker shading represents higher levels/categories across codes. For GOS, simulation was divided into three levels: 1–4, 5–8, 9–12. *Two-tailed p < .05; **one-tailed p < .05.

All high-responders’ rescripts (P1, P2, P4, P5) were rated as well-simulated or ‘highly’ simulated, while low-responders’ rescripts (P3, P6, P7) were in the ‘medium’ range, suggesting less coherent image descriptions. In terms of session content, the clearest observed difference was for activation of new processes. All high-responders incorporated very intense emotions, cognitions and/or physical sensations within the new, rescripted imagery, while low-responders’ new processes were rated as less intense. Apparent differences can also be seen for cognitive/emotional shift, therapist guidance and rescript believability. High-responders appeared to experience higher levels of cognitive/emotional shift and rescript believability, while low-responders appeared to experience lower levels of each. For therapist guidance, low-responders appeared to require more overall guidance than high-responders.

Less pronounced differences were observed for the timing of change in ImRs and new imagery activation. All high-responders incorporated changes during the most aversive scenes of the imagery, while two of the three low-responders chose to imagine changes in the events before the most aversive scenes. High-responders’ new imagery elements also tended to be somewhat more vivid than low-responders’.

There were no pronounced patterns for remaining codes. Although less clear-cut, original imagery activation ratings showed the opposite pattern to new imagery activation, with original imagery elements rated as marginally less vivid for high-responders relative to low-responders. In terms of image departure, all participants but one incorporated some original but mostly new imagery elements. P7, who responded least to ImRs, included some new but mostly original imagery. In terms of processes relating to the original imagery, mean scores for high- and low-responders were similar. However, low-responders’ original processes appeared to be either very intense or minimally intense, while high-responders’ original processes fell more within the middle range of intensity. All participants were rated as being able to stay with the imagery throughout.

Correlation analysis at group Level

Using bootstrapping, GOS and individual session content codes were correlated with symptom severity change (Table 4). GOS showed the strongest association and was significantly correlated with symptom severity change (r = .81, p < .05). In terms of session content, two factors, therapist guidance (r = .82, p < .05) and activation of new processes (r = .69, p < .05) were significantly correlated with symptom severity change. No other session content codes were significantly correlated with symptom severity.

Discussion

This study employed a SCED design and the use of coding to investigate potential underlying factors contributing to symptom change in ImRs, with a particular focus on the role of mental simulation. In summary, goodness of simulation, therapist guidance and activation of new processes were associated with reductions in symptom severity following ImRs for all forms of analysis. In contrast, the ability to stay with the imagery and image departure showed little or no difference across high- and low-responders in any analysis. While not correlated with magnitude of change, cognitive/emotional shift and rescript believability both appeared to show different patterns for high- and low-responders in group and individual visual analysis. While less pronounced, the timing of change also appeared to differ across responder groups at the level of visual analysis.

Imagery activation and activation of processes were coded for both the original and new imagery. With the exception of new process activation, these codes showed unclear links when considered separately. However, descriptive analyses suggested that participants achieved greater symptom severity reductions when there was a balance in activation between old and new processes whereby new rescripted imagery was at least as vivid as the original imagery and contained new processes at least as strong as the original processes. Furthermore, for activation of original processes, optimal levels of activation appeared to fall in the middle ranges, rather than at the extremes. Individual SCED analysis also suggested that this balance unfolded dynamically across ImRs sessions working on the same image, gradually progressing towards optimal levels across sessions, suggesting an iterative process of refining the rescript’s content.

All forms of analysis offer support for the role of goodness of simulation in imagery rescripting. Hence, effective ImRs may rely, in part, on the quality of mental simulation of the imagined events, and the resulting coherence of the rescript. Previous findings suggest that GOS predicts subjective probability (Tversky and Kahneman, Reference Tversky and Kahneman1973) and enhances one’s ability to interpret past events and alter their emotional states (Taylor and Schneider, Reference Taylor and Schneider1989). In line with this, activation of new processes showed the highest correlation with change in symptom severity as well as the largest observed mean differences between high- and low-responders. Furthermore, while correlation with symptom severity was not significant, visual analysis suggests clear differences between high-and low-responders for cognitive/emotional shift and rescript believability. Thus, through producing a well-simulated rescript, it may be that high-responders experienced more intense new thoughts, emotions and sensations, a higher level of cognitive/emotional shift and strong levels of belief in the rescript, relative to low-responders, whose rescripts were less well-simulated.

A significant correlation was also found between symptom severity change and therapist guidance, suggesting that low-responders required more overall therapist guidance than high-responders, who tended to rescript more autonomously. However, individual analysis showed that some high-responders progressed from therapist- to self-guided rescripting across sessions. Thus, initial therapist prompting and guidance may facilitate subsequent independent rescripting and associated relief of symptoms. This may suggest that more therapist guidance in early ImRs sessions helped participants acquire skills necessary to generate well-simulated, compelling images, and in striking the balance in activation between old and new processes.

The observational findings in relation to cognitive/emotional shifts during ImRs align with previous work suggesting that generation of new mental imagery may facilitate strong changes in cognition and emotion (Ji et al., Reference Ji, Heyes, MacLeod and Holmes2016; Long and Quevillon, Reference Long and Quevillon2009) and inhibit negative arousal associated with original imagery (Rusch et al., Reference Rusch, Grunert, Mendelsohn and Smucker2000). However, it has also been suggested that, in order to be effective, ImRs must also link to the key cognitions and emotions linked with the original event (Wheatley and Hackmann, Reference Wheatley and Hackmann2011). In this respect, descriptive group analysis and individual SCED analysis suggested that those who experienced greater changes in intrusive imagery tended to activate both original and new thoughts and emotions. In general, higher levels of activation for new image processes were significantly associated with changes in symptom severity. Observational analysis showed that high-responders also produced moderate to high levels of original processes, while tending to be less intense than for new processes. Whether GOS automatically facilitates the associated thoughts/emotions was not tested. Furthermore, some suggest explicitly focusing on altering the beliefs within the traumatic image (Wilde and Clarke, Reference Wilde and Clarke2011) or facilitating a more compassionate view of self (Wheatley et al., Reference Wheatley, Brewin, Patel, Hackmann, Wells, Fisher and Myers2007) during rescripting. While the approaches used in the current study were open to exploring such processes, they were not necessarily explicitly prompted for due to the socratic nature of rescripting used.

Descriptive and observational analyses also suggested better outcomes when new imagery was as vivid or more vivid than the original imagery and when original activation fell in the middle ranges, rather than at the extremes. As a facet of GOS, higher levels of vividness of new imagery was linked to greater reductions in symptom severity. These findings fit with current PTSD theories (Brewin et al., Reference Brewin, Dalgleish and Joseph1996; Brewin et al., Reference Brewin, Gregory, Lipton and Burgess2010; Ehlers and Clark, 2010; Foa et al., Reference Foa, Steketee and Rothbaum1989; Foa and Rothbaum, Reference Foa and Rothbaum1998) that highlight the necessity of optimally activating the original traumatic memory through imaginal exposure, while at the same time remaining within a ‘window of tolerance’ (Siegel, Reference Siegel1999). Hence, if the original memory and processes are experienced too strongly, the new ImRs imagery may not be able to effectively compete with the original. Conversely, when the new ImRs imagery was maximally activated and vivid, it appeared more often to ‘win out’ in terms of symptom reduction.

Clinical implications for effective ImRs

Based on the current, preliminary findings, a possible framework to guide the clinical application of ImRs to PTSD could be as follows. Change seems best facilitated when imagery consists of some original, but mostly new imagery that coincides in time with the original traumatic event (rather than introducing change beforehand). In addition, emphasis should be placed on enhancing the quality of the mental simulation and coherence of the rescript in question. This could encompass elements such as logical and temporal sequencing, minimisation of uncertainty, and increasing detail and vividness. That is, patients should be supported to elaborate their rescripts, adding details such that they have a sense of temporal flow, with the scenario unfolding over time, a sense of logical sequencingFootnote 2 such that elements of the scenario are logically connected with each step following from the previous one and that the scenario gives a comprehensive account with minimal uncertainty about what is being described.

Focus on a well-simulated rescript that includes both original and new imagery elements may naturally facilitate access to both original and new emotions, thoughts and sensation. This, in turn, may facilitate a sense of plausibility/belief in the rescript as well as a strong shift in cognition/emotion. Ensuring that only some of the original imagery is included may prevent high levels of vividness of original image elements and intense original processes from overwhelming people such that they come out of the imagery or fail to experience a shift in cognition/affect.

In terms of therapist guidance, independent rescripting seems ultimately beneficial, although initial therapist prompting and guidance may be necessary to promote independent rescripting, and this could be viewed as an iterative process of skill acquisition. Thus, in the early stages, therapists may provide explicit guidance to develop rescripts to include those elements described here, reminding clients of omitted elements. Over time, prompts and suggestions should be replaced with Socratic questions as appropriate to help patients to develop their skills in independently using rescripting. The role of the therapist may therefore be viewed as supporting the patient to learn how to become a director of compelling ‘mental movies’ that effectively ‘outsell’ the original distressing intrusive imagery. As part of socialisation to the technique, the therapist can explore the kinds of film their patient likes, and what they think makes a great film – storytelling, dialogue, continuity and timing, emotionality, sound effects, cinematic visuals, memorable endings. This frame may offer an accessible way of understanding how to enhance goodness of simulation and refine the skills of rescripting, as well help to identify the particular kinds of rescripts – whether dramatic, comedic or fantastic – that may work best for each individual.

Finally, previous studies have shown high drop-out rates for traditional exposure therapy (Najavits, Reference Najavits2015). One potential contributing factor for this is the difficulty for patients of re-living the original traumatic imagery. Based on the current findings, including a small amount of original imagery that, while vivid, is less vivid than the subsequent rescripted imagery, may support people to connect with and tolerate this process more effectively.

Strengths and limitations

This is the first study to consider ImRs within the GOS framework. The use of SCED and coding to uncover processes at work in ImRs is a further strength. A broad range of factors were encompassed within this study and results highlight those factors that are most likely to be involved in producing clinical change, thus providing important direction for future research. Furthermore, the naturalistic design and setting, and the heterogeneity of the sample increase the external validity of findings.

While the small sample size is a condition for SCED and therefore not a limitation in and of itself, it does reduce the applicability of statistical analyses. Thus formal statistical analysis was supported by descriptive, observational analysis in the current study. It should be acknowledged that SCED studies typically balance small sample sizes with more intensive, repeated measurement and, where possible, multiple phase changes. Due to the nature of the current intervention and endeavours to maintain a naturalistic design without disrupting routine treatment, pursuing such a strategy was precluded. A further limitation is that coding was based on session recordings. However, it is likely that further practice between sessions may have enhanced factors such as simulation. Monitoring between-session practice and recording a final version of participant rescripts at follow-up for coding purposes would have been beneficial. Finally, while external validity was a strength of this study, with the open approach to ImRs allowing for coding of a broad range of rescripting techniques, this approach makes it difficult to speak to the strengths and weaknesses of one rescripting protocol over another. Rather, this study provides preliminary findings for the role of general ImRs elements (e.g. GOS). Future studies may benefit from looking at mechanisms of change within specific ImRs protocols.

Conclusions

This study provides preliminary, yet promising, support for the role of goodness of simulation in understanding ImRs efficacy. It also provides some evidence of links between conceptual ‘top-down’ processes and observable ‘bottom-up’ ImRs factors such as the activation of new processes, cognitive/emotional shift and believability in the rescript. Thus, GOS may provide a useful framework to guide clinicians when planning and implementing ImRs therapy with clients. It may also provide a useful metaphor to explaining the rescripting process to clients. The field would benefit from further research into the hypothetical links between GOS and session content codes, to identify whether these contribute independently to outcome, whether factors such as emotional/cognitive processes and rescript believability are dependent on GOS, or vice versa. This may contribute to developing a data-driven model of the cognitive processes underlying ImRs, as well as novel directions for basic process research into imagery.

Supplementary material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit: https://doi.org/10.1017/S1352465820000806

Acknowledgements

We would like to acknowledge the invaluable advice and contributions of Dr Hannah Murray and Dr Pippa Stallworthy, two of the treating clinicians in this research.

Financial support

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial or no-for-profit sectors.

Conflicts of interest

Dr Kathy Looney, Dr Sharif El-Leithy and Dr Gary Brown have no conflicts of interest with respect to this publication.

Ethical statements

This research adheres to the ethical principles of Psychologists and is in line with the Code of Conduct as set out by the BABCP and BPS. Ethical approval was sought through the NHS National Research Ethics Service, with a REC reference 13/NW/0432 and IRAS Project ID 124012. The research was subsequently approved by local R&D Departments.

Footnotes

1 All baseline data met stability criteria except for that of P2 (distress and frequency), which showed stable or increasing trend for the first two baseline points followed by a significant decrease for baseline point 3. P2 reported that she spontaneously started to rescript the image independently after completing the second baseline measure, potentially explaining this variability.

2 Note that ‘logical’ in this context does not pertain to events that are logically possible within the real world. Many unrealistic events may be logical within the narrative of the story within the rescript (e.g. use of magical powers or appearance of imaginary/dead people).

References

Arntz, A. (2012). Imagery rescripting as a therapeutic technique: Review of clinical trials, basic studies and research agenda. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 3, 189208. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.5127/jep.024211 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arntz, A. (2015). Imagery rescripting for posttraumatic stress disorder. In Thoma, T. C. & McKay, D. (eds), Working with Emotion in Cognitive-Behavioural Therapy (pp. 203216). New York, USA: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Arntz, A., Tiesema, M., & Kindt, M. (2007). Treatment of PTSD: a comparison of imagninal exposure with and without imagery rescripting. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 38, 345370. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2007.10.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arntz, A., & Weertman, A. (1999). Treatment of childhood memories: theory and practice. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37, 715740. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(98)00173-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brewin, C. (2006). Understanding cognitive behaviour therapy: a retrieval competition account. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 765784. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2006.02.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brewin, C., Dalgleish, T. & Joseph, S. (1996). A dual representation theory of posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychological Review, 103, 670686. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.103.4.670 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brewin, C., Gregory, J., Lipton, M. & Burgess, N. (2010). Intrusive images in psychological disorders: characteristics, neural mechanisms and treatment implications. Psychological Review, 177, 210232. doi: 10.1037/a0018113 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brewin, C., Wheatley, J., Patel, T., Fearon, P., Hackmann, A., Wells, A., … & Myers, S. (2009). Imagery rescripting as a brief stand-alone treatment for depressed patients with intrusive memories. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47, 569576. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2009.03.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, G., MacLeod, A., Tata, P., & Goddard, L. (2002). Worry and the simulation of future outcomes. Anxiety, Stress and Coping, 15, 117. doi: https://doi.org/10.1080/10615800290007254 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ehlers, A., & Clark, D. (2000). A cognitive model of posttraumatic stress disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38, 319345. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(99)00123-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ehlers, A., Clark, D., Hackmann, A., McManus, F., & Fennell, M. (2005). Cognitive therapy for post-traumatic stress disorder: development and evaluation. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43, 413431. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(99)00123-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foa, E., Steketee, G., & Rothbaum, B. (1989). Behavioral/cognitive conceptualizations of post-traumatic stress disorder. Behavior Therapy, 20, 155176. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7894(89)80067-X CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foa, E., & Rothbaum, B. (1998). Treating the Trauma of Rape: Cognitive Behavioural Therapy for PTSD. New York, USA: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Grunert, B., Weis, J., Smucker, M. & Christianson, H. (2007). Imagery rescripting and reprocessing therapy after failed prolonged exposure for post-traumatic stress disorder following industrial injury. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 38, 317328. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2007.10.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huddy, V., Brown, G., Boyd, T., & Wykes, T. (2012). An exploratory investigation of real-world reasoning in paranoia. Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 87, 4459. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8341.2012.02072.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jacobson, N., & Truax, P. (1991). Clinical significance: a statistical approach to defining meaningful change in psychotherapy research. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 59, 1219. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/10109-042 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ji, J., Heyes, S., MacLeod, C., & Holmes, E. (2016). Emotional mental imagery as simulation of reality: fear and beyond – a tribute to Peter Lang. Behavior Therapy, 47. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2015.11.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1982). The simulation heuristic. In Kahneman, D., Slovik, P. and Tversky, A. (eds), Judgement Under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases, pp. 201208. New York, USA: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kazdin, A. (2007). Mediators and mechanisms of change in psychotherapy research. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 3, 127. doi: https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091432 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kazdin, A. (1978). Methodological and interpretive problems of single-case experimental designs. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 46, 629642. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.46.4.629 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keen, N., Brown, G., & Wheatley, J. (2008). Obsessive compulsive symptoms and the simulation of future negative events. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 47, 265279. doi: 10.1348/014466508X282833 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kratochwill, T., Hitchcock, J., Horner, R., Levin, R., Odom, S., Rindskopf, D., & Shadish, W. (2010). Single-case design technical documentation. Retrieved from What Works Clearinghouse website: http://ies.ed.gov/ncee/wwc/pdf/wwc_scd.pdf Google Scholar
Long, M., & Quevillon, R. (2009). Imagery rescripting in the treatment of posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy: An International Quarterly, 23, 6776. doi: 10.1891/0889-8391.23.1.67 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morley, S. (2018). Single Case Methods in Clinical Psychology: A Practical Guide. New York, USA: Routledge.Google Scholar
Morley, S., & Dowzer, C. (2014). Manual for the Leeds Reliable Change Indicator: Simple Excel® applications for the analysis of individual patient and group data. Leeds, UK: University of Leeds. http://medhealth.leeds.ac.uk/info/618/clinical_psychology_dclinpsychol/797/leeds_reliable_change_index Google Scholar
Najavits, L. (2015). The problem of drop-out from ‘gold standard’ PTSD therapies. F1000 Prime Reports, 7, 43. doi: 10.12703/P7-43 Google Scholar
National, Centre for PTSD (2016). Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale for DSM-5 (CAPS-5). Retrieved from: http://www.ptsd.va.gov/professional/assessment/adult-int/caps.asp Google Scholar
Normand, M. (2016). Less is more: psychologists can learn more by studying fewer people. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 934. doi: https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00934 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raune, D., MacLeod, A., & Holmes, E. (2005). The simulation heuristic and visual imagery in pessimism for future negative events in anxiety. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 12, 313325. doi: 10.1002/cpp.455 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rose, M., Ellett, L., Huddy, V., & Brown, G. (2019). The simulation heuristic, paranoia, and social anxiety in a non-clinical sample. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 62, 1521. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2018.08.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rusch, M., Grunert, B., Mendelsohn, R. & Smucker, M. (2000). Imagery rescripting for recurrent, distressing images. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 7, 173182. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1077-7229(00)80028-0 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salter, C., Parker, E., Brown, G., El Leithy, S., Wheatly, J. (2015). Imagery Rescripting Coding Framwork and Manual. doi: 10.13140/RG.2.1.4420.5282 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shadish, W., Rindskopf, D., Hedges, L., & Sullivan, K. (2013). Bayesian estimates of autocorrelations in single-case designs. Behavior Research Methods, 45, 813821. doi: 10.3758/s13428-012-0282-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sidman, M. (1960). Tactics of Scientific Research. New York, USA: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Siegel, D. (1999). The Developing Mind: How Relationships and the Brain Interact to Shape Who We Are. New York, USA: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Smith, P., & Little, R. (2018). Small is beautiful: in defence of the small-N design. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 25, 20832101. doi: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-018-1451-8 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smucker, M. R., & Dancu, C. (2005). Cognitive-Behavioural Treatment for Adult Survivors of Childhood Trauma: Imagery Rescripting and Reprocessing. Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield.Google Scholar
Smucker, M., Dancu, C., Foa, E., & Niederee, J. (1995). Imagery rescripting: a new treatment for survivors of childhood sexual abuse suffering from posttraumatic stress. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 9, 317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smucker, M. R., & Niederee, J. (1995). Treating incest-related PTSD and pathogenic schemas through imaginal exposure and rescripting. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 2, 6393. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1077-7229(05)80005-7 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, S., & Schneider, S. (1989). Coping and the simulation of events. Social Cognition, 7, 174194. doi: 10.1521/soco.1989.7.2.174 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turpin, G. (2001). Single case methodology and psychotherapy evaluation. From research to practice. In Mace, C., Moorey, S., & Roberts, B. (eds), Evidence in the Psychological Therapies: A Critical Guidance for Practitioners (pp. 89110). East Sussex, UK: Brunner-Routledge.Google Scholar
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1973). Availability: a heuristic for judging frequency and probability. Cognitive Psychology, 5, 207232. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(73)90033-9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weathers, F., Keane, T., & Davidson, J. (2001). Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale: a review of the first ten years of research. Depression and Anxiety, 13, 132156. doi: 10.1002/da.1029 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wheatley, J., Brewin, C., Patel, T., Hackmann, E., Wells, A., Fisher, P., & Myers, S. (2007). I’ll believe it when I can see it: imagery rescripting of intrusive sensory memories in depression. Journal of Behavior Therapy and experimental Psychiatry, 38, 371385.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wheatley, J., & Hackmann, A. (2011). Using imagery rescripting to treat major depression: theory and practice. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 18, 444453. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cbpra.2010.06.004 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilde, J., & Clarke, D. (2011). Imagery rescripting of early traumatic memories in social phobia. Cognitive and Behavioural Practice, 18, 433443. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cbpra.2011.03.002 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Individual participant demographic information and symptom severity scores before and after ImRs

Figure 1

Table 2. Summary of session content codes

Figure 2

Figure 1. Scatter plot of pre- and post-symptom severity scores.

Figure 3

Table 3. Patterns of coding within and between high- and low-responders

Figure 4

Table 4. Participant GOS and session content ratings across all ImRs sessions

Supplementary material: File

Looney et al. supplementary material

Looney et al. supplementary material

Download Looney et al. supplementary material(File)
File 178.4 KB
Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.