Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-lj6df Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-18T22:19:45.386Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Familial risk and childhood adversity interplay in the onset of psychosis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Antonella Trotta
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King's College London, UK
Marta Di Forti
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King's College London, UK
Conrad lyegbe
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King's College London, UK
Priscilla Green
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King's College London, UK
Paola Dazzan
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King's College London, UK
Valeria Mondelli
Affiliation:
Section of Stress, Psychiatry and Immunology and Perinatal Psychiatry, Department of Psychological Medicine, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King's College London, UK
Craig Morgan
Affiliation:
Health Service and Population Research Department, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King's College London, UK
Robin M. Murray
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King's College London, UK
Helen L. Fisher*
Affiliation:
MRC Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King's College London, UK
*
Helen L. Fisher, MRC Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King's College London, 16 De Crespigny Park, London SE5 8AF, UK. Email: helen.2.fisher@kcl.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

The association between childhood adversity and psychosis in adulthood is well established. However, genetic factors might confound or moderate this association.

Aims

Using a catchment-based case-control sample, we explored the main effects of, and interplay between, childhood adversity and family psychiatric history on the onset of psychosis.

Method

Childhood adversity (parental separation and death, physical and sexual abuse) was assessed retrospectively in 224 individuals with a first presentation of psychosis and 256 community controls from South London, UK. Occurrence of psychotic and affective disorders in first-degree relatives was ascertained with the Family Interview for Genetic Studies (FIGS).

Results

Parental history of psychosis did not confound the association between childhood adversity and psychotic disorder. There was no evidence that childhood adversity and familial liability combined synergistically to increase odds of psychosis beyond the effect of each individually.

Conclusions

Our results do not support the hypothesis that family psychiatric history amplifies the effect of childhood adversity on odds of psychosis.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC-BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Royal College of Psychiatrists 2015

Childhood adversity has been shown to be associated with early psychotic symptoms, Reference Arseneault, Cannon, Fisher, Polanczyk, Moffitt and Caspi1Reference Wolke, Lereya, Fisher, Lewis and Zammit3 with transition to clinical psychosis in individuals at ultra-high risk (UHR), Reference Bechdolf, Thompson, Nelson, Cotton, Simmons and Amminger4 as well as with the onset of a full-blown psychotic disorder. Reference Fisher, Jones, Fearon, Craig, Dazzan and Morgan5Reference Trotta, Di Forti, Mondelli, Dazzan, Pariante and David7 Furthermore, associations are usually stronger with increasing frequency and severity of the trauma experienced, Reference van Winkel, Stefanis and Myin-Germeys8 indicating a key role for this environmental factor in the development of psychosis. However, genetic vulnerabilities have been repeatedly shown to be involved in the aetiology of psychosis. Reference Sullivan9 Studies have estimated the heritability of schizophrenia to be around 60%, Reference Lichtenstein, Yip, Björk, Pawitan, Cannon and Sullivan10 and concordance for schizophrenia in monozygotic twins is around 50%. Reference Cardno and Gottesman11 Moreover, having one or more biological parents with a history of psychosis has been associated with a greater risk of exposure to stressful life events and adverse experiences during childhood Reference Walsh, MacMillan and Jamieson12,Reference Walder, Faraone, Glatt, Tsuang and Seidman13 and also with the development of psychotic symptoms and disorders. Reference Asarnow, Nuechterlein, Fogelson, Subotnik, Payne and Russell14,Reference Polanczyk, Moffitt, Arseneault, Cannon, Ambler and Keefe15 This suggests that a ‘passive’ type of gene–environment correlation (rGE) Reference Plomin, DeFries and Loehlin16 might be operating such that parents provide their children with both an adverse upbringing and a genetic vulnerability to developing psychosis. This implies that parents’ genetic make-up may be confounding the childhood adversity–psychosis associations observed in previous studies.

It is also possible that genetic factors moderate the association between childhood adversity and psychosis (a gene–environment interaction, G × E), Reference Plomin, DeFries and Loehlin16 potentially by influencing how an individual reacts biologically and/or psychologically following exposure to adversity which may set them off on the path to psychosis. Reference van Os, Rutten and Poulton17 A number of studies have examined rGE and G × E using indirect measures of genetic risk, such as being a relative, a twin or adopted-away offspring of a person with schizophrenia. Reference Carter, Schulsinger, Parnas, Cannon and Mednick18Reference Tienari, Wynne, Läksy, Moring, Nieminen and Sorri20 The advantage of using familial liability to psychosis as a proxy for genetic risk is that it may capture a greater proportion of genetic load, including gene–gene interactions, in contrast to studies using direct molecular genetic measures that tend to incorporate only a small contribution to genetic variation in the form of single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs). Reference McGrath, Mortensen, Visscher and Wray21 Moreover, despite the advent of polygenic risk scores, which combine multiple SNPs and thus increase the amount of genetic variation accounted for, these may not provide any additional mechanistic clues over and above measures of family psychiatric history because they aggregate information across thousands of SNPs, thus making it difficult to disentangle which combinations of SNPs are driving the interaction. Reference Jaffee and Price22 It is important to note though that a history of psychosis and other psychiatric disorders in first-degree relatives is only a proxy for genetic risk and may also reflect some aspects of the environment in which individuals are brought up, Reference van Os, Rutten and Poulton17 though this component is likely to be fairly small. Reference Lichtenstein, Yip, Björk, Pawitan, Cannon and Sullivan10 Therefore, at present there is no ideal measure of genetic risk to employ in exploring rGE and G × E for psychosis and thus triangulation of evidence obtained from different measures across multiple studies is likely to be the best overall strategy. Here we focus on trying to broaden the evidence base in relation to familial liability to psychosis.

Despite several studies previously investigating interactions between genetic liability and childhood adversity in the onset of psychosis, those involving familial liability as a measure of proxy genetic risk have been restricted to general population samples. Reference Arseneault, Cannon, Fisher, Polanczyk, Moffitt and Caspi1,Reference Asarnow, Nuechterlein, Fogelson, Subotnik, Payne and Russell14,Reference Alemany, Goldberg, van Winkel, Gastó, Peralta and Fañanás23Reference Wigman, van Winkel, Ormel, Verhulst, van Os and Vollebergh26 Only one study has investigated the interplay between childhood adversity and familial risk for mental health problems in a first-episode psychosis sample, Reference Fisher, McGuffin, Boydell, Fearon, Craig and Dazzan27 but this focused solely on one form of adversity, namely maternal physical abuse. In light of this, the aim of the present study was to extend existing research by investigating, for the first time, the interplay between various forms of childhood adversity and family psychiatric history in the onset of psychotic disorders. We sought to test two hypotheses: (a) individuals with a parental history of psychosis or affective disorders would have a greater prevalence of both psychotic disorders and childhood adversity than those without this proxy genetic vulnerability; and (b) childhood adversity would combine synergistically (on an additive scale) with familial liability to increase the odds of psychotic disorder.

Method

Participants

The sample was drawn from patients who participated in the Genes and Psychosis (GAP) study from Lambeth, Southwark and Croydon adult in-patient units of the South London & Maudsley (SLAM) Mental Health National Health Service (NHS) Foundation Trust, UK. Inclusion criteria for patients were: age 16–65 years, presenting to psychiatric services for the first time with a psychotic disorder (codes F20-29 and F30-33 from the International Classification of Diseases (ICD-10)), 28 and resident within tightly defined catchment areas in south-east London, UK. Exclusion criteria were: organic psychosis; IQ < 50; previous contact with services for psychosis; and transient psychotic symptoms resulting from acute intoxication (ICD-10). 28 ICD-10 diagnoses were determined using data from the Schedules for Clinical Assessment in Neuropsychiatry (SCAN). 29

Control participants were aged 16–65 years and recruited from the local population living in the area served by the Trust, by means of internet and newspaper advertisements, and distribution of leaflets at train stations, shops and job centres. Efforts were made to obtain a control sample that was representative of the general population in age, gender, ethnicity, educational qualifications and employment status. The Psychosis Screening Questionnaire (PSQ) Reference Bebbington and Nayani30 was administered to all potential control group participants; individuals were excluded if they met criteria for a psychotic disorder.

Measures

A range of sociodemographic information was obtained including age at interview, gender, current level of education and self-ascribed ethnicity (using the UK 2001 census categories) during face-to-face interviews using the Medical Research Council Sociodemographic Schedule. Reference Mallett31

Childhood adversity

The Childhood Experience of Care and Abuse Questionnaire (CECA-Q) Reference Bifulco, Bernazzani, Moran and Jacobs32 was employed to retrospectively elicit information from participants concerning a range of adverse childhood experiences before the age of 17 years. For this analysis, physical abuse by the main mother and father figures (usually but not necessarily the biological parents), sexual abuse by any adult or an individual at least 5 years older than the recipient, separation from a parent for at least 6 months and death of a parent were included. Full details of the questionnaire are provided in Bifulco et al. Reference Bifulco, Bernazzani, Moran and Jacobs32 The CECA-Q has been shown to have good internal consistency, Reference Smith, Lam, Bifulco and Checkley33 satisfactory levels of test–retest reliability over 7 years in a psychosis sample Reference Fisher, Craig, Fearon, Morgan, Dazzan and Lappin34 and reasonable concurrent validity with existing measures. Reference Bifulco, Bernazzani, Moran and Jacobs32Reference Fisher, Craig, Fearon, Morgan, Dazzan and Lappin34 The CECA-Q elicits concrete examples of adverse experiences, and severity of the responses is scored in a standardised manner to enhance validity of the self-reported experiences. Reference Bifulco, Bernazzani, Moran and Jacobs32 Every childhood experience section of the CECA-Q begins with screening questions and then positive responses are followed up with more detailed questions. This questionnaire was read out to all participants to improve the accuracy of the fixed category responses obtained. The physical and sexual abuse variables were dichotomised into severe and non-severe categories using the most conservative published cut-off points. Reference Bifulco, Bernazzani, Moran and Jacobs32

Family history of mental illness

The Family Interview for Genetic Studies (FIGS; https://www.nimhgenetics.org/interviews/figs) was used to obtain information about the participant's family history of mental health problems. This interview begins with a brief construction of a pedigree diagram for the participant's first-degree relatives and a series of screening questions to elicit information about possible mental health problems in these relatives. Positive responses to any of these are followed up with more specific questions to obtain symptom and treatment information for each potentially affected relative. Only three of these supplementary sections were chosen for this study, namely depression, mania and psychosis. For patients, this interview was supplemented by information retrieved from clinical records. To maximise genetic risk, only information on first-degree relatives (participant's biological mother and father, full siblings and children) was used. The FIGS consensus diagnoses were divided into several familial risk variables. First, a ‘family mental illness’ variable referred to the presence (1) or absence (0) of current or past psychosis, mania or depression in at least one first-degree relative. A ‘family psychosis’ variable denoted the presence (1) or absence (0) of a current or previous diagnosis of psychosis in at least one first-degree relative. A ‘parental mental illness’ variable was also created that indicated the presence (1) or absence (0) of a current or previous diagnosis of psychosis, mania or depression in at least one biological parent. Similarly, a variable for ‘parental psychosis’ was created that denoted the presence (1) or absence (0) of current or past psychosis in at least one biological parent.

Ethics

Ethical permission was obtained from the SLAM and the Institute of Psychiatry Research Ethics Committee. All participants provided written consent after reading a detailed information sheet.

Statistical analysis

All analyses were performed using Stata version 11.1 (Stata-Corp, College Station, TX, USA). First, main effects of each type of childhood adversity and (general and psychotic) family mental illness on psychosis caseness were assessed using a series of logistic regressions. Second, we tested whether differences in an individual's proxy genetic liability might drive differential environmental exposure. Specifically, the passive type of rGE was explored using binary logistic regression analysis to estimate odds ratios (ORs) of the associations between history of parental mental illness or parental psychosis and (a) psychotic disorder in the participants, and (b) each subtype of childhood adversity. If familial liability is associated with both disorder and adversity, then this indicates the possibility of a passive rGE (albeit a ‘proxy gene’ by environment correlation). The possibility that parental psychopathology may attenuate the association between childhood adversity and psychosis was also addressed by rerunning the association between childhood adversity and psychotic disorder with parental history of psychosis added as a confounder.

Next, we examined whether there was evidence that childhood adversity combined synergistically with each type of familial liability by testing for interaction on an additive scale using interaction contrast ratios (ICRs). Reference Knol, van der Tweel, Grobbee, Numans and Geerlings35,Reference Schwartz, Susser, Susser, Schwartz, Morabia and Bromet36 This approach uses ORs to estimate the relative excess risk due to interaction. Biological synergism (the odds of psychosis among individuals with both risk factors being greater than the sum of the independent effects of each risk factor) can be better estimated from additive statistical interaction than multiplicative statistical interaction. Reference Morgan, Reininghaus, Fearon, Hutchinson, Morgan and Dazzan37,Reference Morgan, Reininghaus, Reichenberg and Frissa38 As the numbers of cases and controls with a family history of psychosis were very small, interaction analyses were only conducted for family and parental history of mental illness. Post hoc estimations of power and sample size were estimated using the ‘sampsi’ command in Stata 11. All analyses were controlled for gender (male or female), age at interview (16–64 years), ethnicity (White British, Black Caribbean, Black African, Asian [all], Mixed or Other) and level of education (no qualification, school-leaving qualifications, A-levels/vocational/college or university/professional qualifications).

Results

Information on family history of mental illness was available on 224 of the 285 patients and 250 of the 256 controls with a completed CECA-Q. The patients with and without FIGS data did not differ in terms of gender (χ2 = 0.003, P = 1.000), age (t = 0.587, P = 0.558) or diagnosis (χ2 = 0.184, P = 1.000). The basic demographic data by case and control status for those included in the analyses are presented in Table 1.

Table 1 Basic demographic characteristics of psychosis patients and unaffected controls

Demographic variable Patients (n = 285)
n (%)
Controls (n = 256)
n (%)
χ2 d.f. P
Gender 2.57 1 0.065
 Male 172 (60.4) 137 (53.5)
 Female 113 (39.6) 119 (46.5)
Ethnicity 32.60 5 <0.001
 White British 72 (25.3) 102 (39.9)
 Black Caribbean 56 (19.6) 39 (15.2)
 Black African 65 (22.9) 32 (12.5)
 White other 30 (10.5) 50 (19.5)
 Asian (all) 24 (8.4) 16 (6.3)
 Other 38 (13.3) 17 (6.6)
Level of education 76.73 4 <0.001
 No qualifications 48 (17.6) 7 (3.0)
 School leaving qualifications 64 (23.5) 23 (10.0)
 A-levels/college level qualifications 40 (14.7) 53 (22.9)
 Vocational qualifications 66 (24.3) 37 (16.0)
 University or professional qualifications 54 (19.9) 111 (48.1)
Age in years t=0.342 536 0.733
Mean (s.d.) 28.9 (9.3) 29.2 (9.9)

d.f., degrees of freedom; s.d., standard deviation.

More than half of the patients were male (60.4%) and from Black or other minority ethnic groups (BME; 74.7%). The majority of the controls were also male (53.5%) and from BME groups (60.1%). Mean age at interview was around 29 years both for patients and controls. As expected, patients were significantly more likely to be from a BME group (P < 0.001), and have none or only school-leaving qualifications (P < 0.001) compared with controls. There was no significant difference in gender (P = 0.065) or age (P = 0.733) between patients and controls. These demographic factors were all controlled for in the analysis, either because they differed between patients and controls or because they have previously been shown to be associated with adversity exposure and psychosis. A total of 17 patients (6% of the overall sample) were found to have an IQ of 70 or below which is considered be the cut-off for mild intellectual impairment. Reference Wechsler39 These individuals were sufficiently cognitively able to complete the assessments and thus were retained in the sample. Unfortunately, IQ data were not available on a large enough number of participants to be included as a confounder.

Association between childhood adversity and psychotic disorder

Table 2 presents the prevalence of each type of childhood adversity for psychosis patients and controls along with the ORs of the associations with case status. All types of childhood adversity, except for sexual abuse, occurred more often among psychosis patients than unaffected controls. Following adjustment for demographic factors, only the associations between parental separation and psychosis remained statistically significant, with sexual abuse (P = 0.05) and parental loss (P = 0.06) approaching significance. These results confirm the previously demonstrated association between childhood adversity and psychosis.

Table 2 Prevalence of childhood adversity in psychosis patients and unaffected controls

Type of childhood adversity Patients (n=285)
n (%)
Controls (n=256)
n (%)
Unadjusted
OR
95% CI P Adjusted
OR a
95% CI P
Parental separation 158 (56.0) 90 (35.3) 2.34 1.65–3.31 <0.001 1.96 1.32–2.91 0.001
Parental loss 33 (11.7) 16 (6.3) 1.99 1.06–3.71 0.031 1.99 0.98–4.06 0.058
Physical abuse 65 (22.8) 39 (15.3) 1.63 1.05–2.53 0.029 1.47 0.89–2.43 0.127
Sexual abuse 41 (14.4) 28 (11.0) 1.36 0.81–2.27 0.245 1.81 1.00–3.30 0.050

Bold text indicates result statistically significant at P < 0.05.

CI, confidence interval; OR, odds ratio.

a Adjusted for gender, age at interview, ethnicity and level of education.

Association between familial liability and psychotic disorder

Table 3 presents the prevalence of each type of familial liability for psychosis patients and unaffected controls along with the ORs of the associations with case status. All types of familial risk were significantly associated with psychosis in probands. Psychotic disorders were around four times more common in first-degree relatives of patients than controls, while more broadly defined mental illness (psychosis, depression or mania) was almost twice as common. This indicates that familial liability could be considered as a proxy genetic risk factor for psychosis, though it could also indicate the negative environmental effects of living with a first-degree relative who has a serious mental disorder. In both cases, familial liability might play a role in the previously demonstrated association between childhood adversity and psychosis.

Table 3 Prevalence of familial risk in psychosis patients and unaffected controls

Type of familial risk Patients (n=224)
n (%)
Controls (n=250)
n (%)
Unadjusted
OR
95% CI P Adjusted OR a 95% CI P
Family mental illness 94 (42.0) 70 (28.0) 1.86 1.27–2.73 0.002 1.76 1.14–2.70 0.010
Family psychosis 38 (17.3) 12 (5.1) 3.90 1.98–7.68 <0.001 4.11 1.94–8.72 <0.001
Parental mental illness 65 (29.5) 49 (20.8) 1.60 1.04–2.45 0.031 1.56 0.97–2.49 <0.001
Parental psychosis 28 (12.8) 8 (3.4) 4.20 1.87–9.43 0.001 4.71 1.90–11.67 <0.001

Bold text indicates result statistically significant at P < 0.05.

Mental illness includes psychosis, depression, and mania. Family refers to first-degree relatives.

CI, confidence interval; OR, odds ratio.

a Adjusted for gender, age at interview, ethnicity and level of education.

Proxy rGE for parental psychopathology and childhood adversity

In order to investigate the presence of a passive rGE, we tested whether parental psychopathology was also associated with childhood adversity in this sample. Therefore, the reported prevalence of parental mental illness and psychosis by exposure to childhood adversity for patients and controls is presented separately in Table 4. Parental psychopathology was not associated with greater exposure to any type of childhood adversity among patients in this sample. However, parental history of depression, mania or psychosis was more common among controls with, compared with those without, a history of parental separation and physical abuse, and these associations remained significant following adjustment for potential confounders. These results do not confirm the presence of a passive rGE, as a parental history of psychosis was associated with greater odds of psychotic disorder but not with greater exposure to childhood adversity among patients in this sample.

Table 4 Association between parental mental illness and childhood adversity in psychosis patients and unaffected controls

Type of parental psychopathology Childhood adversity present, n (%) Childhood adversity absent, n (%) Unadjusted
OR
95% CI P Adjusted OR a 95% CI P
Parental separation
Psychosis patients n = 119 n = 98
 Parental mental illness 34 (28.6) 30 (30.6) 0.91 0.50–1.63 0.743 1.02 0.53–1.94 0.955
 Parental psychosis 13 (11.1) 14 (14.3) 0.75 0.33–1.68 0.485 1.01 0.40–2.52 0.986
Unaffected controls n = 82 n = 153
 Parental mental illness 25 (30.5) 24 (15.7) 2.36 1.24–4.47 0.009 2.58 1.30–5.11 0.007
 Parental psychosis 5 (6.1) 3 (2.0) 3.25 0.76–13.94 0.113 4.36 0.78–24.43 0.094
Parental loss
Psychosis patients = 26 = 190
 Parental mental illness 10 (38.5) 54 (28.4) 1.57 0.67–3.68 0.296 1.91 0.77–4.73 0.162
 Parental psychosis 5 (19.2) 23 (12.2) 1.71 0.59–4.97 0.326 2.23 0.71–6.96 0.167
Unaffected controls n = 16 n = 219
 Parental mental illness 2 (12.5) 47 (21.5) 0.52 0.11–2.38 0.402 0.56 1.12–2.65 0.463
 Parental psychosis 0 (0.0) 8 (3.6)
Physical abuse
Psychosis patients n = 47 n = 173
 Parental mental illness 14 (29.8) 51 (29.5) 1.01 0.50–2.05 0.967 1.09 0.52–2.31 0.816
 Parental psychosis 7 (14.9) 21 (12.3) 1.25 0.50–3.15 0.636 1.42 0.53–3.83 0.487
Unaffected controls n = 38 n = 196
 Parental mental illness 15 (39.5) 34 (17.3) 3.11 1.47–6.57 0.003 3.74 1.68–8.33 0.001
 Parental psychosis 3 (7.9) 5 (2.55) 3.27 0.75–14.33 0.115 4.54 0.93–22.18 0.061
Sexual abuse
Psychosis patients n = 29 n = 191
 Parental mental illness 10 (34.5) 55 (28.8) 1.30 0.57–2.98 0.533 1.24 0.53–2.89 0.625
 Parental psychosis 2 (7.0) 26 (13.8) 0.46 0.10–2.07 0.315 0.46 0.10–2.11 0.317
Unaffected controls n = 22 n = 212
 Parental mental illness 8 (36.4) 41 (19.3) 2.38 0.94–6.06 0.068 1.99 0.71–5.60 0.192
 Parental psychosis 2 (9.1) 6 (2.8) 3.43 0.65–18.14 0.146 1.60 0.16–15.57 0.685

Bold text indicates result statistically significant at P < 0.05.

Mental illness includes psychosis, depression and mania.

CI, confidence interval; OR, odds ratio.

a Adjusted for gender, age at interview, ethnicity and level of education; – indicates unable to calculate values due to at least one cell containing a zero value.

Testing for confounding by parental psychopathology

Given that parental psychosis was shown to be strongly associated with psychosis case status, we investigated whether this form of familial risk could be a confounder in the original associations between childhood adversity and psychosis. As parental separation was the only form of adversity to be robustly associated with psychotic disorder we only investigated the impact on this association. The original association between parental separation and psychotic disorder (adjusted OR = 1.96, 95% CI: 1.32–2.91, P = 0.001) was only slightly attenuated when further adjusting for parental psychosis (adjusted OR = 1.64, 95% CI: 1.07–2.50, P = 0.022).

Interaction between familial liability and childhood adversity

The associations between each combination of childhood adversity and family or parental mental illness and psychotic disorder along with the results of the interaction analyses are presented in Table 5. Associations were evident between parental separation and psychotic disorder regardless of whether or not participants had a family or parental history of mental illness. There was a trend for associations between physical abuse, sexual abuse and psychosis to be stronger among those with no familial liability for mental illness. However, there was no evidence of a positive additive interaction between these forms of childhood adversity and family history of mental illness.

Table 5 The synergistic effects of childhood adversity and familial liability to mental illness on the presence of psychotic disorder

Association with psychotic disorder
Combination of risk factors Unadjusted OR 95% CI P Adjusted OR a 95% CI P
Parental loss (PL)
No PL and no family mental illness (FMI) [reference] [reference]
PL only (FMI absent) 1.53 0.71–3.27 0.276 0.92 0.31–2.80 0.896
FMI only (PL absent) 1.20 0.81–1.78 0.354 1.12 0.67–1.89 0.664
Both PL and FMI present 3.82 1.24–11.73 0.019 2.57 0.70–9.36 0.153
ICR: 2.09, 95% CI −2.29 to 6.47, P=0.350 ICR: 1.52, 95% CI −1.90 to 4.93, P=0.384
No PL and no parental mental illness (PMI) [reference] [reference]
PL only (PMI absent) 1.63 0.81–3.25 0.170 0.78 0.29–2.12 0.631
PMI only (PL absent) 1.14 0.73–1.76 0.566 0.89 0.49–1.61 0.693
Both PL and PMI present 4.95 1.07–22.88 0.041 4.85 0.91–25.64 0.063
ICR: 3.18, 95% CI −4.43 to 10.80, P=0.412 ICR: 4.18, 95% CI −3.88 to 12.25, P=0.309
Parental separation (PS)
No PS and no family mental illness (FMI) [reference] [reference]
PS only (FMI absent) 3.09 2.02–4.72 <0.001 4.14 2.19–7.81 <0.001
FMI only (PS absent) 1.90 1.13–3.20 0.015 2.25 1.11–4.54 0.024
Both PS and FMI present 2.33 1.38–3.93 0.002 2.20 1.09–4.44 0.028
ICR: −1.66, 95% CI −3.48 to 0.15, P=0.072 ICR: −3.18, 95% CI −6.33 to 0.04, P=0.047
No PS and no parental mental illness (PMI) [reference] [reference]
PS only (PMI absent) 2.86 1.92–4.26 <0.001 3.75 2.09–6.75 <0.001
PMI only (PS absent) 1.88 1.03–3.41 0.039 2.36 1.04–5.36 0.040
Both PS and PMI present 2.04 1.14–3.64 0.016 1.62 0.75–3.51 0.224
ICR: −1.70, 95% CI −3.53 to 0.14, P=0.069 ICR: −3.50, 95% CI −6.60 to 0.40, P=0.027
Physical abuse (PA)
No PA and no family mental illness (FMI) [reference] [reference]
PA only (FMI absent) 2.53 1.43–4.48 0.001 1.69 0.74–3.88 0.212
FMI only (PA absent) 1.63 1.07–2.48 0.023 1.59 0.90–2.82 0.113
Both PA and FMI present 1.18 0.61–2.30 0.622 0.80 0.34–1.91 0.617
ICR: −1.97, 95% CI −3.74 to 0.21, P=0.028 ICR: −1.48, 95% CI −3.29 to 0.33, P=0.109
No PA and no parental mental illness (PMI) [reference] [reference]
PA only (PMI absent) 2.28 1.34–3.86 0.002 1.53 0.71–3.30 0.280
PMI only (PA absent) 1.61 0.99–2.60 0.054 1.48 0.76–2.86 0.250
Both PA and PMI present 1.00 0.47–2.13 0.999 0.67 0.26–1.76 0.419
ICR: −1.88, 95% CI −3.49 to −0.27, P=0.022 ICR: −1.33, 95% CI −3.00 to 0.34, P=0.118
Sexual abuse (SA)
No SA and no family mental illness (FMI) [reference] [reference]
SA only (FMI absent) 1.73 0.90–3.33 0.101 2.32 0.87–6.20 0.092
FMI only (SA absent) 1.41 0.95–2.11 0.091 1.33 0.78–2,28 0.298
Both SA and FMI present 1.19 0.54–2.66 0.663 1.33 0.46–3.81 0.596
ICR: −0.95, 95% CI −2.49 to 0.60, P=0.231 ICR: −1.32, 95% CI −4.03 to 1.38, P=0.337
No SA and no parental mental illness (PMI) [reference] [reference]
SA only (PMI absent) 1.52 0.83–2.76 0.172 2.30 0.96–5.51 0.062
PMI only (SA absent) 1.31 0.84–2.06 0.238 1.27 0.69–2.34 0.434
Both SA and PMI present 1.22 0.47–3.17 0.678 0.93 0.26–3.31 0.908
ICR: −0.61, 95% CI −2.16 to −0.94, P=0.444 ICR: −1.64, 95% CI −4.09 to 0.80, P=0.188

Bold text indicates result statistically significant at P < 0.05.

CI, confidence interval; ICR, interaction contrast ratio; OR, odds ratio.

a Adjusted for gender, age at interview, ethnicity and level of education.

Only for parental loss and familial liability was there suggestive evidence of departure from additivity (namely a stronger association with psychotic disorder for individuals with both a family psychiatric history and parental loss) but this failed to reach statistical significance.

Discussion

The present study investigated the role of different forms of childhood adversity and familial liability to mental illness, as well as the interaction between them, in the development of psychosis. The strongest associations between childhood adversity and psychotic disorder were found for parental separation, parental loss and physical abuse, in keeping with previous findings from an overlapping geographical area. Reference Fisher, Jones, Fearon, Craig, Dazzan and Morgan5,Reference Morgan, Kirkbride, Leff, Craig, Hutchinson and McKenzie40 Moreover, within this sample, family history of mental illness was unsurprisingly a significant risk factor for psychotic disorder. Indeed, a history of psychosis in at least one parent was four times more common among participants with psychotic disorder than community controls. There was a smaller but significant association between current or past mental illness (psychosis, depression or mania) in a first-degree relative and clinical presentation of psychosis in this sample.

However, we did not find an association between parental history of psychosis and childhood adversity among the psychosis patients and, in keeping with these findings, controlling for parental history of psychosis only resulted in a small reduction in the strength of the association between parental separation and psychotic disorder. Therefore, our results could not confirm the presence of a potential passive rGE, in which parents pass on both a genetic liability to psychosis and create an abusive environment, which has been reported in a previous study. Reference Fisher, McGuffin, Boydell, Fearon, Craig and Dazzan27 An adoption design would be required to fully exclude the possibility of a passive rGE Reference Plomin, DeFries and Loehlin16 operating in this association but suitable samples are rarely available. Unfortunately, it was not possible in the current study to explore other forms of rGE, namely evocative or active, for example, the child's genetic propensities eliciting harsher methods of physical punishment or making them more likely to select solitary environments. Reference Plomin, DeFries and Loehlin16

Moreover, there was no evidence for additive interactions between parental separation, physical abuse or sexual abuse in childhood and family psychiatric history in relation to the presence of psychotic disorder. This could suggest that these forms of childhood adversity may be associated with psychotic disorder independently of proxy genetic risk but might also reflect a lack of statistical power in this sample. Our findings are in line with previous studies reporting that the effect of childhood trauma on later experience of psychotic symptoms was independent of proxy genetic liability to psychosis. Reference Arseneault, Cannon, Fisher, Polanczyk, Moffitt and Caspi1,Reference Wigman, van Winkel, Ormel, Verhulst, van Os and Vollebergh26,Reference Fisher, McGuffin, Boydell, Fearon, Craig and Dazzan27 However, our findings suggest that this may not be the case for parental loss. Overall, our results suggest that biological and environmental risk factors are both important in the aetiology of psychosis but the effects of some forms of childhood trauma might potentially act largely independently of pre-existing genetic liability to increase risk of psychosis.

Strengths and limitations

To our knowledge, this is the first study to explore the interplay between familial liability and various forms of childhood adversity in relation to the presence of psychotic disorder. This extends a previous report from our group that focused exclusively on maternal physical abuse. Reference Fisher, McGuffin, Boydell, Fearon, Craig and Dazzan27 The current study has several advantages, such as the inclusion of a sample of patients that had recently presented to mental health services with a psychotic disorder, thus extending previous reports that only examined psychotic symptoms or probable psychosis in the general population. Reference Arseneault, Cannon, Fisher, Polanczyk, Moffitt and Caspi1,Reference Alemany, Goldberg, van Winkel, Gastó, Peralta and Fañanás23Reference Wigman, van Winkel, Ormel, Verhulst, van Os and Vollebergh26 Our controls screened negative for psychotic disorder and had prevalence rates of childhood adversity similar to those reported in studies of the UK general population. Reference Radford, Corral, Bradley, Collishaw and Fisher41 The proportion of patients reporting a first-degree relative with psychosis in this sample was 17.3% which is also within the range of existing studies. Reference Tienari, Wynne, Läksy, Moring, Nieminen and Sorri20,Reference Fisher, McGuffin, Boydell, Fearon, Craig and Dazzan27 Additionally, we used a standardised measure of adverse childhood experiences Reference Bifulco, Bernazzani, Moran and Jacobs32 and we were able to control for the potentially confounding effects of a range of demographic characteristics.

However, we had only 25% power to detect the 5% difference in proportions exposed to parental separation among individuals with a family history (n=162), compared with 100% power to detect the 27% difference in those without a family psychiatric history (n=308). Thus, we did not have enough power to test for interactions between childhood adversity and family psychiatric history in the association with psychotic disorder. Therefore, our findings should be interpreted with caution and need to be replicated in larger samples.

We also assessed childhood adversity using retrospective self-report that might have led to bias. Retrospective assessment is commonly used in studies investigating the role of childhood risk factors in psychosis as it avoids the high expense associated with following up a very large number of participants over several decades to obtain sufficient numbers with diagnosed psychosis. Although several studies have shown some bias in retrospective reports, Reference Della Femina, Yeager and Lewis42 such bias is not considered sufficiently great to invalidate retrospective case–control studies of childhood experiences. Reference Hardt and Rutter43 Moreover, previous studies have demonstrated that the effect of childhood adversity on psychosis remains significant regardless of the study design Reference Varese, Smeets, Drukker, Lieverse, Lataster and Viechtbauer44 and histories of childhood adversity obtained from psychosis patients appear remarkably reliable over time and unaffected by current symptoms. Reference Fisher, Craig, Fearon, Morgan, Dazzan and Lappin34 We also attempted to enhance the validity of the self-reported experiences by utilising the CECA-Q Reference Bifulco, Bernazzani, Moran and Jacobs32 which elicits concrete examples of adverse experiences, has a manual to score the severity of the responses in a standardised manner (http://cecainterview.com/), and uses conservative cut-offs to ensure only severe adversity is considered in analyses. All of these factors increase the likelihood of an individual accurately remembering past adverse experiences. Reference Hardt and Rutter43 However, if time had permitted it would have been preferable to conduct a more in-depth interview, such as the full Childhood Experience of Care and Abuse interview, Reference Bifulco, Brown and Harris45 with participants to obtain more detailed information about their experiences and potentially further improve accuracy of reporting. Reference Hardt and Rutter43

Additionally, as only trauma occurring during childhood was investigated in this study, it is possible that other environmental risk factors such as cannabis use Reference Di Forti, Morgan, Dazzan, Pariante, Mondelli and Reis Marques46 or trauma occurring in adulthood Reference Beards, Gayer-Anderson, Borges, Dewey, Fisher and Morgan47 might demonstrate stronger associations with psychotic disorder and confound this relationship. Unfortunately, there was insufficient information within the GAP study to explore the role of cannabis use or adversity in adulthood in potentially modifying the childhood adversity–psychosis association. Ideally, large samples would allow inclusion of several environmental variables in the same model, such as cannabis use and preceding or subsequent adversity, in order to address this issue more comprehensively and to obtain a greater understanding of psychosis aetiology.

Finally, we used familial psychopathology as a proxy for genetic liability which may not have captured all of the relevant genetic influences in the participants. Reference Farmer, McGuffin and Gottesman48 For instance, negative family history can include undeclared or unknown positive family history of mental illness. We supplemented the interviews with information obtained from clinical records (for the cases) but there are still likely to be familial cases that were missed. It is also possible that family members have a genetic propensity to developing mental health problems but this has not (yet) been phenotypically expressed. Family psychiatric history also captures familial effects of non-genetic origin. Reference van Os, Rutten and Poulton17 However, the shared familial (non-genetic) component of schizophrenia risk is estimated to account for just a small proportion of the overall trait variance (4.5–11%). Reference Lichtenstein, Yip, Björk, Pawitan, Cannon and Sullivan10

Unfortunately, it was not possible in the current study to adopt more sensitive measures of genetic risk. Reference Iyegbe, Campbell, Butler, Ajnakina and Sham49 Consequently, the impact of familial liability in this sample might have been underestimated and replication using more comprehensive molecular measures of genetic risk is needed. However, very large samples are required to identify sufficient common SNPs to explain a reasonable proportion of the genetic architecture of psychotic disorders and polygenic risk scores may not get us closer to understanding the specific mechanisms involved in G × E. Reference Jaffee and Price22 A recent study showed that the excess risk of offspring having schizophrenia in families affected by psychotic, bipolar affective or other psychiatric disorder is essentially unchanged when SNP-based variation is taken into account. Reference Agerbo, Mortensen, Wiuf, Pedersen, McGrath and Hollegaard50 This provides some reassurance that the data obtained in the current study on psychiatric disorder in first-degree relatives had an adequate degree of accuracy. Nonetheless, future research using larger clinical samples and exploring whether measured genes moderate the impact of childhood adversity on the onset and course of psychotic disorders would be beneficial.

Clinical implications and further directions of research

Our results have implications for both clinical and research practices. Given that several forms of childhood adversity have been shown in the present study to be associated with psychotic disorder regardless of the presence or absence of familial liability, preventing trauma occurring or helping children to cope better in the aftermath of exposure could potentially help to prevent the onset of psychosis. Indeed, as recently shown by Kelleher et al, Reference Kelleher, Keeley, Corcoran, Ramsay, Wasserman and Carli51 the cessation of exposure to traumatic experiences might lead to a reduction in the incidence of psychotic experiences. Therefore, interventions focused on stopping childhood adversity or dealing with its consequences might have an impact not only on preventing the onset of psychosis but also on its longer-term course. Furthermore, research has shown that if the caregiver is perceived as unavailable, unresponsive and insensitive, this could lead to the development of an insecure attachment style in the child and to the child experiencing difficulties in relating to others. Reference Mathews, Onwumere, Bissoli, Ruggeri, Kuipers and Valmaggia52 Therefore, interventions focused on helping parents with psychosis and other severe mental health problems to develop better relationships with their families and/or providing family education and support could improve their children's attachment relationships and in turn, help children develop more positive relationships with others in adulthood. Reference Mathews, Onwumere, Bissoli, Ruggeri, Kuipers and Valmaggia52 Increased social networks and perceived support may reduce the likelihood of such children developing psychosis Reference Gayer-Anderson and Morgan53 and warrants further investigation.

Acknowledgements

The authors gratefully acknowledge the help of the GAP study team, all study participants, South London & Maudsley Mental Health NHS Trust, Dr Paddy Power, consultant psychiatrist, Lambeth Early Onset Psychosis team, the UK National Institute of Health Research Biomedical Research Centre and the Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King's College London.

Funding

The study was funded by the Maudsley Charitable Fund and a UK National Institute of Health Research Specialist Biomedical Research Centre for Mental Health grant (BRC–SLAM). Helen L. Fisher was supported by a UK Medical Research Council (MRC) Population Health Scientist fellowship (G1002366) and Antonella Trotta by the Psychiatry Research Trust. None of the funding bodies had any further role in the collection, analysis or interpretation of data, the writing of this manuscript, or the decision to submit this manuscript for publication.

Footnotes

Declaration of interest

None.

References

1 Arseneault, L, Cannon, M, Fisher, HL, Polanczyk, G, Moffitt, TE, Caspi, A. Childhood trauma and children's emerging psychotic symptoms: a genetically sensitive longitudinal cohort study. Am J Psychiatry 2011; 168: 6572.Google Scholar
2 Kelleher, I, Harley, M, Lynch, F, Arseneault, L, Fitzpatrick, C, Cannon, M. Associations between childhood trauma, bullying and psychotic symptoms among a school-based adolescent sample. Br J Psychiatry 2008; 193: 378–82.Google Scholar
3 Wolke, D, Lereya, ST, Fisher, HL, Lewis, G, Zammit, S. Bullying in elementary school and psychotic experiences at 18 years: a longitudinal, population-based cohort study. Psychol Med 2014; 44: 2199–211.Google Scholar
4 Bechdolf, A, Thompson, A, Nelson, B, Cotton, S, Simmons, MB, Amminger, GP, et al. Experience of trauma and conversion to psychosis in an ultra-high-risk (prodromal) group. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2010; 121: 377–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
5 Fisher, H, Jones, P, Fearon, P, Craig, TK, Dazzan, P, Morgan, K, et al. The varying impact of type, timing and frequency of exposure to childhood adversity on its association with adult psychotic disorder. Psychol Med 2010; 40: 1976–8.Google Scholar
6 Bebbington, P, Jonas, S, Kuipers, E, King, M, Cooper, C, Brugha, T, et al. Childhood sexual abuse and psychosis: data from a cross-sectional national psychiatric survey in England. Br J Psychiatry 2011; 199: 2937.Google Scholar
7 Trotta, A, Di Forti, M, Mondelli, V, Dazzan, P, Pariante, C, David, A, et al. Prevalence of bullying victimisation amongst first-episode psychosis patients and unaffected controls. Schizophr Res 2013; 150: 169–75.Google Scholar
8 van Winkel, R, Stefanis, NC, Myin-Germeys, I. Psychosocial stress and psychosis. A review of the neurobiological mechanisms and the evidence for gene-stress interaction. Schizophr Bull 2008; 34: 1095–105.Google Scholar
9 Sullivan, PF. The genetics of schizophrenia. PLoS Med 2005; 2: e212.Google Scholar
10 Lichtenstein, P, Yip, BH, Björk, C, Pawitan, Y, Cannon, TD, Sullivan, PF, et al. Common genetic determinants of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder in Swedish families: a population-based study. Lancet 2009; 373: 234–9.Google Scholar
11 Cardno, AG, Gottesman, II. Twin studies of schizophrenia: from bow-and-arrow concordances to star wars Mx and functional genomics. Am J Med Genet 2000; 97: 12–7.3.0.CO;2-U>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
12 Walsh, C, MacMillan, H, Jamieson, E. The relationship between parental psychiatric disorder and child physical and sexual abuse: findings from the Ontario Health Supplement. Child Abuse Negl 2002; 26: 1122.Google Scholar
13 Walder, DJ, Faraone, SV, Glatt, SJ, Tsuang, MT, Seidman, LJ. Genetic liability, prenatal health, stress and family environment: risk factors in the Harvard Adolescent Family High Risk for Schizophrenia Study. Schizophr Res 2014; 157: 142–8.Google Scholar
14 Asarnow, RF, Nuechterlein, KH, Fogelson, D, Subotnik, KL, Payne, DA, Russell, AT, et al. Schizophrenia and schizophrenia-spectrum personality disorders in the first-degree relatives of children with schizophrenia: the UCLA family study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2001; 58: 581–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
15 Polanczyk, G, Moffitt, TE, Arseneault, L, Cannon, M, Ambler, A, Keefe, RS, et al. Etiological and clinical features of childhood psychotic symptoms: results from a birth cohort. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2010; 67: 328–38.Google Scholar
16 Plomin, R, DeFries, JC, Loehlin, JC. Genotype-environment interaction and correlation in the analysis of human behaviour. Psychol Bull 1977; 84: 309–22.Google Scholar
17 van Os, J, Rutten, BPF, Poulton, R. Gene-environment interactions in schizophrenia: review of epidemiological findings and future directions. Schizophr Bull 2008; 34: 1066–82.Google Scholar
18 Carter, JW, Schulsinger, F, Parnas, J, Cannon, T, Mednick, SA. A multivariate prediction model of schizophrenia. Schlzophr Bull 2002; 28: 649–82.Google Scholar
19 van Os, J, Sham, P. Gene-environment interactions. In The Epidemiology of Schizophrenia (eds Murray, RM, Jones, PB, Susser, E, Van Os, J, Cannon, M): 235–54. Cambridge University Press, 2003.Google Scholar
20 Tienari, P, Wynne, LC, Läksy, K, Moring, J, Nieminen, P, Sorri, A, et al. Genetic boundaries of the schizophrenia spectrum: evidence from the Finnish Adoptive Family Study of Schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 2003; 160: 1587–94.Google Scholar
21 McGrath, JJ, Mortensen, PB, Visscher, PM, Wray, NR. Where GWAS and epidemiology meet: opportunities for the simultaneous study of genetic and environmental risk factors in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 2013; 39: 955–9.Google Scholar
22 Jaffee, SR, Price, TS. The implications of genotype-environment correlation for establishing causal processes in psychopathology. Dev Psychopathol 2012; 24: 1253–64.Google Scholar
23 Alemany, S, Goldberg, X, van Winkel, R, Gastó, C, Peralta, V, Fañanás, L. Childhood adversity and psychosis: examining whether the association is due to genetic confounding using a monozygotic twin differences approach. Eur Psychiatry 2013; 28: 207–12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
24 Heins, M, Simons, C, Lataster, T, Pfeifer, S, Versmissen, D, Lardinois, M, et al. Childhood trauma and psychosis: a case-control and case-sibling comparison across different levels of genetic liability, psychopathology, and type of trauma. Am J Psychiatry 2011; 168: 1286–94.Google Scholar
25 Pfeifer, S, Krabbendam, L, Myin-Germeys, I, Derom, C, Wichers, M, Jacobs, N, et al. A cognitive intermediate phenotype study confirming possible gene-early adversity interaction in psychosis outcome: a general population twin study. Psychosis 2010; 2: 111.Google Scholar
26 Wigman, JT, van Winkel, R, Ormel, J, Verhulst, FC, van Os, J, Vollebergh, WA. Early trauma and familial risk in the development of the extended psychosis phenotype in adolescence. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2012; 126: 266–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
27 Fisher, HL, McGuffin, P, Boydell, J, Fearon, P, Craig, TK, Dazzan, P, et al. Interplay between childhood physical abuse and familial risk in the onset of psychotic disorders. Schizophr Bull 2014; 40: 1443–51.Google Scholar
28 World Health Organization. The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders. Clinical Description and Diagnostic Guidelines. World Health Organization, 1992.Google Scholar
29 World Health Organization. Schedules for Clinical Assessment in Neuropsychiatry (SCAN). World Health Organization, 1994.Google Scholar
30 Bebbington, P, Nayani, T. The psychosis screening questionnaire. J Methods Psychiatr Res 1995; 5: 1120.Google Scholar
31 Mallett, R. Sociodemographic Schedule. Section of Social Psychiatry, Institute Pof Psychiatry, 1997.Google Scholar
32 Bifulco, A, Bernazzani, O, Moran, PM, Jacobs, C. The Childhood Experiences of Care and Abuse Questionnaire (CECA.Q) – validation in a community series. Br J Clin Psychol 2005; 44: 563–81.Google Scholar
33 Smith, N, Lam, D, Bifulco, A, Checkley, S. Childhood Experience of Care and Abuse Questionnaire (CECA.Q): validation of a screening instrument for childhood adversity in clinical populations. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 2002; 37: 572–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
34 Fisher, HL, Craig, TK, Fearon, P, Morgan, K, Dazzan, P, Lappin, J, et al. Reliability and comparability of psychosis patients' retrospective reports of childhood abuse. Schizophr Bull 2011; 37: 546–53.Google Scholar
35 Knol, MJ, van der Tweel, I, Grobbee, DE, Numans, ME, Geerlings, MI. Estimating interaction on an additive scale between continuous determinants in a logistic regression model. Int J Epidemiol 2007; 36: 1111–8.Google Scholar
36 Schwartz, S, Susser, E. Relationships among causes. In Psychiatric Epidemiology: Searching for the Causes of Mental Disorders (eds Susser, E, Schwartz, S, Morabia, A, Bromet, EJ): 6274. Oxford University Press, 2006.Google Scholar
37 Morgan, C, Reininghaus, U, Fearon, P, Hutchinson, G, Morgan, K, Dazzan, P, et al. Modelling the interplay between childhood and adult adversity in pathways to psychosis: initial evidence from the AESOP study. Psychol Med 2014; 44: 407–19.Google Scholar
38 Morgan, C, Reininghaus, U, Reichenberg, A, Frissa, S; SELCoH study team, Hotopf M, et al. Adversity, cannabis use and psychotic experiences: evidence of cumulative and synergistic effects. Br J Psychiatry 2014; 204: 346–53.Google Scholar
39 Wechsler, D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Third Edition. The Psychological Corporation, 1997.Google Scholar
40 Morgan, C, Kirkbride, J, Leff, J, Craig, T, Hutchinson, G, McKenzie, K, et al. Parental separation, loss and psychosis in different ethnic groups: a case-control study. Psychol Med 2007; 37: 495503.Google Scholar
41 Radford, L, Corral, S, Bradley, C, Collishaw, S, Fisher, HL. The prevalence and impact of child maltreatment and victimisation in the UK: findings from a population survey of children and young people aged 0 to 17 years. Child Abuse Negl 2013; 37: 801–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
42 Della Femina, D, Yeager, CA, Lewis, DC. Child abuse: adolescent records versus adult recall. Child Abuse Negl 1990; 14: 227–31.Google Scholar
43 Hardt, J, Rutter, M. Validity of adult retrospective reports of adverse childhood experiences: review of the evidence. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2004; 45: 260–73.Google Scholar
44 Varese, F, Smeets, F, Drukker, M, Lieverse, R, Lataster, T, Viechtbauer, W, et al. Childhood adversities increase the risk of psychosis: a meta-analysis of patient-control, prospective- and cross-sectional cohort studies. Schizophr Bull 2012; 38: 661–71.Google Scholar
45 Bifulco, A, Brown, GW, Harris, TO. Childhood Experience of Care and Abuse (CECA): a retrospective interview measure. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1994; 35: 1419–35.Google Scholar
46 Di Forti, M, Morgan, C, Dazzan, P, Pariante, C, Mondelli, V, Reis Marques, T, et al. High-potency cannabis and the risk of psychosis. Br J Psychiatry 2009; 195: 488–91.Google Scholar
47 Beards, S, Gayer-Anderson, C, Borges, S, Dewey, ME, Fisher, HL, Morgan, C. Life events and psychosis: a review and meta-analysis. Schizophr Bull 2013; 39: 740–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
48 Farmer, A, McGuffin, P, Gottesman, II. Problems and pitfalls of the family history positive and negative dichotomy: response to Dalén. Schizophr Bull 1990; 16: 367–70.Google Scholar
49 Iyegbe, C, Campbell, D, Butler, A, Ajnakina, O, Sham, P. The emerging molecular architecture of schizophrenia, polygenic risk scores and the clinical implications for GxE research. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 2014; 49: 169–82.Google Scholar
50 Agerbo, E, Mortensen, PB, Wiuf, C, Pedersen, MS, McGrath, J, Hollegaard, MV. Modelling the contribution of family history and variation in single nucleotide polymorphisms to risk of schizophrenia: a Danish national birth cohort-based study. Schizophr Res 2012; 134: 246–52.Google Scholar
51 Kelleher, I, Keeley, H, Corcoran, P, Ramsay, H, Wasserman, C, Carli, V, et al. Childhood trauma and psychosis in a prospective cohort study: cause, effect and directionality. Am J Psychiatry 2013; 170: 734–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
52 Mathews, S, Onwumere, J, Bissoli, S, Ruggeri, M, Kuipers, E, Valmaggia, L. Measuring attachment and parental bonding in psychosis and its clinical implications. Epidemiol Psychiatr Sci 2014; 19: 18.Google Scholar
53 Gayer-Anderson, C, Morgan, C. Social networks, support and early psychosis: a systematic review. Epidemiol Psychiatr Sci 2013; 22: 131–46.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Basic demographic characteristics of psychosis patients and unaffected controls

Figure 1

Table 2 Prevalence of childhood adversity in psychosis patients and unaffected controls

Figure 2

Table 3 Prevalence of familial risk in psychosis patients and unaffected controls

Figure 3

Table 4 Association between parental mental illness and childhood adversity in psychosis patients and unaffected controls

Figure 4

Table 5 The synergistic effects of childhood adversity and familial liability to mental illness on the presence of psychotic disorder

Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.