Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-mlc7c Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T20:08:15.514Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Social functioning and emotion recognition in adults with triple X syndrome

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 February 2021

Maarten Otter*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Neuropsychology, School for Mental Health and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, The Netherlands; Department of Forensic Psychiatry & Mild Intellectual Disabilities, STEVIG, The Netherlands; and Department of Community Mental Health in Mild Intellectual Disabilities, Trajectum, The Netherlands
Peter M. L. Crins
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Neuropsychology, School for Mental Health and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, The Netherlands
Bea C. M. Campforts
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Neuropsychology, School for Mental Health and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, The Netherlands
Constance T. R. M. Stumpel
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical Genetics and School for Oncology and Developmental Biology, Maastricht University Medical Centre, The Netherlands
Thérèse A. M. J. van Amelsvoort
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Neuropsychology, School for Mental Health and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, The Netherlands
Claudia Vingerhoets
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Neuropsychology, School for Mental Health and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, The Netherlands; Heeren Loo Zorggroep, The Netherlands; and Department of Radiology and Nuclear Medicine, Amsterdam University Medical Centre, The Netherlands
*
Correspondence: Dr Maarten Otter. Email: m.otter@maastrichtuniversity.nl
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Triple X syndrome (TXS) is caused by aneuploidy of the X chromosome and is associated with impaired social functioning in children; however, its effect on social functioning and emotion recognition in adults is poorly understood.

Aims

The aim of this study was to investigate social functioning and emotion recognition in adults with TXS.

Method

This cross-sectional cohort study was designed to compare social functioning and emotion recognition between adults with TXS (n = 34) and an age-matched control group (n = 31). Social functioning was assessed with the Adult Behavior Checklist and Social Responsiveness Scale for Adults. Emotion recognition was assessed with the Emotion Recognition Task in the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery. Differences were analysed by Mann-Whitney U-test.

Results

Compared with controls, women with TXS scored higher on the Adult Behavior Checklist, including the Withdrawn scale (P < 0.001, effect size 0.4) and Thought Problems scale (P < 0.001, effect size 0.4); and higher on the Social Responsiveness Scale for Adults, indicating impaired social functioning (P < 0.001, effect size 0.5). In addition, women with TXS performed worse on the Emotion Recognition Task, particularly with respect to recognising sadness (P < 0.005, effect size 0.4), fear (P < 0.01, effect size 0.4) and disgust (P < 0.02, effect size 0.3).

Conclusions

Our findings indicate that adults with TXS have a higher prevalence of impaired social functioning and emotion recognition. These results highlight the relevance of sex chromosome aneuploidy as a potential model for studying disorders characterised by social impairments such as autism spectrum disorder, particularly among women.

Type
Papers
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - ND
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is unaltered and is properly cited. The written permission of Cambridge University Press must be obtained for commercial re-use or in order to create a derivative work.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of the Royal College of Psychiatrists

Triple X syndrome (TXS) is a genetic syndrome first described back in 1959 in an infertile woman.Reference Jacobs, Baikie, Court Brown, MacGregor, Maclean and Harnden1 TXS is characterised by a 47, XXX karyotype and has an estimated incidence of 1 in 1000 newborn girls.Reference Otter, Schrander-Stumpel and Curfs2 The phenotype associated with TXS is variable, but is generally mild; therefore, estimates suggest that only 16% of cases are clinically diagnosed.Reference Viuff, Stochholm, Uldbjerg, Nielsen and Gravholt3 Individuals with TXS typically present with tall stature, seizure susceptibility, urogenital abnormalities, reduced fertilityReference Otter, Schrander-Stumpel and Curfs2,Reference Tuke, Ruth, Wood, Beaumont, Tyrrell and Jones4 and an increased prevalence of several psychiatric conditions, including anxiety, depression and psychotic disorders.Reference Otter, Schrander-Stumpel and Curfs2

The majority of women with TXS have a full-scale IQ in the low end of the average range, as well as a verbal IQ that is generally lower than their performance IQ.Reference Otter, Schrander-Stumpel and Curfs2 The behavioural phenotype (increased anxiety, depressive disorder, attention deficits, and delayed speech and language development) and cognitive phenotype (poorer performance on verbal tasks and executive function deficits) are described primarily in children, adolescents and young adults, and the samples were often biased.Reference Otter, Schrander-Stumpel and Curfs2,Reference Skuse, Printzlau and Wolstencroft5 Therefore, how the phenotype develops when these women reach adulthood is largely unknown, although in a small biased sample, emotional problems, increased levels of anxiety, low self-esteem and social problems were reported.Reference Freilinger, Kliegel, Hanig, Oehl-Jaschkowitz, Henn and Meyer6 The phenotype of adults with TXS appears to not be very distinct, illustrated by the fact that they are typically diagnosed after, for example, recurrent abortions or diagnosis of a family member. As a result, cases can go undiagnosed, even in our clinical practice.Reference Otter, Stumpel and van Amelsvoort7 However, case reports of adult women with TXS suggest a higher prevalence of traumatic experience, suicidal tendencies, psychosis and/or autism spectrum disorder (ASD),Reference Otter, Schrander-Stumpel, Didden and Curfs8 and adults with TXS can have a relatively poor socioeconomic position. For example, a Danish study found that women with TXS were significantly less likely to have a partner, become a mother or achieve a higher education compared with a control group; moreover, the authors found that women with TXS were more likely to retire at a younger age compared with the control group.Reference Stochholm, Juul and Gravholt9 Nevertheless, more research into the adult TXS phenotype is clearly warranted.

TXS has also been associated with impaired social functioning, a key characteristic in several neurodevelopmental, psychiatric and neurological disorders.Reference Morel, Peyroux, Leleu, Favre, Franck and Demily10,Reference Cotter, Granger, Backx, Hobbs, Looi and Barnett11 The biological mechanisms underlying impaired social functioning are currently unknown; however, a growing body of evidence suggests that the sex chromosomes may play a role via haploinsufficiency,Reference Raznahan, Parikshak, Chandran, Blumenthal, Clasen and Alexander-Bloch12 differences in the spatial organisation of chromosomes in the nucleus,Reference Jowhar, Shachar, Gudla, Wangsa, Torres and Russ13 or an increased dosage of X-linked and/or autosomal genes.Reference Green, Flash and Reiss14Reference Zhang, Castillo-Morales, Jiang, Zhu, Hu and Urrutia18 Indeed, an increased prevalence of ASD has been reported in disorders resulting from either an increase or decrease in the number of sex chromosomes, including TXS, Klinefelter syndrome (47,XXY), Jacobs syndrome (47,XYY) and Turner (45,X0) syndrome.Reference Skuse, Printzlau and Wolstencroft5,Reference Green, Flash and Reiss14 Therefore, sex chromosome disorders, such as TXS, may serve as valuable genetic models for studying impaired social functioning in ASD. For example, in a multicentre longitudinal study, a total of 200 000 newborn children were screened for a sex chromosome disorder,Reference Robinson, Lubs, Nielsen and Sorensen19Reference Robinson, Bender and Linden22 and unbiased cases of a sex chromosome disorder were followed through young adulthood (for review, see Otter et alReference Otter, Schrander-Stumpel and Curfs2 ), revealing that girls and young women with TXS often have difficulties forming meaningful interpersonal relationships.Reference Robinson, Bender and Linden22 Researchers at one of the participating centres (the University of Edinburgh) studied the psychiatric symptoms and behavioural characteristics in adolescent and young women (≥16 years of age) with TXS and found that four of the 14 young women with TXS had fair, poor or grossly inadequate friendships compared with none of the young women in the control group; in addition, five of the 14 young women with TXS (35.7%) were found to lead an isolated life compared with only 4% of the control group.Reference Götz23 Another participating centre (Denver, Colorado) followed 11 individuals with TXS from birth to adolescence/early adulthood, and reported social reticence in two of these 11 individuals (18.2%) compared with 0% in the control group.Reference Linden, Bender, Harmon, Mrazek and Robinson24

Furthermore, recent studies found that girls with TXS have higher levels of shyness, social dysfunction and social anxiety, and a high prevalence of autism-like features.Reference Wilson, King and Bishop25,Reference Lenroot, Blumenthal, Wallace, Clasen, Lee and Giedd26 Moreover, a recent self-report study, in which adults with TXS used the Symptom Checklist-90 Revised, found that women with TXS have an increased risk of social deficits, although approximately half of the participants did not report any behavioural or social deficitsReference Freilinger, Kliegel, Hanig, Oehl-Jaschkowitz, Henn and Meyer6 ; in contrast, approximately one in 200 women in the general population have ASD.Reference Green, Flash and Reiss14 Moreover, the Symptom Checklist-90-Revised is not considered a suitable tool for diagnosing or ruling out ASD.Reference Derogatis and Unger27 In addition, women with social communication difficulties often find it hard to self-identify these difficulties. Taken together, these studies suggest that the prevalence of social impairments is higher among adults with TXS compared with controls; however, studies performed to date regarding impaired social functioning were not designed specifically to assess ASD and/or limited their reporting to relatively young adults.

The social impairments associated with TXS may be, in part, attributable to a delay in the development of speech and/or language,Reference Otter, Schrander-Stumpel and Curfs2,Reference Skuse, Printzlau and Wolstencroft5,Reference van Elst PC, Otter, Wijnen and Junge28 as well as impaired cognitive processes such as attention and executive functioning,Reference Urbanus, van Rijn and Swaab29 two cognitive domains that have been associated with ASD. However, little is currently known regarding social cognition in adults with TXS. Social cognition encompasses all of the cognitive processes that are required for social functioning. Moreover, the ability to recognise emotions is an important part of social cognition and is required for adequate social functioning, including the ability to form and maintain personal relationships. Although impaired emotion recognition has been reported in other sex chromosome disorders, including Klinefelter and Turner syndrome,Reference Babinet, Rigard, Peyroux, Dragomir, Plotton and Lejeune30Reference Wolstencroft and Skuse32 to date no studies have examined emotion recognition in adults with TXS.

Aim

Given that social impairments and a high prevalence of autism-like traits have been observed in girls and young women with TXS, TXS may serve as a valuable model for studying neurodevelopmental disorders such as ASD. Therefore, the aim of this study was to examine social functioning in adults with TXS by assessing a broad range of social behaviours generally associated with ASD, including social motivation, social communication, social awareness and rigidity, and repetitive behaviours; in addition, we examined whether women with TXS have impaired social cognition. We therefore tested the hypothesis that women with TXS have a lower level of social functioning and reduced emotion recognition compared with age-matched controls.

Method

Ethical statement

All procedures in this study were performed in accordance with the ethical standards established by the respective national and institutional committees regarding human experimentation and in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. In addition, all procedures involving human subjects were approved by the medical ethics committee at Maastricht University Medical Centre and Maastricht University (approval number: NL46871.068.14/METC143051). Written informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Participants

A total of 65 adult women aged 18–63 years participated in the study; 34 women had TXS (defined as a 47,XXX chromosomal composition determined by conventional karyotyping), and 31 were age-matched controls. To be eligible to participate in this study, participants had to be capable and competent to give informed consent, and had to be sufficiently proficient in the Dutch language.

All 65 participants were White. In the TXS group, ten of the women were diagnosed prenatally, whereas the remaining 24 women were diagnosed postnatally because of the following indications: infertility/recurrent abortions (n = 9), atypical development (n = 6), a family member with a genetic condition (n = 4), small head (n = 2), intestinal malformation (n = 1), nuchal oedema (n = 1) and epicanthal folds (n = 1).

Recruitment and assessment

Participants with TXS were recruited through flyers, digital newsletters, social media, a TXS support group in The Netherlands, advertising and via the Department of Clinical Genetics at Maastricht University Medical Centre. Healthy age-matched controls were recruited through the families and friends of women with TXS and through advertising. Where possible, all assessments were performed within 1 day.

Study design and setting

This cross-sectional cohort study including an age-matched control group was conducted at the University of Maastricht in Maastricht, The Netherlands.

Instruments

Full-scale intelligence quotient

Full-scale IQ was estimated with a shortened version of the Dutch Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, Third Edition.Reference Velthorst, Levine, Henquet, de Haan, van Os and Myin-Germeys33

Social functioning

The Adult Behavior Checklist (ABCL) evaluates 134 behaviour problem items in the preceding 6 months. Behaviour problem statements were scored on a three-level rating scale (‘absolutely not true’, ‘somewhat or sometimes true’ or ‘very true or often true’) by someone who knows the participant well.Reference Achenbach and Rescorla34,Reference Tenneij and Koot35 The Withdrawn and Thought Problems scales were shown in a Dutch study to provide a reliable measure of ASD.Reference Tenneij and Koot35 Because norms differ between participants aged 18–35 years and participants aged 35–59 years, we present T-scores and three ranges (normal, borderline and clinical), with higher scores representing increased levels of behavioural problems.

A Dutch translation of the informant/observer version of the Social Responsiveness Scale for Adults (SRS-A) was used to screen for ASD-related social problems; this translated version has been shown to have psychometric properties comparable to the original version.Reference Chan, Smith, Hong, Greenberg and Mailick36,Reference Constantino, Gruber, Noens, De la Marche and Scholte37 For our study, we chose the informant version of the SRS-A, as women with social communication difficulties often find it hard to self-identify these difficulties, and therefore often underestimate their own level of social difficulties. The SRS-A has 64 questions divided among the following four subscales: social motivation, social communication, social awareness and rigidity, and repetitive behaviour.Reference Constantino, Gruber, Noens, De la Marche and Scholte37,Reference Constantino and Gruber38 The SRS-A was completed by a family member, partner or significant other. Symptoms were rated on a four-point Likert scale, with scores ranging from 0 (not true) to 3 (almost always true), and T-scores are presented. A higher score represents more difficulties in social responsiveness. Seventeen of the 64 items required reverse coding.Reference Constantino, Gruber, Noens, De la Marche and Scholte37,Reference Constantino and Gruber38 The T-scores reflect the following levels of clinical severity: high functioning (T-score 0–39), normal functioning (T-scores 40–59), mild-to-moderate deficits (T-scores 60–74) and severe deficits (T-scores ≥75).Reference Constantino, Gruber, Noens, De la Marche and Scholte37,Reference Constantino and Gruber38 Although the SRS-A is not considered a diagnostic instrument, severe deficits on this scale are often associated with a diagnosis of autism in clinical practice.Reference De la Marche, Noens, Luts, Scholte, Van Huffel and Steyaert39

Emotion recognition

The Emotion Recognition Task (ERT) in the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery for Windows (Cambridge Cognition, Cambridge, UK; see www.cantab.com) was used to assess the participants’ ability to identify the following six emotions: happiness, sadness, anger, disgust, fear and surprise. During the ERT, the participants were shown two series of 90 facial expressions depicting various intensities of these six emotions (Figs 1 and 2), with 15 levels of intensity per emotion. The task procedure was as follows: a fixation cross was displayed in the centre of an otherwise black screen for 1.5 to 2.5 s, then a stimulus showing a face depicting one of six possible emotions was shown briefly (200 ms). The stimulus was then masked with a speckled grey rectangle for 250 ms. After masking, six buttons were displayed and the participant had to choose the button which they thought most closely corresponded to the shown emotion. Participants were instructed not to overthink and to respond as quickly as possible. Correct identifications of the separate emotions and total correct identifications were recorded. The ERT was performed as described previously.40

Fig. 1 The Emotion Recognition Task (ERT) from CANTAB. Permission to replicate this figure has been given to the authors by Cambridge Cognition Ltd.

Fig. 2 Three out of 15 intensity levels from the Emotion Recognition Task (ERT). Permission to replicate this figure has been given to the authors by Cambridge Cognition Ltd.

Statistical analyses

Normally distributed data (age, full-scale IQ score and ERT total score) were compared with the Student's t-test. Non-normally distributed data (ABCL T-score, SRS-A T-score and ERT data) were compared with the Mann-Whitney U-test. Categorical data were analysed by Fisher's exact test. Spearman's rank correlation coefficients were calculated between the total SRS-A scores and full-scale IQ scores.

ERT scores were transformed into standardised Z-scores to identify potential outliers defined as a Z-score smaller than –3 or larger than 3; no outliers were identified. Spearman's rank correlation coefficients were also calculated between the total ERT score and age, and between the total ERT score and full-scale IQ. In addition, an analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was performed to examine the effect of full-scale IQ on ERT performance. Spearman's rank correlation coefficients were also calculated between the social motivation subscale of the SRS-A and total ERT score.

Differences between the women who were diagnosed with TXS prenatally and the women who were diagnosed with TXS postnatally were also analysed, to determinate the level of ascertainment bias between these two groups. These differences were analysed with the Student's t-test (full-scale IQ), the Mann-Whitney U-test (SRS-A total score) and an ANCOVA (ERT total score, controlling for full-scale IQ as a potential confounder).

All statistical analyses were two-tailed and were performed with Stata for Mac version 13.1 (StataCorp, College Station, Texas). Variables that were normally distributed are reported as the mean, s.d. and 95% confidence interval; variables that were non-normally distributed are reported as the median and interquartile range (IQR). Differences with a P-value < 0.05 were considered significant.

Results

We found no significant difference in age between the TXS group and the control group (Table 1). In contrast, we found that full-scale IQ was significantly lower in the TXS group compared with the control group (Table 1).

Table 1 Summary of the study participants

a. Student's t-test.

b. Data in the Triple X group are based on n = 32.

Social functioning

The ABCL T-scores were significantly higher in the TXS group (n = 33) compared with the control group (n = 31) on the Withdrawn syndrome scale (median 64, IQR 55–69 v. median 55, IQR 50–61, respectively; P < 0.001, effect size 0.4) and the Thought Problems syndrome scale (median 59, IQR 54–66 v. median 50, IQR 50–56, respectively; P < 0.001, effect size 0.4). Similar results were obtained when we excluded the two participants who were >59 years of age (data not shown). With respect to the results of the Withdrawn syndrome scale, 60.6%, 15.2% and 24.2% of the women in the TXS group scored in the normal, borderline and clinical range, respectively, compared with 90.3%, 9.7% and 0.0%, respectively, in the control group (P < 0.01). With respect to the results of the Thought Problems syndrome scale, 66.7%, 24.2% and 9.1% of the women in the TXS group scored in the normal, borderline and clinical range, respectively, compared with 96.8%, 3.2% and 0.0%, respectively, in the control group (P < 0.01).

The SRS-A T-scores are summarised in Table 2. We found that the women in the TXS group had significantly higher scores on all four SRS-A subscales compared with the control group, with medium effect sizes. With respect to functioning, we found that 62.5% of the women in the TXS group were in either the high-functioning or normal-functioning range compared with 96.8% of the control group; in contrast, 37.5% of the women in the TXS group had either mild/moderate or severe deficits compared with only 3.2% of the control group (P = 0.002; Table 3).

Table 2 Summary of SRS-A T-scores in the TXS group and control group

SRS-A, Social Responsiveness Scale for Adults; TXS, triple X syndrome; IQR, interquartile range.

a. Mann-Whitney U-test.

b. Effect size was calculated with the equation: $r = Z/\surd N$.

Table 3 Distribution of SRS-A group scores in the TXS and control groups

SRS-A, Social Responsiveness Scale for Adults; TXS, triple X syndrome.

a. Defined as a T-score of 0–39.

b. Defined as a T-score of 40–59.

c. Defined as a T-score of 60–74.

d. Defined as a T-score of ≥75.

Relationship between full-scale IQ and SRS-A score

We found no significant correlation between full-scale IQ and total SRS-A score in either the TXS group (rs = –0.30, P = 0.11) or the control group (rs = –0.08, P = 0.69).

Emotion recognition

The results of the ERT are summarised in Table 4. Overall, we found that the women in the TXS group performed significantly worse than controls at recognising emotions, with mean ± s.d. scores of 100.8 ± 20.5 (95% CI 93.6–108.1) and 115.2 ± 16.0 (95% CI 109.3–121.0), respectively (P = 0.0028). When we examined each emotion separately, we found that the women in the TXS group were significantly worse than the control group at recognising sadness, fear and disgust, but were similar to the control group at recognising happiness, surprise and anger.

Table 4 Summary of the number of correctly identified emotions in the TXS and control groups

TXS, triple X syndrome; IQR, interquartile range.

a. Mann-Whitney U-test.

b. Effect size was calculated with the equation: $r = Z/\surd N$.

An ANCOVA of ERT total scores, with full-scale IQ as a covariate, confirmed that the women in the TXS group performed worse than the control group at recognising emotion (F(1,60) = 7.143, P = 0.010, η 2 = 0.106). Moreover, full-scale IQ did not significantly affect overall performance on the ERT (F(1,60) = 1.00, P = 0.32, η 2 = 0.016).

Our analysis also revealed a significant correlation between ERT total score and age in both the TXS group (rs = –0.48, P = 0.0044) and the control group (rs = –0.47, P = 0.0072). In contrast, we found a significant correlation between ERT total score and full-scale IQ in the TXS group (rs = 0.36, P = 0.045), but not in the control group (rs = 0.08, P = 0.65).

Relationship between SRS-A and ERT score

Lastly, we found a significant correlation between the SRS-A social motivation subscale and ERT total score in the TXS group (rs = 0.51, P = 0.004), but not in the control group (rs = –0.10, P = 0.59).

Explorative comparison between the prenatally diagnosed and postnatally diagnosed women in the TXS group

Finally, we compared the women in the TXS group who were diagnosed prenatally with those who were diagnosed postnatally. We found that these two subgroups did not differ with respect to full-scale IQ (mean values of 86.7 ± 7.6 (95% CI 80.8–92.5) v. 85.9 ± 11.5 (95% CI 80.8–90.9), respectively; P = 0.85); SRS-A total score (median values of 56.5 (IQR 52–60) v. 59.5 (IQR 50.5–65.5), respectively; P = 0.63); or total ERT score (F(2,29) = 3.48, P = 0.09).

Discussion

Here, we report the results of the first study designed to investigate social functioning and emotion recognition in adults with TXS.Reference Skuse, Printzlau and Wolstencroft5,Reference Green, Flash and Reiss14 Consistent with our hypothesis, we found that women with TXS have reduced social functioning compared with a control group. We found that the women in the TXS group performed significantly worse than the control group with respect to recognising emotions, particularly sadness, fear and disgust. We found no significant correlation between full-scale IQ and total SRS-A score.

Our results are consistent with previous reports that girls with TXS can experience social impairments, have inadequate social skills and live a relatively isolated life.Reference Götz23,Reference Linden, Bender, Harmon, Mrazek and Robinson24 Thus, these findings suggest that TXS could be a risk factor for developing autism and/or autism-related conditions with impaired social functioning.

Previous studies analysed the data separately between prenatally diagnosed TXS and postnatally diagnosed TXS.Reference Wilson, King and Bishop25,Reference Robinson, Bender and Linden41,Reference Wigby, D'Epagnier, Howell, Reicks, Wilson and Cordeiro42 In our study, we analysed all of the women with TXS group as a single group, for several reasons. We did not find differences in full-scale IQ, social responsiveness and emotion recognition between prenatally and postnatally diagnosed women. Therefore, we choose to report the results for the complete TXS sample, to increase statistical power.

Importantly, we estimated that the level of bias was relatively low in our sample, although (as in all studies involving sex chromosome disorders) our results may have been influenced to some extent by ascertainment bias,Reference Skuse, Printzlau and Wolstencroft5,Reference Wilson, King and Bishop25 given that higher functioning patients are often not clinically identified. Nevertheless, 82.4% of the women in our TXS group were diagnosed for a reason other than behavioural problems or learning difficulties. Although many studies have a general risk of bias in favour of healthier participants, leading to an under-recruitment of participants with social impairmentsReference Wilson, King and Bishop25 and/or reduced social functioning,Reference Tuke, Ruth, Wood, Beaumont, Tyrrell and Jones4,Reference Fry, Littlejohns, Sudlow, Doherty, Adamska and Sprosen43 our results are similar to previous reports with respect to impaired social functioningReference Götz23,Reference Linden, Bender, Harmon, Mrazek and Robinson24 and reduced full-scale IQReference Robinson, Bender and Linden22,Reference Ratcliffe, Murray and Teague44 in unbiased groups with TXS, and therefore likely represent impaired social functioning among adults with TXS. Thus, we conclude that our sample reliably reflects differences in social impairments between women with and without TXS in the general population.

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study designed to examine emotion recognition in adults with TXS, although emotion recognition and face processing have been extensively studied in ASD, albeit with conflicting results. For example, compared with controls, individuals with ASD can have an atypical gaze pattern; whereas most individuals fixate on the eyes, individuals with ASD generally gaze at other parts of the face, such as the nose or mouth.Reference Webb, Neuhaus and Faja45 Similarly, individuals with Turner syndrome often fixate on the mouth region rather than on the eyes, particularly when looking at a face expressing fear.Reference Mazzola, Seigal, MacAskill, Corden, Lawrence and Skuse46 It is therefore possible that women with TXS may also fixate on regions of the face other than the eyes, thus affecting their ability to recognise specific emotions as sadness and fear. In addition, being confronted with a sad or fearful face could result in gaze avoidance by individuals who may be sensitive, easily hurt emotionally and/or easily upset, attributes that that have been associated with TXS.Reference Otter, Schrander-Stumpel and Curfs2 Viewing disgust in a face can also affect individuals with low self-esteem, another property that has been reported in TXS.Reference Otter, Schrander-Stumpel and Curfs2,Reference Freilinger, Kliegel, Hanig, Oehl-Jaschkowitz, Henn and Meyer6 Finally, increased feelings of depression and/or anxiety can cause a bias toward identifying negative emotions such as sadness and fear, particularly during a forced choice task paradigm such as the ERT used in our study.Reference Webb, Neuhaus and Faja45

Interestingly, we found an association between improved performance on the ERT and a higher level of impairment in the social motivation subdomain of the SRS-A in the TXS group, but not in the control group. Moreover, we found that the ability to recognise emotions appeared to be age-dependent in both the TXS group and the control group, with performance on the ERT decreasing with age.

Our finding that emotion recognition decreases with increasing levels of social motivation in the TXS group was somewhat unexpected, although such an association has been described previously.Reference Webb, Neuhaus and Faja45,Reference Garman, Spaulding, Webb, Mikami, Morris and Lerner47 Abnormal processing of facial expressions has been suggested to be independent of social functioning, as individuals with intact emotion recognition can experience difficulties in social functioning.Reference Webb, Neuhaus and Faja45 The relationship between social motivation and emotion recognition is complex and was studied recently by Garman et al in a relatively small group of individuals with ASD, most of whom were boys.Reference Garman, Spaulding, Webb, Mikami, Morris and Lerner47 The authors found that participants with higher social motivation generally performed worse at recognising emotions, whereas lower social motivation was correlated with improved recognition of emotion, particularly anger, in children's faces. Thus, the relationship between social motivation and emotion recognition is clearly complex, and appears to depend on the participant's age, the emotion being expressed and whether the emotion is expressed by children or adults. Additional study regarding the development of emotion recognition and social motivation is needed to understand our findings and may provide valuable insight into social development in general.

Further study regarding the genetic and other factors that contribute to the clinical variability in TXS may provide new insights into TXS, as well as gender-related differences in ASD in general.Reference Wainer-Katsir and Linial48 In this respect, questions regarding TXS and social functioning warrant further study; for example, the relationship between social functioning and societal functioning and the relationship between social functioning and impaired language and communication, thus providing new treatment options relevant to women with TXS, their families and caregivers.

Strengths and limitations

Although this is the first study to examine social functioning and emotion recognition in adults with TXS, certain limitations must be considered when interpreting the results. First, although the SRS-A is a widely accepted screening instrument for ASD, it is not considered a diagnostic instrument. Therefore, we can only make statements regarding impaired social functioning regardless of the presence of ASD. Second, we did not assess social anxiety or linguistic function, factors that could have affected our measure of impaired social functioning. Third, the ERT contains 90 specific stimuli, with six emotions expressed at 15 different intensities. Although it can be difficult in practice to distinguish among the lowest intensities, during the task the participant is required to select an intensity, which can cause the participant to guess to some degree when presented with stimuli with low intensity. Finally, the ERT was the final task performed in the neuropsychological test battery, which could have led to tiredness and/or frustration, particularly in the women with TXS, thereby affecting the results.Reference Otter, Schrander-Stumpel and Curfs2 We repeat what was discussed above that there is no study on sex chromosome disorders without any risk of bias, including this study.

In conclusion, our results indicate that adults with TXS have impaired social functioning and emotion recognition. Thus, the role of X chromosome aneuploidy in this social impairment should be examined to explain the variability in phenotypic presentation. Nevertheless, TXS may serve as a suitable model for studying the effect of genetics on social cognition and behaviour, particularly in females.

Data availability

The data in this study are not publicly available.

Supplementary material

Supplementary material is available online at https://doi.org/10.1192/bjo.2021.8

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to the Triple X support network in The Netherlands for their support in recruiting participants with TXS and controls, as well as their helpful input when designing this study.

Author contributions

M.O., C.T.R.M.S. and T.A.M.J.v.A. formulated the research questions. M.O. and T.A.M.J.v.A. designed the study. M.O. and B.C.M.C. performed the study. M.O., P.M.L.C. and C.V. analysed the data, and M.O., C.T.R.M.S., T.A.M.J.v.A. and C.V. wrote the manuscript, with input from all other authors.

Funding

This research was not supported by any specific grant from any funding agency, commercial enterprise or not-for-profit sector.

Declaration of interest

None.

ICMJE forms are in the supplementary material, available online at https://doi.org/10.1192/bjo.2021.8

References

Jacobs, PA, Baikie, AG, Court Brown, WM, MacGregor, TN, Maclean, N, Harnden, DG. Evidence for the existence of the human “super female”. Lancet 1959; 274: 423–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Otter, M, Schrander-Stumpel, CTRM, Curfs, LM. Triple X syndrome: a review of the literature. Eur J Hum Genet 2010; 18(3): 265–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Viuff, MH, Stochholm, K, Uldbjerg, N, Nielsen, BB, Danish Fetal Medicine Study G, Gravholt, CH. Only a minority of sex chromosome abnormalities are detected by a national prenatal screening program for Down syndrome. Hum Reprod 2015; 30(10): 2419–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tuke, MA, Ruth, KS, Wood, AR, Beaumont, RN, Tyrrell, J, Jones, SE, et al. Mosaic Turner syndrome shows reduced penetrance in an adult population study. Genet Med 2019; 21(4): 877–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skuse, D, Printzlau, F, Wolstencroft, J. Sex chromosome aneuploidies. Handb Clin Neurol 2018; 147: 355–76.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freilinger, P, Kliegel, D, Hanig, S, Oehl-Jaschkowitz, B, Henn, W, Meyer, J. Behavioral and psychological features in girls and women with triple-X syndrome. Am J Med Genet A 2018; 176(11): 2284–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Otter, M, Stumpel, CTRM, van Amelsvoort, T. Client-centred clinical genetic diagnostics. Adv Ment Health Intell Disabil 2018; 12(1): 110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Otter, M, Schrander-Stumpel, CT, Didden, R, Curfs, LM. The psychiatric phenotype in triple X syndrome: new hypotheses illustrated in two cases. Dev Neurorehabil 2012; 15(3): 233–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stochholm, K, Juul, S, Gravholt, CH. Poor socio-economic status in 47,XXX --an unexpected effect of an extra X chromosome. Eur J Med Genet 2013; 56(6): 286–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morel, A, Peyroux, E, Leleu, A, Favre, E, Franck, N, Demily, C. Overview of social cognitive dysfunctions in rare developmental syndromes with psychiatric phenotype. Front Pediatr 2018; 6: 102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cotter, J, Granger, K, Backx, R, Hobbs, M, Looi, CY, Barnett, JH. Social cognitive dysfunction as a clinical marker: a systematic review of meta-analyses across 30 clinical conditions. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2018; 84: 92–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raznahan, A, Parikshak, NN, Chandran, V, Blumenthal, JD, Clasen, LS, Alexander-Bloch, AF, et al. Sex-chromosome dosage effects on gene expression in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2018; 115(28): 7398–403.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jowhar, Z, Shachar, S, Gudla, PR, Wangsa, D, Torres, E, Russ, JL, et al. Effects of human sex chromosome dosage on spatial chromosome organization. Mol Biol Cell 2018; 29(20): 2458–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, T, Flash, S, Reiss, AL. Sex differences in psychiatric disorders: what we can learn from sex chromosome aneuploidies. Neuropsychopharmacology 2019; 44(1): 921.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bishop, DV, Jacobs, PA, Lachlan, K, Wellesley, D, Barnicoat, A, Boyd, PA, et al. Autism, language and communication in children with sex chromosome trisomies. Arch Dis Child 2011; 96(10): 954–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Navarro-Cobos, MJ, Balaton, BP, Brown, CJ. Genes that escape from X-chromosome inactivation: potential contributors to Klinefelter syndrome. Am J Med Genet C Semin Med Genet 2020; 184(2): 226–38.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hong, DS, Hoeft, F, Marzelli, MJ, Lepage, JF, Roeltgen, D, Ross, J, et al. Influence of the X-chromosome on neuroanatomy: evidence from Turner and Klinefelter syndromes. J Neurosci 2014; 34(10): 3509–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhang, Y, Castillo-Morales, A, Jiang, M, Zhu, Y, Hu, L, Urrutia, AO, et al. Genes that escape X-inactivation in humans have high intraspecific variability in expression, are associated with mental impairment but are not slow evolving. Mol Biol Evol 2013; 30(12): 2588–601.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, A, Lubs, HA, Nielsen, J, Sorensen, K. Summary of clinical findings: profiles of children with 47,XXY, 47,XXX and 47,XYY karyotypes. Birth Defects Orig Artic Ser 1979; 15(1): 261–6.Google ScholarPubMed
Stewart, DA, Netley, CT, Park, E. Summary of clinical findings of children with 47,XXY, 47,XYY, and 47,XXX karyotypes. Birth Defects Orig Artic Ser 1982; 18(4): 15.Google ScholarPubMed
Netley, CT. Summary overview of behavioural development in individuals with neonatally identified X and Y aneuploidy. Birth Defects Orig Artic Ser 1986; 22(3): 293306.Google ScholarPubMed
Robinson, A, Bender, BG, Linden, MG. Summary of clinical findings in children and young adults with sex chromosome anomalies. Birth Defects Orig Artic Ser 1990; 26(4): 225–8.Google Scholar
Götz, M. The Psychiatric Consequences of Sex Chromosome Abnormalities: A Cohort Study. University of Edinburgh, 1996.Google Scholar
Linden, MG, Bender, BG, Harmon, RJ, Mrazek, DA, Robinson, A. 47,XXX: what is the prognosis? Pediatrics 1988; 82(4): 619–30.Google ScholarPubMed
Wilson, AC, King, J, Bishop, DVM. Autism and social anxiety in children with sex chromosome trisomies: an observational study. Wellcome Open Res 2019; 4(32): 32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lenroot, RK, Blumenthal, JD, Wallace, GL, Clasen, LS, Lee, NR, Giedd, JN. A case-control study of brain structure and behavioral characteristics in 47,XXX syndrome. Genes Brain Behav 2014; 13(8): 841–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Derogatis, LR, Unger, R. Symptom Checklist-90-Revised. The Corsini Encyclopedia of Psychology. John Wiley & Sons, 2010.Google Scholar
van Elst PC, , Otter, M, Wijnen, F, Junge, C. Evaluating the Scope of Language Impairments in a Patient with Triple X Syndrome: A Brief Report. Dev Neurorehabil 2020; 23(6): 402406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Urbanus, E, van Rijn, S, Swaab, H. A review of neurocognitive functioning of children with sex chromosome trisomies: identifying targets for early intervention. Clin Genet 2020; 97(1): 156–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Babinet, MN, Rigard, C, Peyroux, E, Dragomir, AR, Plotton, I, Lejeune, H, et al. [Social cognition disorders in Klinefelter syndrome: a specific phenotype? (KS)]. Encephale 2017; 43(5): 423–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Rijn, S, Swaab, H, Aleman, A, Kahn, RS. X Chromosomal effects on social cognitive processing and emotion regulation: a study with Klinefelter men (47,XXY). Schizophr Res 2006; 84(2-3): 194203.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wolstencroft, J, Skuse, D. Social skills and relationships in Turner syndrome. Curr Opin Psychiatry 2019; 32(2): 8591.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Velthorst, E, Levine, SZ, Henquet, C, de Haan, L, van Os, J, Myin-Germeys, I, et al. To cut a short test even shorter: reliability and validity of a brief assessment of intellectual ability in schizophrenia--a control-case family study. Cogn Neuropsychiatry 2013; 18(6): 574–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Achenbach, TM, Rescorla, LA. Manual for the ASEBA Adult Forms & Profiles. University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families, 2003.Google Scholar
Tenneij, NH, Koot, HM. A preliminary investigation into the utility of the Adult Behavior Checklist in the assessment of psychopathology in people with low IQ. J Appl Res Intellect Disabil 2007; 20(5): 391400.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chan, W, Smith, LE, Hong, J, Greenberg, JS, Mailick, MR. Validating the Social Responsiveness Scale for adults with autism. Autism Res 2017; 10(10): 1663–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Constantino, JN, Gruber, CP, Noens, IL, De la Marche, MF, Scholte, EM. SRS-A the Social Responsiveness Scale for Adults (Dutch manual). Hogrefe, 2012.Google Scholar
Constantino, JN, Gruber, CP. Social Responsiveness Scale: Manual. Western Psychological Services, 2005.Google Scholar
De la Marche, W, Noens, I, Luts, J, Scholte, E, Van Huffel, S, Steyaert, J. Quantitative autism traits in first degree relatives: evidence for the broader autism phenotype in fathers, but not in mothers and siblings. Autism 2012; 16(3): 247–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cambridge Cognition, Emotion Recognition Task (ERT). In CANTAB Research Suite 6 Test Administration Guide: Ch. 10. Cambridge Cognition Ltd, 2014.Google Scholar
Robinson, A, Bender, BG, Linden, MG. Prognosis of prenatally diagnosed children with sex chromosome aneuploidy. Am J Med Genet 1992; 44(3): 365–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wigby, K, D'Epagnier, C, Howell, S, Reicks, A, Wilson, R, Cordeiro, L, et al. Expanding the phenotype of Triple X syndrome: a comparison of prenatal versus postnatal diagnosis. Am J Med Genet A 2016; 170(11): 2870–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fry, A, Littlejohns, TJ, Sudlow, C, Doherty, N, Adamska, L, Sprosen, T, et al. Comparison of sociodemographic and health-related characteristics of UK biobank participants with those of the general population. Am J Epidemiol 2017; 186(9): 1026–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ratcliffe, SG, Murray, L, Teague, P. Edinburgh study of growth and development of children with sex chromosome abnormalities. III. Birth Defects Orig Artic Ser 1986; 22(3): 73118.Google ScholarPubMed
Webb, SJ, Neuhaus, E, Faja, S. Face perception and learning in autism spectrum disorders. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 2017; 70(5): 970–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mazzola, F, Seigal, A, MacAskill, A, Corden, B, Lawrence, K, Skuse, DH. Eye tracking and fear recognition deficits in Turner syndrome. Soc Neurosci 2006; 1(3–4): 259–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garman, HD, Spaulding, CJ, Webb, SJ, Mikami, AY, Morris, JP, Lerner, MD. Wanting it too much: an inverse relation between social motivation and facial emotion recognition in autism spectrum disorder. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev 2016; 47(6): 890902.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wainer-Katsir, K, Linial, M. Chromosome X factors and phenotype variability. Obstet Gynecol Rep 2019; 3(1): 13.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1 The Emotion Recognition Task (ERT) from CANTAB. Permission to replicate this figure has been given to the authors by Cambridge Cognition Ltd.

Figure 1

Fig. 2 Three out of 15 intensity levels from the Emotion Recognition Task (ERT). Permission to replicate this figure has been given to the authors by Cambridge Cognition Ltd.

Figure 2

Table 1 Summary of the study participants

Figure 3

Table 2 Summary of SRS-A T-scores in the TXS group and control group

Figure 4

Table 3 Distribution of SRS-A group scores in the TXS and control groups

Figure 5

Table 4 Summary of the number of correctly identified emotions in the TXS and control groups

Supplementary material: File

Otter et al. supplementary material

Table S1

Download Otter et al. supplementary material(File)
File 15.6 KB
Supplementary material: File

Otter et al. supplementary material

Otter et al. supplementary material 2

Download Otter et al. supplementary material(File)
File 7 MB
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.