Published online by Cambridge University Press: 10 September 2018
Common understandings of neuroethics, that is, of its distinctive nature, are premised on two distinct sets of claims: (1) neuroscience can change views about the nature of ethics itself and neuroethics is dedicated to reaping such an understanding of ethics, and (2) neuroscience poses challenges distinct from other areas of medicine and science and neuroethics tackles those issues. Critiques have rightfully challenged both claims, stressing how the first may lead to problematic forms of reductionism whereas the second relies on debatable assumptions about the nature of bioethics specialization and development. Informed by philosophical pragmatism and our experience in neuroethics, we argue that these claims are ill founded and should give way to pragmatist reconstructions; namely, that neuroscience, much like other areas of empirical research on morality, can provide useful information about the nature of morally problematic situations, but does not need to promise radical and sweeping changes to ethics based on neuroscientism. Furthermore, the rationale for the development of neuroethics as a specialized field need not to be premised on the distinctive nature of the issues it tackles or of neurotechnologies. Rather, it can espouse an understanding of neuroethics as both a scholarly and a practical endeavor dedicated to resolving a series of problematic situations raised by neurological and psychiatric conditions.
The writing of this article was supported by a career award from the Fonds de recherche du Québec–Santé (ER) and a grant from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research and Fonds de recherche du Québec–Santé in collaboration with ERANET: NEURON (ER). We would like to thank Sonja Chu for helpful feedback on a previous version of this manuscript and for editorial assistance.
1. Gazzaniga, MS. The Ethical Brain. New York/Washington, D.C.: Dana Press; 2005Google Scholar
2. In this article, we have used the expression “neuroscience of morality” to refer to what many authors describe as a “neuroscience of ethics” (Roskies, A. Neuroethics for the new millenium. Neuron 2002;35:21–23CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed), including some of our previous writing. The reason is that the term “ethics” more appropriately designates an academic field or discipline, and the “neuroscience of ethics” is sometimes misinterpreted as taking this discipline as its object (Racine, E, Dubljevic, V, Jox, RJ, Baertschi, B, Christensen, JF, Farisco, M, et al. Can neuroscience contribute to practical ethics? A critical review and discussion of the methodological and translational challenges of ethics. Bioethics 2017;31(5):328–37CrossRefGoogle Scholar). However, it is more accurate to understand the neuroscience of ethics as referring to a “neuroscience of morality,” because the neuroscience of ethics does not investigate ethics per se but rather the domain of morality such as moral judgments, moral emotions, moral intuitions, and moral behavior. This distinction also helps to state that, from a pragmatist standpoint, the empirical or scientific understanding of morality (with the help of neuroscience, psychology, anthropology, or sociology) is crucial for the task of ethics, but it does not replace the projective and deliberative nature of ethics as an empirically informed effort to resolve morally problematic situations and seek resolution in action.
3. Greene, JD. Social neuroscience and the soul’s last stand. In: Todorov, A, Fiske, S, Prentics, D, eds. Social Neuroscience: Toward Understanding the Underpinnings of the Social Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2006.Google Scholar
4. Changeux, J. Le point de vue d’un neurobiologiste sur les fondements de l’éthique. In: Huber, G, ed. Cerveau et psychisme humains: quelle éthique? Paris: John Libbey Eurotext; 1996:97–109.Google Scholar
5. Changeux, J, Ricoeur, P. Ce qui nous fait penser: la nature et la règle. Paris: Odile Jacob; 2000.Google Scholar
6. Churchland, PS. The Engine of Reason, the Seat of the Soul: A Philosophical Journey into the Brain. Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books/MIT Press; 1995.Google Scholar
7. Churchland, PS. Feeling reasons. In: Churchland, PM, Churchland, PS, eds. On the Contrary. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1998:231–54.Google Scholar
8. Decety, JWT, ed. The Moral Brain: A Multidisciplinary Perspective. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 2015.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
9. Wolpe, PR. Neuroethics. In: Post, SG, ed. The Encyclopedia of Bioethics, 3rd ed. New York: MacMillan Reference; 2004:1894–8.Google Scholar
10. See note 2, Roskies 2002.
11. Zizzo, N, Bell, E, Racine, E. What is everyday ethics? A review and a proposal for an integrative concept. Journal of Clinical Ethics 2016;27:117–28.Google Scholar
12. Zizzo, N, Bell, E, Racine, E. What are the focal points in bioethics literature? Examining the discussions about everyday ethics in Parkinson’s Disease. Clinical Ethics 2016;12(1):19–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
13. Illes, J, Raffin, TA. Neuroethics: An emerging new discipline in the study of brain and cognition. Brain and Cognition 2002;50:341–4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
14. Racine, E. Pragmatic Neuroethics: Improving Treatment and Understanding of the Mind-Brain. Cambridge: MIT Press; 2010.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
15. Racine, E. Interdisciplinary approaches for a pragmatic neuroethics. The American Journal of Bioethics 2008;8:52–3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
16. Andre, J. Bioethics As Practice. Chapel Hill and London: The University of North Carolina Press; 2002.Google Scholar
17. Racine, E, Sample, M. The competing identities of neuroethics: Remarks on theoretical and methodological assumptions and their practical implications for the future of neuroethics. In: Rommelfanger, K, Johnson, S, eds. Handbook of Neuroethics. New York: Routledge; 2017:23–33.Google Scholar
18. Racine, E. Comment on “Does it make sense to speak of neuroethics?” European Molecular Biology Organization Reports 2008;9:2–3.Google Scholar
19. Fins, JJ. A leg to stand on: Sir William Osler and Wilder Penfield’s “Neuroethics.” American Journal of Bioethics 2008;8:37–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
20. See note 14, Racine 2010.
21. See note 2, Roskies 2002.
22. Levy, N. Neuroethics: A new way of doing ethics. American Journal of Bioethics Neuroscience 2011;2:3–9.Google ScholarPubMed
23. See note 4, Changeux 1996.
24. Changeux J. L’homme neuronal. Paris: Hachette;1983.
25. See note 5, Changeux, Ricoeur 2000.
26. Churchland, PS. Neurophilosophy: Toward a Unified Science of the Mind-Brain. Cambridge, MA: Bradford Book/MIT Press;1986.Google Scholar
28. Churchland, PS. Neuroconscience: Reflections on the neural basis of morality. In: Marcus, SJ, ed. Neuroethics: Mapping the Field, Conference Proceedings. San Francisco: The Dana Foundation; 2002:20–6.Google Scholar
29. Churchland, PS. Braintrust. Princeton: Princeton University Press; 2012.Google Scholar
30. Churchland, PM. Eliminative materialism and the propositional attitudes. Journal of Philosophy 1981;77:67–90.Google Scholar
31. Brothers, L. A biological perspective on empathy. American Journal of Psychiatry 1989;146(9):10–9.Google ScholarPubMed
32. Blair, RJR, Colledge, E, Mitchell, DGV. Somatic markers and response reversal: Is there orbitofrontal cortex dysfunction in boys with psychopathic tendencies. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 2001;29:499–511.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
33. Blair, RJR. A cognitive developmental approach to morality: Investigating the psychopath. Cognition 1995;57:1–29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
34. Eslinger, PJ, Damasio, AR. Severe distrubance of higher cognition after bilateral frontal lobe ablation: patient EVR. Neurology 1985;35:1731–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
35. Eslinger, PJ. Neurological and neuropsychological bases of empathy. European Journal of Neurology 1998;39:193–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
36. Damasio, AR. Descartes’ Error. New York: Penguin Putnam Publishers; 1994.Google ScholarPubMed
37. Greene, JD, Sommerville, RB, Nystrom, LE, Darley, JM, Cohen, JD. An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment. Science 2001;293:2105–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
38. Moll, J, Eslinger, PJ, Oliveira-Souza, R. Frontopolar and anterior temporal cortex activation in a moral judgment task: Preliminary functional MRI results in normal subjects. Arquivos de Neuro-Psiquiatria 2001;59:657–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
39. Haidt, J. The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review 2001;108:814–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
40. Cushman, F, Young, L, Greene, JD. Multi-sytem moral psychology. In: Doris, J, ed. The Moral Psychology Handbook. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2010:47–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
41. See note 1, Gazzaniga 2005, at xv.
42. Piperberg, M, Vidal, F. Born free: The theory and practice of neuroethical exceptionalism. In: Racine, E, Aspler, J, eds. Debates about Neuroethics: Perspectives on Its Development, Focus, and Future. New York: Springer; 2017:67–81.Google Scholar
43. See note 2, Racine et al. 2017.
44. Illes, J, Racine, E. Imaging or imagining? A neuroethics challenge informed by genetics. American Journal of Bioethics 2005;5:5–18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
45. Berker, S. The normative insignificance of neuroscience. Philosophy & Public Affairs 2009;37(4):293–329CrossRefGoogle Scholar
46. Rest, JR. A psychologist looks at the teaching of ethics. Hastings Center Report 1982;12:29–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
47. Rest, JR. Moral Development. Advances in Research and Theory. New York: Praeger; 1986.Google Scholar
48. Turiel, E. The Development of Social Knowledge: Morality and Convention. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1983.Google Scholar
49. See note 46, Rest 1982.
50. Racine, E. Identifying challenges and conditions for the use of neuroscience in bioethics. The American Journal of Bioethics - Neuroscience 2007;7:74–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
51. Racine, E, Bar-Ilan, O, Illes, J. fMRI in the public eye. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 2005;6:159–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
52. Morse, S. Brain overclaim syndrome and criminal responsibility: A diagnostic note. Ohio State Journal of Criminal Law 2006;3:397–412.Google Scholar
53. Racine, E, Zimmerman, E. Pragmatic neuroethics and neuroscience’s potential to radically change ethics. In: Littlefield, M, Johnson, MJ, eds. The Neuroscientific Turn: Transdisciplinarity in the Age of the Brain. Ann Arbor: The University of Michigan Press; 2012:135–51.Google Scholar
54. McCabe, DP, Castel, AD. Seeing is believing: The effect of brain images on judgments of scientific reasoning. Cognition 2008;107:343–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
55. Weisberg, DS, Keil, FC, Goodstein, J, Rawson, E, Gray, JR. The seductive allure of neuroscience explanations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 2008;20:470–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
56. Dewey, J. The Quest for Certainty: A Study of the Relation of Knowledge and Action. New York: Milton, Balch & Company; 1929.Google Scholar
57. See note 56, Dewey 1929.
58. Dewey, J. Human Nature and Conduct: An Introduction to Social Psychology. New York: Holt; 1922.Google Scholar
59. Sherwin, S. Foundations, frameworks, lenses: The role of theories in bioethics. Bioethics 1999;13:198–205.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
60. See note 53, Racine, Zimmerman 2012.
61. Racine, E, Waldman, S, Rosenberg, J, Illes, J. Contemporary neuroscience in the media. Social Science and Medicine 2010;71:725–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
62. Vidal, F. Brainhood, anthropological figure of modernity. History of the Human Sciences 2009;22:5–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
63. Racine, E, Illes, J. Neuroethical responsibilities. Canadian Journal of Neurological Sciences 2006;33:269–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
64. Hagner, M. Cultivating the cortex in German neuroanatomy. Science in Context 2001;14:541–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
65. Bedeutung, Vogt O., der Hirnforschung, Ziele und Wege. Nord and Süd. Nord and Süd 1909;36:309 (see note 62, Hagner 2001).Google Scholar
66. See note 64, Hagner 2001.
67. Jasanoff, S, ed. States of Knowledge: The Co-Production of Science and the Social Order. Chicago: Routledge; 2004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
68. See note 63, Racine, Illes 2006. See also Cascio, MA, Racine, E. Person-oriented research ethics: Integrating relational and everyday ethics in research. Accountability in Research 2018;25(3):170–97.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
69. See note 14, Racine 2010.
70. Gordon, AH. Special articles: The patient as a person. The Canadian Medical Association Journal 1934;31(2):191–3.Google Scholar
71. Durand, G. Introduction générale à la bioéthique: histoire, concepts et outils. Montréal: Fides-Cerf; 1999.Google Scholar
72. See note 14, Racine 2010.
73. Stent, GS. The poverty of neurophilosophy. Journal of Medicine and Philosophy 1990;15:539–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
74. Macintyre, A. What can moral philosophers learn from the study of the brain? Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 1998;58:865–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
75. See note 5, Changeux, Ricoeur 2000.
76. Racine, E. Pourquoi et comment doit-on tenir compte des neurosciences en éthique? Esquisse d’une approche neurophilosophique émergentiste et interdisciplinaire. Laval théologique et philosophique 2005;61:77–105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
77. See note 14, Racine 2010.
78. van der Scheer, L,Widdershoven, G. Integrated empirical ethics: Loss of normativity? Medicine, Health Care, and Philosophy 2004;7:71–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
79. Borry, P, Schotsmans, P, Dierickx, K. The birth of the empirical turn in bioethics. Bioethics 2005;19: 49–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
80. See note 58, Dewey 1922.
81. See note 50, Racine 2007.
82. Potter, VR. Bioethics the science of survival. Perspectives in Biology and Medicine 1970;14:127–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
83. Dewey, J. An address delivered before the College of Physicians in St. Louis, April 21, 1937. In: Ratner, J, ed. Intelligence in the Modern World: John Dewey’s Philosophy. New York: The Modern Library; 1939.Google Scholar (From: Gouinlock, J. The Moral Writtings of John Dewey, Amherst, NY: Prometheus Books; 1994, at 48.)Google Scholar
84. See note 13, Illes, Raffin 2002.
85. Shook, J, Giordano, J. Neuroethical engagement on interdisciplinary and international scales. In: Aspler, Racine 2017 (see note 42).Google Scholar
86. See note 9, Wolpe 2004.
87. See note 2, Roskies 2002.
88. Farah, MJ. Neuroethics: A guide for the perplexed. Cerebrum 2004;6:29–38.Google ScholarPubMed
89. See note 88, Farah 2004.
90. Sententia, W. Neuroethical considerations: Cognitive liberty and converging technologies for improving human cognition. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 2004;1013:221–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
91. Parens, E, Johnston, J. Does it make sense to speak of neuroethics? Three problems with keying ethics to hot new science and technology. European Molecular Biology Organization Reports 2007;8(Spec No.):S61–4.Google ScholarPubMed
92. See note 14, Racine 2010.
93. See note 44, Illes, Racine 2005.
94. Turner, L. Bioethic$ Inc. Nature Biotechnology 2004;22:947–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
95. Turner, L. The tyranny of ’genethics’. Nature Biotechnology 2003;21:1282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
96. Turner, L. Bioethics needs to rethink its agenda. British Medical Journal 2004;328:175.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
97. Evans, JH. A sociological account of the growth of principlism. Hastings Center Report 2000;30:37.Google ScholarPubMed
98. Evans, JH. Playing God? Human Genetic Engineering and the Rationalization of Public Bioethical Debate. Chicago: Chicago University Press; 2002.Google Scholar
99. De Vries, R. Framing neuroethics: A sociological assessment of the neuroethical imagination. American Journal of Bioethics 2005;5:25–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
100. De Vries, R. Who will guard the guardians of neuroscience? Firing the neuroethical imagination. European Molecular Biology Organization Reports 2007;8(Spec No.):S65–9.Google ScholarPubMed
101. Parens, E, Johnston, J. Against hyphenated ethics. Bioethics Forum 2006; available at https://www.thehastingscenter.org/against-hyphenated-ethics/ (last accessed 9 May 2018).Google Scholar
102. Pickersgill, M. The social life of the brain: Neuroscience in society. Current Sociology 2013;61(3):322–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
104. Gieryn, TF. Boundary-work and the demarcation of science from non-science: Strains and interests in professional ideologies of scientists. American Sociological Review 1983;781–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
105. See note 16, Andre 2002.
106. See note 14, Racine 2010.
107. Brohan, E, Henderson, C, Wheat, K, Malcolm, E, Clement, S, Barley, EA, et al. Systematic review of beliefs, behaviours and influencing factors associated with disclosure of a mental health problem in the workplace. BioMed Central Psychiatry 2012;12:11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
108. Bell, E, Andrew, G, Di Pietro, N, Chudley, AE, Reynolds, JN, Racine, E. It’s a shame! Stigma against fetal alcohol spectrum disorder: Examining the ethical implications for public health practices and policies. Public Health Ethics 2016;9:65–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
109. Racine, E, Bell, E, Zizzo, N, Green, C. Public discourse on the biology of alcohol addiction: Implications for stigma, self-control, essentialism, and coercive policies in pregnancy. Neuroethics 2015;1–10.Google Scholar
110. World Health Organization. Neurological Disorders: Public Health Challenges. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2006.Google Scholar
111. Racine, E, Lariviere-Bastien, D, Bell, E, Majnemer, A, Shevell, M. Respect for autonomy in the healthcare context: Observations from a qualitative study of young adults with cerebral palsy. Child: Care, Health and Development 2013;39:873–9.Google ScholarPubMed
112. Larivière-Bastien, D, Majnemer, A, Shevell, M, Racine, E. Perspectives of adolescents and young adolescents with cerebral palsy on the ethical and social challenges encountered in healthcare services. Narrative Inquiry on Bioethics: A Journal of Qualitative Research 2011;1:43–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
113. Pontius, AA. Neuro-ethics of “walking” in the newborn. Perceptual and Motor Skills 1973;37:235–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
114. See note 18, Racine 2008.
115. See note 19, Fins 2008.
116. See note 14, Racine 2010.
117. See note 14, Racine 2010.