Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jn8rn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-21T14:13:35.795Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Continuous effects of bilingualism and attention on Flanker task performance

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 January 2020

Ashley Chung-Fat-Yim
Affiliation:
York University, Toronto, Ontario
Geoffrey B. Sorge
Affiliation:
Upper Canada College, Toronto, Ontario
Ellen Bialystok*
Affiliation:
York University, Toronto, Ontario
*
Address for correspondence: Ellen Bialystok, E-mail: ellenb@yorku.ca

Abstract

Both bilingualism and attention contribute to the development of executive functioning (EF), with higher levels of both leading to better outcomes. The present study treats bilingualism and attention as continuous variables to investigate their impact on EF. Eighty-two 9-year-olds who were attending a French school in an anglophone community completed a flanker task. Children's progress in French represented their level of bilingualism, and attention was assessed through a standard standardized instrument. Degree of bilingualism and degree of attention were both positively related to performance, but exposure to a third language in the home did not further affect outcomes.

Type
Research Notes
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

American Psychiatric Association (2013) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.Google Scholar
Anderson, JAE, Hawrylewicz, K and Bialystok, E (in press) Who is bilingual? Snapshots across the lifespan. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. doi:10.1017/S1366728918000950Google Scholar
Antón, E, Duñabeitia, JA, Estévez, A, Hernández, JA, Castillo, A, Fuentes, LJ, Davidson, DJ and Carreiras, M (2014) Is there a bilingual advantage in the ANT task? Evidence from children. Frontiers in Psychology 5, 398. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00398Google Scholar
Antoniou, M (2019) The advantages of bilingualism debate. Annual Review of Linguistics 5, 395415. doi:10.1146/annurev-linguistics-011718-011820CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bak, TH (2019) Cooking pasta in la paz: Bilingualism, bias, and the replication crisis. In Sekerina, IA, Spradlin, L and Valian, V (eds), Bilingualism, executive function, and beyond: Questions and insights. Amsterdam, Netherlands: John Benjamins Publishing Company, pp. 8199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E (2017) The bilingual adaptation: How minds accommodate experience. Psychological Bulletin 143, 233262. doi:10.1037/bul0000099CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E, Craik, FIM, Klein, R and Viswanathan, M (2004) Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging 19, 290303. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.19.2.290CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E, Hawrylewicz, K, Wiseheart, M and Toplak, M (2017) Interaction of bilingualism and Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity disorder in young adults. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 20, 588601. doi:10.1017/S1366728915000887CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blom, E, Boerma, T, Bosma, E, Cornips, L and Everaert, E (2017) Cognitive advantages of bilingual children in different sociolinguistic contexts. Frontiers in Psychology 8, 522. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00552CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bradley, RH and Corwyn, RF (2002) Socioeconomic status and child development. Annual Review of Psychology 53, 371399. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135233CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calvo, A and Bialystok, E (2014) Independent effects of bilingualism and socioeconomic status on language ability and executive functioning. Cognition 130, 278288. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2013.11.015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chung-Fat-Yim, A, Himel, C and Bialystok, E (2019) The impact of bilingualism on executive function in adolescents. International Journal of Bilingualism 23, 12781290. doi:10.1177/1367006918781059CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Costa, A, Hernández, M and Sebastián-Gallés, N (2008) Bilingualism aids conflict resolution: Evidence from the ANT task. Cognition 106, 5986. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2006.12.013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dash, T, Berroir, P, Joanette, Y and Ansaldo, AI (2019) Alerting, orienting, and executive control: The effect of bilingualism and age on the subcomponents of attention. Frontiers in Neurology 10, 1122. doi:10.3389/fneur.2019.01122CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeLuca, V, Rothman, J, Bialystok, E and Pliatsikas, C (2019a) Redefining bilingualism as a spectrum of experiences that differentially affects brain structure and function. PNAS 116, 75657574. doi:10.1073/pnas.1811513116CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeLuca, V, Rothman, J, Bialystok, E and Pliatsikas, C (2019b) Duration and extent of bilingual experience modulate neurocognitive outcomes. NeuroImage 204, 116222. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116222CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diamond, A (2013) Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology 64, 135168. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, LM and Dunn, LM (1997) Peabody picture vocabulary test-III. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Dunn, LM, Dunn, LM and Thériault-Whalen, CM (1993) Échelle de vocabulaire en images Peabody. Toronto, ON: PSYCAN.Google Scholar
Engel de Abreu, PM, Cruz-Santos, A, Tourinho, CJ, Martin, R and Bialystok, E (2012) Bilingualism enriches the poor: Enhanced cognitive control in low-income minority children. Psychological Science 23, 13641371. doi:10.1177/0956797612443836CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eriksen, BA and Eriksen, CW (1974) Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception and Psychophysics 16, 143149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gullifer, JW, Chai, XJ, Whitford, V, Pivneva, I, Baum, S, Klein, D and Titone, D (2018) Bilingual experience and resting-state brain connectivity: Impacts of L2 age of acquisition and social diversity of language use on control networks. Neuropsychologia 117, 123134. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.04.037CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hartanto, A, Toh, WX and Yang, H (2019) Bilingualism narrows socioeconomic disparities in executive functions and self-regulatory behaviors during early childhood: Evidence from the early childhood longitudinal study. Child Development 90, 12151235. doi:10.1111/cdev.13032CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hilchey, MD and Klein, RM (2011) Are there bilingual advantages on nonlinguistic interference tasks? Implications for the plasticity of executive control processes. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 18, 625658. doi:10.3758/s13423-011-0116-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Incera, S and McLennan, CT (2018) Bilingualism and age are continuous variables that influence executive function. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition 25, 443463. doi:10.1080/13825585.2017.1319902CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kline, RB (2001) Brief Cognitive Assessment of Children: Review of Instruments and Recommendations for Best Practice In Andrews, JJW, Saklofske, DH and Janzen, HL (eds), Handbook of psychoeducational assessment: Ability, achievement, and behavior in children. New York, NY: Academic Press, pp. 103132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kovács, AM and Mehler, J (2009) Cognitive gains in 7-month-old bilingual infants. PNAS 106, 65566560. doi:10.1073/pnas.0811323106CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuss, O (2013) The danger of dichotomizing continuous variables: A visualization. Teaching Statistics: An International Journal for Teachers 35, 7879. doi:10.1111/test.12006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Melzi, G, Schick, AR and Escobar, K (2017) Early bilingualism through the looking glass: Latino preschool children's language and self-regulation skills. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics 37, 93109. doi:10.1017/S0267190517000083CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mezzacappa, E (2004) Alerting, orienting, and executive attention: Developmental properties and sociodemographic correlates in an epidemiological sample of young, urban children. Child Development 75, 13731386. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00746.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mor, B, Yitzhaki-Amsalem, S and Prior, A (2015) The joint effect of bilingualism and ADHD on executive functions. Journal of Attention Disorders 19, 527541. doi:10.1177/1087054714527790CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morton, JB and Harper, SN (2007) What did Simon say? Revisiting the bilingual advantage. Developmental Science 10, 719726. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00623.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mullane, JC, Corkum, PV, Klein, RM and McLaughlin, E (2009) Interference control in children with and without ADHD: A systematic review of Flanker and Simon task performance. Child Neuropsychology 15, 321342. doi:10.1080/09297040802348028CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Naeem, K, Filippi, R, Periche-Tomas, E, Papageorgiou, A and Bright, P (2018) The importance of socioeconomic status as a modulator of the bilingual advantage in cognitive ability. Frontiers in Psychology 9, e1818. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01818CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Noble, KG, Norman, MF and Farah, MJ (2005) Neurocognitive correlates of socioeconomic status in kindergarten children. Developmental Science 8, 7487. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00394.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paap, KR and Greenberg, ZI (2013) There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology 66, 232258. doi:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2012.12.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paap, KR, Johnson, HA and Sawi, O (2015) Bilingual advantages in executive functioning either do not exist or are restricted to very specific and undetermined circumstances. Cortex 69, 265278. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2015.04.014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Papageorgiou, A, Bright, P, Periche Tomas, E and Filippi, R (2019) Evidence against a cognitive advantage in the older bilingual population. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 72, 13541363. doi:10.1177/1747021818796475CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poarch, GJ and Bialystok, E (2015) Bilingualism as a model for multitasking. Developmental Review 35, 113124. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2014.12.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poarch, GJ and Van Hell, J (2012) Executive functions and inhibitory control in multilingual children: Evidence from second language learners, bilinguals, and trilinguals. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 113, 535551. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2012.06.013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Psychology Software Tools, Inc. [E-Prime 1.0]. (2016) Retrieved from https://www.pstnet.com.Google Scholar
Raven, J, Raven, J.C. and Court, JH (2004) Manual for Raven's Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary Scales. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment.Google Scholar
Shalev, L and Tsal, Y (2003) The wide attentional window: A major deficit of children with attention difficulties. Journal of Learning Disabilities 36, 517527. doi:10.1177/00222194030360060301CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sorge, GB, Toplak, ME and Bialystok, E (2017) Interactions between levels of attention and levels of bilingualism in children's executive functioning. Developmental Science 20, e12408. doi:10.1111/desc.12408CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sulpizio, S, Del Maschio, N, Del Mauro, G, Fedeli, D and Abutalebi, J (2020) Bilingualism as a gradient measure modulates functional connectivity of language and control networks, NeuroImage 205, 116306. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116306CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Surrain, S and Luk, G (2017) Describing bilinguals: A systematic review of labels and descriptions used in the literature between 2005–2015. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 22, 401415. doi:10.1017/S1366728917000682CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swanson, JM, Schuck, S, Mann Porter, M, Carlson, C, Hartman, CA, Sergeant, JA, Clevenger, W, Wasdell, M, McCleary, R, Lakes, K and Wigal, T (2012) Categorical and dimensional definitions and evaluations of symptoms of ADHD: History of the SNAP and the SWAN rating scales. International Journal of Educational Psychological Assessment 10, 5170.Google ScholarPubMed
Thomas-Sunesson, D, Hakuta, K and Bialystok, E (2018) Degree of bilingualism modifies executive control in Hispanic children in the USA. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism 21, 197206. doi:10.1080/13670050.2016.1148114CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willcutt, EG, Doyle, AE, Nigg, JT, Faraone, SV and Pennington, BF (2005) Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analytic review. Biological Psychiatry 57, 13361346. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.02.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yamasaki, BL, Stocco, A and Prat, CS (2018) Relating individual differences in bilingual language experiences to executive attention. Language, Cognition, and Neuroscience 33, 11281151. doi:10.1080/23273798.2018.1448092CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yang, S, Yang, H and Lust, B (2011) Early childhood bilingualism leads to advances in executive attention: Dissociating culture and language. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 14, 412422. doi:10.1017/S13667289100000611CrossRefGoogle Scholar