Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-g8jcs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-20T19:19:13.327Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Computational mechanisms of development? Connectionism and bilingual lexical representation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 November 2021

Ping Li*
Affiliation:
TheHong KongPolytechnic University, Hong Kong
Xiaowei Zhao
Affiliation:
Emmanuel College, Boston, USA
*
*Address for correspondence: Ping Li, Department of Chinese and Bilingual Studies, Faculty of Humanities, The Hong Kong Polytechnic University, Hung Hom, Hong Kong. Email: pi2li@polyu.edu.hk

Abstract

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Peer Commentaries
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bordag, D, Gor, K and Opitz, A (2021) Ontogenesis Model of the L2 Lexical Representation. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 117. doi:10.1017/S1366728921000250Google Scholar
Claussenius-Kalman, H, Hernandez, A and Li, P (2021) Expertise, Ecosystem, and Emergentism: Dynamic developmental bilingualism. Brain and Language 222, 105013. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2021.105013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellis, NC (2003) Constructions, chunking, and connectionism: The emergence of second language structure. In Doughty, CJ and Long, MH (eds.), The handbook of second language acquisition Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing Ltd pp. 63103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
French, RM (1998) A simple recurrent network model of bilingual memory. In Gernsbacher, MA and Derry, SJ (eds.), Proceedings of the 20th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 368373). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
French, RM and Jacquet, M (2004) Understanding bilingual memory: Models and data. Trends in Cognitive Science 8, 8793.10.1016/j.tics.2003.12.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grant, A, Legault, J and Li, P (2019) What do bilingual models tell us about the neurocognition of multiple languages? In Schwieter, J (ed.), The handbook of the neuroscience of multilingualism. Wiley-Blackwell, pp. 4874.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hernandez, A, Li, P and MacWhinney, B (2005) The emergence of competing modules in bilingualism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9(5), 220225.10.1016/j.tics.2005.03.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hebb, D (1949). The organization of behavior: A neuropsychological theory. New York, NY: Wiley.Google Scholar
Kiran, S, Graesman, U, Sandberg, C and Miikkulainen, R (2013) A computational account of bilingual aphasia rehabilitation. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 16 (Special Issue), 325342. doi:10.1017/S1366728912000533CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kohonen, T (2001) Self-organizing maps (3rd ed.). Berlin: Springer.10.1007/978-3-642-56927-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewy, N and Grosjean, F (2008) The Lewy and Grosjean BIMOLA model. In Grosjean, F (ed.), Studying bilinguals. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 201210.Google Scholar
Li, P (2002) Bilingualism is in dire need of formal models. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 5(3), 213. doi:10.1017/S1366728902253018CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, P (2013) Computational modeling of bilingualism: How can models tell us more about the bilingual mind? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 16(2), 241245. doi:10.1017/S1366728913000059CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, P and Farkas, I (2002) A self-organizing connectionist model of bilingual processing. In Heredia, R and Altarriba, J (eds.), Bilingual sentence processing. North-Holland: Elsevier Science Publisher, pp. 5985.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, P and Zhao, X (2013) Self-organizing map models of language acquisition. Frontiers in Psychology. 4(828). doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00828Google Scholar
Li, P and Zhao, X (2017) Computational modeling. In de Groot, AMB and Hagoort, P (eds.), Research methods in psycholinguistics and the neurobiology of language: A practical guide. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc, pp. 208229.Google Scholar
Li, P and Zhao, X (2020) Connectionism. In Aronoff, M (ed.), Oxford bibliographies online (Linguistics). Oxford University Press. (www.oxfordbibliographies.com; originally published 2012, last updated 2020)Google Scholar
Li, P, Zhao, X and MacWhinney, B (2007) Dynamic self-organization and early lexical development in children. Cognitive Science: A Multidisciplinary Journal 31, 581612.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McClelland, J and Rumelhart, D (1981). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 1. An account of basic findings. Psychological Review 88, 375407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peñaloza, C, Grasemann, U, Dekhtyar, M, Miikkulainen, R and Kiran, S (2019) BiLex: A computational approach to the effects of age of acquisition and language exposure on bilingual lexical access. Brain and Language 195, 104643. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2019.104643CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plunkett, K and Marchman, VA (1996) Learning from a connectionist model of the acquisition of the English past tense. Cognition 61(3), 299308. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0010-0277(96)00721-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shook, A and Marian, V (2013) The bilingual language interaction network for comprehension of speech. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 16(2), 304324.10.1017/S1366728912000466CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shirai, Y (2018) Connectionism and second language acquisition. Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203118085CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomas, MSC (1997) Connectionist networks and knowledge representation: The case of bilingual lexical processing. PhD thesis, Oxford University.Google Scholar
Wen, Z, Biedroń, A and Skehan, P (2017). Foreign language aptitude theory: Yesterday, today and tomorrow. Language Teaching 50(1), 131. doi:10.1017/S0261444816000276CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhang, X, Yang, J, Wang, R and Li, P (2020) A neuroimaging study of semantic representation in first and second languages. Language, Cognition, and Neuroscience 35, 12231238. https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2020.1738509CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhao, X and Li, P (2010) Bilingual lexical interactions in an unsupervised neural network model. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism. 13, 505524.10.1080/13670050.2010.488284CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhao, X and Li, P (2013) Simulating cross-language priming with a dynamic computational model of the lexicon. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 16, 288303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhao, X and Li, P (2021) Fuzzy or Clear? A Computational Approach Towards Dynamic L2 Lexical-Semantic Representation. Frontiers in Communication. Ms. Under review10.3389/fcomm.2021.726443CrossRefGoogle Scholar