Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-lnqnp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-20T04:39:10.295Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Gender matters: From L1 grammar to L2 semantics*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 September 2016

SVETLANA V. COOK*
Affiliation:
University of Maryland
*
Address for Correspondence: Svetlana V. Cook, University of Maryland, National Foreign Language Center, 5245 Greenbelt Rd, Severn Building 810, College Park, MD 20742, USAsvcook@umd.edu

Abstract

The study investigates the effects of grammatical gender on bilingual processing. Native speakers of Russian (a gendered language) learning English and monolingual English controls performed a self-paced reading task in English (a non-gendered language). As predicted, bilingual speakers showed delayed latencies to gendered pronouns (he or she) that were incongruent with the noun's grammatical gender in Russian, indicating that first language (L1) grammatical gender assignment can be interpreted as biological gender in nonnative (L2) processing. The L1 gender bias was only found in sentences containing animate, but not inanimate, nouns. These results speak against the syntactic mechanism being solely responsible for gender biases, but rather support a semantic transfer account due to coactivation of linguistic and conceptual features as proposed in the sex and gender hypothesis (SAGH, Vigliocco, Vinson, Paganelli & Dworzynski, 2005). Overall, the study provides clear evidence for the L1 grammatical gender bias in bilingual processing, albeit constrained by animacy.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2016 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

The author is deeply indebted to Nan Jiang, Kira Gor, and Robert DeKeyser for guidance at various stages of the study, Jonathan Cook for inspiration for the study, Carrie Bonilla for her comments on an earlier version of the manuscript, and to three anonymous reviewers for their insightful suggestions and comments. The remaining mistakes are my own. The study was supported in part by the School of Languages, Literatures and Cultures, and the College of Arts and Humanities at the University of Maryland.

References

Alarcón, I. (2009). The processing of gender agreement in L1 and L2 Spanish: Evidence from reaction time data. Hispania, 814–828.Google Scholar
Alarcón, I. V. (2010). Gender assignment and agreement in L2 Spanish: The effects of morphological marking, animacy, and gender. Studies in Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistics, 3 (2), 267300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andonova, E., D'Amico, S., Devescovi, A., & Bates, E. (2004). Gender and lexical access in Bulgarian. Perception & psychophysics, 66 (3), 496507.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Antón-Méndez, M. I. (1999). Gender and number agreement processing in Spanish. Unpublished PhD dissertation, University of Arizona. Google Scholar
Aronoff, M. (1994). Morphology by itself: Stems and inflectional classes (No. 22). MIT Press. Google Scholar
Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of memory and language, 59 (4), 390412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of memory and language, 68 (3), 255278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bassetti, B. (2007). Bilingualism and thought: Grammatical gender and concepts of objects in Italian–German bilingual children. International Journal of Bilingualism, 11 (3), 251273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bassetti, B., & Nicoladis, E. (2015). Research on grammatical gender and thought in early and emergent bilinguals. International Journal of Bilingualism, 1367006915576824.Google Scholar
Bates, D., Maechler, M., & Bolker, B. (2012). lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes.Google Scholar
Belacchi, C., & Cubelli, R. (2012). Implicit knowledge of grammatical gender in preschool children. Journal of psycholinguistic research, 41 (4), 295310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bender, A., Beller, S., & Klauer, K. C. (2011). Grammatical gender in German: A case for linguistic relativity? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64 (9), 18211835.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boroditsky, L., & Schmidt, L. (2000). Sex, syntax and semantics. Proceedings of the Twenty-Second Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society. Philadelphia, PA.Google Scholar
Boroditsky, L., Schmidt, L., & Phillips, W. (2003). Sex, Syntax, and Semantics. In Gentner & Goldin-Meadow (Eds.), Language in Mind: Advances in the study of Language and Cognition, 6179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boutonnet, B., Athanasopoulos, P., & Thierry, G. (2012). Unconscious effects of grammatical gender during object categorisation. Brain research, 1479, 7279.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brysbaert, M. (1998). Word recognition in bilinguals: Evidence against the existence of two separate lexicons. Psychologica Belgica, 38 (3–4), 163175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H., & Felser, C. (2006). Continuity and shallow structures in language processing. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27 (01), 107126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, H. H. (1973). The language-as-fixed-effect fallacy: A critique of language statistics in psychological research. Journal of verbal learning and verbal behavior, 12 (4), 335359.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Comrie, B. (1999). Grammatical gender systems: a linguist's assessment. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 28 (5), 457466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corbett, G. G., & Fraser, N. M. (2000). Gender assignment: a typology and a model. Systems of nominal classification, 4, 293325.Google Scholar
Corbett, G. G. (1991). Gender. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corbett, G. G. (1982). Gender in Russian: An account of gender specification and its relationship to declension. Russian Linguistics, 6, 197232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cornips, L., & Hulk, A. (2008). Factors of success and failure in the acquisition of grammatical gender in Dutch. Second Language Research, 24 (3), 267295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cornips, L., van der Hoek, M., & Verwer, R. (2006). The acquisition of grammatical gender in bilingual child acquisition of Dutch (by older Moroccan and Turkish children). Linguistics in the Netherlands, 40, 5.Google Scholar
Coughlin, C. E., & Tremblay, A. (2013). Proficiency and working memory based explanations for nonnative speakers’ sensitivity to agreement in sentence processing. Applied Psycholinguistics, 34 (03), 615646.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cubelli, R., Lotto, L., Paolieri, D., Girelli, M., & Job, R. (2005). Grammatical gender is selected in bare noun production: Evidence from the picture–word interference paradigm. Journal of Memory and Language, 53 (1), 4259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cubelli, R., Paolieri, D., Lotto, L., & Job, R. (2011). The effect of grammatical gender on object categorization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37 (2), 449.Google ScholarPubMed
Desrochers, A., & Brabant, M. (1995). Interaction entre facteurs phonologiques et sémantiques dans une épreuve de catégorisation lexicale. Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Expérimentale, 49, 240262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deutsch, A., & Bentin, S. (2001). Syntactic and semantic factors in processing gender agreement in Hebrew: Evidence from ERPs and eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language, 45 (2), 200224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deutsch, A., Bentin, S., & Katz, L. (1999). Semantic influence on processing gender agreement: Evidence from Hebrew. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 28 (5), 515535.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dijkstra, T., & Van Heuven, W. J. (1998). The BIA model and bilingual word recognition. Localist connectionist approaches to human cognition, 189225.Google Scholar
Dijkstra, T., Van Heuven, W. J., & Grainger, J. (1998). Simulating cross-language competition with the bilingual interactive activation model. Psychologica Belgica, 38 (3–4), 177196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dijkstra, T., & Van Heuven, W. J. (2002). The architecture of the bilingual word recognition system: From identification to decision. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 5 (03), 175197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dilkina, K., McClelland, J. L., & Boroditsky, L. (2007). How language affects thought in a connectionist model. In Proceedings of the 29th Annual Cognitive Science Society Conference (p. 215).Google Scholar
Dixon, P. (2008). Models of accuracy in repeated-measures designs. Journal of Memory and Language, 59 (4), 447456.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dussias, P. E., Kroff, J. R. V., Tamargo, R. E. G., & Gerfen, C. (2013). When gender and looking go hand in hand. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 35 (02), 353387.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eberhard, K. M., Scheutz, M., & Heilman, M. (2005). An empirical and computational test of linguistic relativity. In Proceedings of the 27th annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 618–623).Google Scholar
Faraway, J. J. (2005). Extending the linear model with R: generalized linear, mixed effects and nonparametric regression models. CRC press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forster, K. I., & Forster, J. C. (2003). DMDX: A Windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 35 (1), 116124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foucart, A., & Frenck-Mestre, C. (2012). Can late L2 learners acquire new grammatical features? Evidence from ERPs and eye-tracking. Journal of Memory and Language, 66 (1), 226248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friederici, A. D., Rueschemeyer, S. A., Hahne, A., & Fiebach, C. J. (2003). The role of left inferior frontal and superior temporal cortex in sentence comprehension: localizing syntactic and semantic processes. Cerebral cortex. 13 (2), 170177.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grainger, J., & Dijkstra, T. (1992). On the representation and use of language information in bilinguals. Advances in psychology, 83, 207220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grüter, T., Lew-Williams, C., & Fernald, A. (2012). Grammatical gender in L2: A production or a real-time processing problem? Second Language Research, 28 (2), 191215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hagoort, P. (2003). Interplay between syntax and semantics during sentence comprehension: ERP effects of combining syntactic and semantic violations. Cognitive Neuroscience, Journal of, 15 (6), 883899.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jaeger, T. F. (2008). Categorical data analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards logit mixed models. Journal of memory and language, 59 (4), 434446.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jiang, N. (2004). Morphological insensitivity in second language processing. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 603634.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jiang, N. (2007). Selective integration of linguistic knowledge in adult second language learning. Language learning, 57 (1), 133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jiang, N., Hu, G., Chrabaszcz, A., & Ye, L. (2015). The activation of grammaticalized meaning in L2 processing: Toward an explanation of the morphological congruency effect. International Journal of Bilingualism, 1367006915603823.Google Scholar
Juffs, A., & Harrington, M. (1995). Parsing effects in second language sentence processing. Studies in second language acquisition, 17 (4), 483516.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kazanina, N., Lau, E. F., Lieberman, M., Yoshida, M., & Phillips, C. (2007). The effect of syntactic constraints on the processing of backwards anaphora. Journal of Memory and Language, 56 (3), 384409.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keating, G. D. (2009). Sensitivity to Violations of Gender Agreement in Native and Nonnative Spanish: An Eye-Movement Investigation. Language Learning, 59 (3), 503535.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keating, G. D., & Jegerski, J. (2014). Experimental designs in sentence processing research. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 132.Google Scholar
Konishi, T. (1993). The semantics of grammatical gender: A cross-cultural study. Journal of psycholinguistic research, 22 (5), 519534.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kousta, S. T., Vinson, D. P., & Vigliocco, G. (2008). Investigating linguistic relativity through bilingualism: The case of grammatical gender. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34 (4), 843.Google ScholarPubMed
Kroll, J. F., Van Hell, J. G., Tokowicz, N., & Green, D. W. (2010). The Revised Hierarchical Model: A critical review and assessment. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13 (03), 373381.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kurinski, E., & Sera, M. D. (2011). Does learning Spanish grammatical gender change English-speaking adults' categorization of inanimate objects? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14 (02), 203220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, Bruun, P., & Haubo Bojesen Christensen, R. (2013). lmerTest: Tests for Random and Fixed Effects for Linear Mixed Effect Modles (lmer Objects of lmer4 Package). R package version 2.0-0. Available online at: http://cran.r-prject.org/package=lmerTest Google Scholar
Lardiere, D. (2000). Mapping features to forms in second language acquisition. Second language acquisition and linguistic theory, 102–129.Google Scholar
Levelt, W.J.M. (1989). Speaking: From intention to articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Lupyan, G. (2012). Linguistically modulated perception and cognition: the label-feedback hypothesis. Frontiers in psychology, 3 (54).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marcoux, D. R. (1973). Deviation in English gender. American Speech, 48 (1/2), 98107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marian, V., Spivey, M., & Hirsch, J. (2003). Shared and separate systems in bilingual language processing: Converging evidence from eyetracking and brain imaging. Brain and language, 86 (1), 7082.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martinez, I. M., & Shatz, M. (1996). Linguistic influences on categorization in preschool children: A crosslinguistic study. Journal of Child Language, 23 (3), 529545.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morales, L., Paolieri, D., Cubelli, R., & Bajo, M. T. (2014). Transfer of Spanish grammatical gender to English: Evidence from immersed and non-immersed bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paolieri, D., Lotto, L., Leoncini, D., Cubelli, R., & Job, R. (2011). Differential effects of grammatical gender and gender inflection in bare noun production. British Journal of Psychology, 102 (1), 1936.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Payne, J., & Huddleston, R. D. (2002). Noun and noun phrases. In Huddleston, R. D. and Pullum, G. K., Cambridge Grammar of the English Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Pearlmutter, N. J., Garnsey, S. M., & Bock, K. (1999). Agreement processes in sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory and language, 41 (3), 427456.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pérez-Pereira, M. (1991). The acquisition of gender: What Spanish children tell us. Journal of Child Language, 18 (03), 571590.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phillips, W., & Boroditsky, L. (2003). Can quirks of grammar affect the way you think? Grammatical gender and object concepts. Proceedings of the 25th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society. Boston, MA.Google Scholar
Pienemann, M. (1998). Language processing and second language development: Processability theory (Vol. 15). John Benjamins Publishing.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
R Core Team (2012). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R foundation for Statistical Computing.Google Scholar
Rayner, K., & Duffy, S. A. (1986). Lexical complexity and fixation times in reading: Effects of word frequency, verb complexity, and lexical ambiguity. Memory & Cognition, 14 (3), 191201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sabourin, L. L. (2003). Grammatical gender and second language processing: An ERP study. Unpublished PhD dissertation, Rijksuniversiteit Groningen.Google Scholar
Sabourin, L., & Stowe, L. A. (2008). Second language processing: when are first and second languages processed similarly? Second Language Research, 24 (3), 397430.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sabourin, L., Stowe, L. A., & De Haan, G. J. (2006). Transfer effects in learning a second language grammatical gender system. Second Language Research, 22 (1), 129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sagarra, N., & Herschensohn, J. (2011). Proficiency and animacy effects on L2 gender agreement processes during comprehension. Language Learning, 61 (1), 80116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sera, M. D., Elieff, C., Forbes, J., Burch, M. C., Rodriguez, W., & Dubois, D. P. (2002). When language affects cognition and when it does not: an analysis of grammatical gender and classification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131 (3), 377.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sera, M. D., Berge, C. A., & Pintado, J. D. C. (1994). Grammatical and conceptual forces in the attribution of gender by English and Spanish speakers. Cognitive Development, 9 (3), 261292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sharkey, N. E., & Sharkey, A. J. (1987). What is the point of integration? The loci of knowledge-based facilitation in sentence processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 26 (3), 255276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slobin, D. I. (1997). Mind, code, and text. In Bybee, J., Haiman, J. & Thompson, S. A. (eds.), Essays on Language Function and Language Type. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company. 437467 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skeide, M. A., Brauer, J., & Friederici, A. D. (2014). Syntax gradually segregates from semantics in the developing brain. NeuroImage, 100, 106111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sorace, A., & Filiaci, F. (2006). Anaphora resolution in near-native speakers of Italian. Second Language Research, 22 (3), 339368.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spinner, P., & Thomas, J. A. (2014). L2 learners' sensitivity to semantic and morphophonological information on Swahili nouns. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 52 (3), 283311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spivey, M. J., & Marian, V. (1999). Cross talk between native and second languages: Partial activation of an irrelevant lexicon. Psychological science, 10 (3), 281284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tokowicz, N., & MacWhinney, B. (2005). Implicit and explicit measures of sensitivity to violations in second language grammar: An event-related potential investigation. Studies in second language acquisition, 27 (2), 173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Truscott, J., & Sharwood-Smith, M. (2004). Acquisition by processing: A modular perspective on language development. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 7, 120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tyler, L. K., & Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (1977). The on-line effects of semantic context on syntactic processing. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16 (6), 683692.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Gompel, R. P., & Liversedge, S. P. (2003). The influence of morphological information on cataphoric pronoun assignment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29 (1), 128.Google ScholarPubMed
Vatz, K. L. (2009). Grammatical gender representation and processing in advanced second language learners of French. Unpublished PhD dissertation, University of Maryland.Google Scholar
Vigliocco, G., & Franck, J. (1999). When sex and syntax go hand in hand: Gender agreement in language production. Journal of Memory and Language, 40 (4), 455478.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vigliocco, G., Vinson, D. P., Arciuli, J., & Barber, H. (2008). The role of grammatical class on word recognition. Brain and language, 105 (3), 175184.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vigliocco, G., Vinson, D. P., Paganelli, F., & Dworzynski, K. (2005). Grammatical gender effects on cognition: implications for language learning and language use. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134 (4), 501.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vinson, D. P., & Vigliocco, G. (2002). A semantic analysis of grammatical class impairments: semantic representations of object nouns, action nouns and action verbs. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 15 (3), 317351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Von Studnitz, R. E., & Green, D. W. (2002). Interlingual homograph interference in German–English bilinguals: Its modulation and locus of control. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 5 (01), 123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whorf, B. L. (1956). Language, thought, and reality: Selected writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf. In Carroll, J. B. (Ed.). New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar