Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-hc48f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T02:27:23.425Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Power of the eyes: deterring sexual harassment in Tokyo subways using images of watchful eyes

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 March 2020

KIKI K. CHU*
Affiliation:
London School of Economics and Political Science, London, UK
*
*Correspondence to: London School of Economics and Political Science, Houghton St, Holborn, LondonWC2A 2AE, UK. Email: k.chu@alumni.lse.ac.uk

Abstract

Sex crime in subways poses an ever-present threat to Japanese commuters. This paper proposes a simple and viable adjunct to the current tactics by harnessing the potential of the watching eyes effect to deter sexual harassment. It attempts to observe the human biological sensitivity to gaze and to explore its application to the specific Japanese context where powerful informal control exists. The paper also identifies the possible advantages and limitations of such measure, with the goal of stimulating more discussion over evidence-based behavioural interventions in deterring crimes. A tentative proposal is composed to summarize and demonstrate a number of behaviourally informed suggestions regarding the practical implementation and configuration of the proposed measure.

Type
New Voices
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Baltazar, M., Hazem, N., Vilarem, E. and Beaucousin, V. (2014), ‘Eye contact elicits bodily self-awareness in human adults’, Cognition, 133(1): 120127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S. (1995), Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bateson, M., Callow, L., Holmes, J. R., Redmond Roche, M. L. and Nettle, D. (2013), ‘Do images of ‘watching eyes’ induce behaviour that is more pro-social or more normative? A field experiment on littering’, PLoS ONE, 8(12): e82055.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bateson, M., Nettle, D. and Roberts, G. (2006), ‘Cues of being watched enhance cooperation in a real-world setting’, Biology Letters, 2: 412414.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Behavioural Insights Team (2014), EAST: Four simple ways to apply behavioural insight. Retrieved from: https://www.bi.team/wp-content/uploads/2015/07/BIT-Publication-EAST_FA_WEB.pdfGoogle Scholar
Bramel, D. and Friend, R. (1981), ‘Hawthorne, the myth of the docile worker, and class bias in psychology’, American Psychologist, 36(8): 867e78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, J., Maxey, V. and Watson, W. (1995), ‘Hawthorne effect: Implications for pre-hospital research’, Annals of Emergency Medicine, 26(5): 590594.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chiesa, M. and Hobbs, S. (2008), ‘Making sense of social research: How useful is the Hawthorne effect?’, European Journal of Social Psychology, 38(1): 67e74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chudek, M. and Henrich, J. (2011), ‘Culture–gene coevolution, norm-psychology and the emergence of human prosociality’, Trends in cognitive sciences, 15(5): 218226.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cialdini, R. B., Reno, R. R. and Kallgren, C. A. (1990), ‘A focus theory of normative conduct: Recycling the concept of norms to reduce littering in public places’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58(6): 10151026.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clarke, R. V. (2012), ‘Opportunity makes the thief. Really? And so what?’, Crime Science, 1(3).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conty, L., George, N. and Hietanen, J. K. (2012), ‘Watching Eyes effects: When others meet the self’, Consciousness and Cognition, 45(2016) 184197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ernest-Jones, M., Nettle, D. and Bateson, M. (2011), ‘Effects of eye images on everyday cooperative behaviour: a field experiment’, Evolution and Human Behaviour, 32: 172178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Francey, D., Bergmüller, R. (2012), ‘Images of eyes enhance investments in a real-life public good’, PLoS One, 7, e37397CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gabor, T. (1990), ‘Crime displacement and situational prevention - toward the development of some principles’, Canadian Journal of Criminology-Revue Canadienne De Criminologie, 32: 4173.Google Scholar
Guerette, R. T. & Bowers, K. J. (2009), ‘Assessing the extent of crime displacement and diffusion of benefits: a review of situational crime prevention evaluations’, Criminology, 47(4): 13311368.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haley, K. J. and Fessler, D. M. T. (2005), ‘Nobody's watching? Subtle cues affect generosity in an anonymous economic game’, Evolution and Human Behavior, 26: 245256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halpern, D. (2015), Inside the Nudge unit, London: WH Allen.Google Scholar
Horii, M. and Burgess, A. (2012), ‘Constructing sexual risk: ‘Chikan’, collapsing male authority and the emergence of women-only train carriages in Japan’, Health, Risk & Society, 14(1): 4155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iida, Y., (2005), ‘Beyond the ‘feminization of masculinity’: Transforming patriarchy with the ‘feminine’ contemporary Japanese youth culture’, Inter-Asia Cultural Studies, 6(1): 5674.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Izuma, K. (2012), ‘The social neuroscience of reputation’, Neuroscience Research, 72: 283288.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kahneman, D. (2011), Thinking, fast and slow, New York, NY: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Keller, J. and Pfattheicher, S. (2011), ‘Vigilant self-regulation, cues of being watched and cooperativeness’, European Journal of Personality, 25: 363372.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirchhoff, C. F., Joseph, M. G. C. and Barkhuizen, J. (2007), ‘The Asian passengers’ safety study of sexual molestation on train and buses: The Indonesian study’, Acta Criminologica, 20(4): 113.Google Scholar
Kobayashi, H. and Kohshima, S. (1997), ‘Unique morphology of the human eye’, Nature, 387(6635): 767768.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Komiya, N. (1999), ‘A cultural study of the low crime rate in Japan’, British Journal of Criminology, 39(3) 369390.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langton, R. H., Watt, R. J. and Bruce, V. (2000), ‘Do the eyes have it? Cues to the direction of social attention’, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(2): 5059.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leonardsen, D. (2003), Crime in Japan: Paradise Lost?, Palgrave Macmillan UK.Google Scholar
Lewis, A. Z. (2016), Can images of “watching eyes” kick-start donations to crowdfunding campaigns? Senior Project in Cognitive Science, Yale University. Retrieved from https://cogsci.yale.edu/sites/default/files/files/Thesis2016Lewis.pdfGoogle Scholar
Lewis, K. (2010) ‘Yellowtown: Urban signage, class, and race’, Design and Culture, 2(2): 183198. DOI: 10.2752/175470710X12696138525668.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Macrae, C., Hood, B. M., Milne, A. B., Rowe, A. C. and Mason, M. F. (2002), ‘Are you looking at me? eye gaze and person perception’, Psychological Science, 13: 460464.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mareschal, I. W., Clifford, C.W.G.J. and Calder, A. J. (2013), ‘Humans have an expectation that gaze is directed toward them’, Current Biology, 23(8): 717721.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNees, M. P., Egli, D. S., Marshall, R. S., Schnelle, J. F. and Risley, T. R. (1976), ‘Shoplifting prevention: Providing information through signs’, Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 9: 399405.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nettle, D., Nott, K. and Bateson, M. (2012), ‘‘Cycle Thieves, We Are Watching You’: Impact of a Simple Signage Intervention against Bicycle Theft’, PLoS ONE, 7(12): e51738.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nettle, D., Harper, Z., Kidson, A., Stone, R., Penton-Voak, I. and Bateson, M. (2013), ‘The watching eyes effect in the Dictator Game: it's not how much you give, it's being seen to give something’, Evolution and Human Behavior, 34(1): 3540.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Panagopoulos, C. and van der Linden, S. (2017), “‘The Feeling of Being Watched: Do Eye Cues Elicit Negative Affect?”. North American Journal of Psychology, 19(1): 113.Google Scholar
Pfattheicher, S. & Keller, J. (2015), ‘The watching eyes phenomenon: The role of a sense of being seen and public self-awareness’, European Journal of Social Psychology, 45(5): 560566.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Powell, K. L., Roberts, G. and Nettle, D. (2012), ‘Eye images increase charitable donations: Evidence from an opportunistic field experiment in a supermarket’, Ethology, 118: 10961101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sadr, J., Jarudi, I. and Sinha, P. (2003), ‘The role of eyebrows in face recognition’, Perception, 32(3): 285293.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saito, A. K. (2017), The reasons why men became Chikan, Tokyo: East Press.Google Scholar
Shukan Josei PRIME. (2016), 「安心して立ち上がって」痴漢抑止バッジに込められた思い[Online]. Retrieved from https://www.jprime.jp/articles/-/7598Google Scholar
Smith, M. J., Clarke, R. V. and Pease, K. (2002), Anticipatory Benefit in Crime Prevention in Tilley, N. (ed.), Analysis for Crime Prevention: Crime Prevention Studies 13, Criminal Justice Press.Google Scholar
Sparks, A. and Barclay, P. (2013) ‘Eye images increase generosity, but not for long: the limited effect of a false cue’, Evolution and Human Behavior, 34: 317322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tsuji, Y. and Shimada, S. (2015), ‘Socially anxious tendencies affect autonomic responses during eye gaze perception’, Psychology, 6: 16461652.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whalen, P. J., Kagan, J., Cook, R. G., Davis, F. C., Kim, H., Polis, S., et al. . (2004), ‘Human amygdala responsivity to masked fearful eye whites’, Science, 306(5704): 2061.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wykowska, A., Schubö, A. and Hommel, B. (2009), ‘How you move is what you see: action planning biases selection in visual search’, J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform, 35: 17551769.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed