Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-l7hp2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-24T12:26:37.890Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The reduced embodiment of a second language

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 December 2021

Tal Norman*
Affiliation:
Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel
Orna Peleg
Affiliation:
The program of cognitive studies of language use and Sagol school of neuroscience Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel
*
Address for correspondence: Tal Norman, Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders University of Haifa, 199 Abba Khoushi Ave., 3498838, Haifa, Israeltalnorm@gmail.com

Abstract

Substantial evidence indicates that first language (L1) comprehension involves embodied visual simulations. The present study tested the assumption that a formally learned second language (L2), which is less related to real-life experiences, is processed in a less embodied manner relative to a naturally acquired L1. To this end, bilingual participants completed the same task in their L1 and L2. In the task, they read sentences and decided immediately after each sentence whether a pictured object had been mentioned in the preceding sentence. Responses were significantly faster when the shape of the object in the picture matched rather than mismatched the sentence-implied shape, but only in the L1, and only when the L1 block was performed before the L2 block. These findings suggest that embodied visual simulations are reduced in a formally learned L2 and may be subjected to cross-language influences.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ahn, S and Jiang, N (2018) Automatic semantic integration during L2 sentential reading. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 21, 375383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baayen, RH, Davidson, DJ and Bates, DM (2008) Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of memory and language 59, 390412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barsalou, LW (1999) Perceptions of perceptual symbols. Behavioral and brain sciences 22, 637660.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barsalou, LW (2008) Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology 59, 617645.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barsalou, LW, Santos, A, Simmons, WK and Wilson, CD (2008) Language and simulation in conceptual processing. Symbols, embodiment, and meaning, 245283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bedny, M and Caramazza, A (2011) Perception, action, and word meanings in the human brain: the case from action verbs. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1224, 8195.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ben-Dror, I, Frost, R and Bentin, S (1995) Orthographic representation and phonemic segmentation in skilled readers: A cross-language comparison. Psychological Science 6(3), 176181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brunyé, TT, Ditman, T, Mahoney, CR, Walters, EK and Taylor, HA (2010) You heard it here first: Readers mentally simulate described sounds. Acta Psychologica 135, 209215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, D, Wang, R, Zhang, J and Liu, C (2020) Perceptual Representations in L1, L2 and L3 Comprehension: Delayed Sentence–Picture Verification. Journal of psycholinguistic research, 117.Google ScholarPubMed
Connell, L (2007) Representing object colour in language comprehension. Cognition 102, 476485.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Degani, T, Kreiner, H, Ataria, H and Khateeb, F (2019) The impact of brief exposure to the second language on native language production: Global or item specific? Applied Psycholinguistics, 131.Google Scholar
Dekker, TM, Mareschal, D, Johnson, MH and Sereno, MI (2014) Picturing words? Sensorimotor cortex activation for printed words in child and adult readers. Brain and language 139, 5867.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Koning, BB, Wassenburg, SI, Bos, LT and van der Schoot, M (2017a) Size does matter: Implied object size is mentally simulated during language comprehension. Discourse processes 54, 493503.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Koning, BB, Wassenburg, SI, Bos, LT and van der Schoot, M (2017b) Mental simulation of four visual object properties: similarities and differences as assessed by the sentence–picture verification task. Journal of Cognitive Psychology 29, 420432.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dudschig, C, de la Vega, I and Kaup, B (2014) Embodiment and second-language: Automatic activation of motor responses during processing spatially associated L2 words and emotion L2 words in a vertical Stroop paradigm. Brain and Language 132, 1421.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fan, SP, Liberman, Z, Keysar, B and Kinzler, KD (2015) The exposure advantage: Early exposure to a multilingual environment promotes effective communication. Psychological science 26, 10901097.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glenberg, AM and Kaschak, MP (2002) Grounding language in action. Psychonomic bulletin & review 9, 558565.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grosjean, F (2010) Bilingual. Harvard university press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Havas, DA, Glenberg, AM and Rinck, M (2007) Emotion simulation during language comprehension. Psychonomic bulletin & review 14, 436441.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayakawa, S and Keysar, B (2018) Using a foreign language reduces mental imagery. Cognition 173, 815.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holt, LE and Beilock, SL (2006) Expertise and its embodiment: Examining the impact of sensorimotor skill expertise on the representation of action-related text. Psychonomic bulletin & review 13, 694701.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hsu, CT, Jacobs, AM and Conrad, M (2015) Can Harry Potter still put a spell on us in a second language? An fMRI study on reading emotion-laden literature in late bilinguals. Cortex 63, 282295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jared, D, Poh, RPY and Paivio, A (2013) L1 and L2 picture naming in Mandarin–English bilinguals: A test of bilingual dual coding theory. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 16, 383396.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keysar, B, Hayakawa, SL and An, SG (2012) The foreign-language effect: Thinking in a foreign tongue reduces decision biases. Psychological science 23, 661668.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koster, D, Cadierno, T and Chiarandini, M (2018) Mental simulation of object orientation and size: A conceptual replication with second language learners. Journal of the European Second Language Association 2(1).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kreiner, H and Degani, T (2015) Tip-of-the-tongue in a second language: The effects of brief first-language exposure and long-term use. Cognition 137, 106114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lemhöfer, K and Broersma, M (2012) Introducing LexTALE: A quick and valid lexical test for advanced learners of English. Behavior research methods 44, 325343. http://lextale.com/takethetest.htmlCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mahon, BZ (2015) The burden of embodied cognition. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale 69, 172.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marian, V, Blumenfeld, HK and Kaushanskaya, M (2007) The Language Experience and Proficiency Questionnaire (LEAP-Q): Assessing language profiles in bilinguals and multilinguals. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morey, RD, Kaschak, MP, Díez-Álamo, AM, Glenberg, AM, Zwaan, RA, Lakens, D, Ibáñez, A, García, A, Gianelli, C, Jones, JL, Madden, J, Alifano, F, Bergen, B, Bloxsom, NG, Bub, DN, Cai, ZG, Chartier, CR, Chatterjee, A, Conwell, E, Cook, SW, Davis, JD, Evers, E, Girard, S, Harter, D, Hartung, F, Herrera, E, Huettig, F, Humphries, S, Juanchich, M, Kühne, K, Lu, S, Lynes, T, Masson, MEJ, Ostarek, M, Pessers, S, Reglin, R, Steegen, S, Thiessen, ED, Thomas, LE, Trott, S, Vandekerckhove, J, Vanpaemel, W, Vlachou, M, Williams, K and Ziv-Crispel, N (2021) A pre-registered, multi-lab non-replication of the action-sentence compatibility effect (ACE). Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.Google Scholar
Myung, JY, Blumstein, SE and Sedivy, JC (2006) Playing on the typewriter, typing on the piano: manipulation knowledge of objects. Cognition 98(3), 223243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Niedenthal, PM, Winkielman, P, Mondillon, L and Vermeulen, N (2009) Embodiment of emotion concepts. Journal of personality and social psychology 96, 1120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oldfield, RC (1971) The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9, 97113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ostarek, M and Huettig, F (2019) Six challenges for embodiment research. Current Directions in Psychological Science 28, 593599.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peleg, O, Ozer, R, Norman, T and Segal, O (2018) Perceptual simulations during sentence comprehension: A comparison between typical adolescents and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Neurolinguistics 45, 3644.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pexman, PM (2019) The role of embodiment in conceptual development. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 34, 12741283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pulvermüller, F (1999) Words in the brain's language. Behavioral and brain sciences 22, 253279.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pulvermüller, F (2013) How neurons make meaning: brain mechanisms for embodied and abstract-symbolic semantics. Trends in cognitive sciences 17, 458470.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sato, M, Schafer, AJ and Bergen, BK (2013) One word at a time: mental representations of object shape change incrementally during sentence processing. Language and Cognition 5, 345373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanfield, RA and Zwaan, RA (2001) The effect of implied orientation derived from verbal context on picture recognition. Psychological science 12, 153156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vukovic, N and Shtyrov, Y (2014) Cortical motor systems are involved in second-language comprehension: Evidence from rapid mu-rhythm desynchronisation. NeuroImage 102, 695703.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Winter, B and Bergen, B (2012) Language comprehenders represent object distance both visually and auditorily. Language and Cognition 4, 116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yaxley, RH and Zwaan, RA (2007) Simulating visibility during language comprehension. Cognition 105, 229236.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhang, Y (2014) Online tool for handedness assessment. Retrieved from http://zhanglab.wikicom/handedness.Google Scholar
Zwaan, RA (2004) The immersed experiencer: Toward an embodied theory of language comprehension. Psychology of learning and motivation 44, 3562.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zwaan, RA and Madden, CJ (2005) Embodied sentence comprehension. Grounding cognition: The role of perception and action in memory, language, and thinking, 224245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zwaan, RA, Madden, CJ, Yaxley, RH and Aveyard, ME (2004) Moving words: Dynamic representations in language comprehension. Cognitive Science 28, 611619.Google Scholar
Zwaan, RA and Pecher, D (2012) Revisiting mental simulation in language comprehension: Six replication attempts. PloS one 7, e51382.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zwaan, RA, Stanfield, RA and Yaxley, RH (2002) Language comprehenders mentally represent the shapes of objects. Psychological Sience 13, 168171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar