Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7fkt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-21T09:35:38.693Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A four-year multi-wave prospective study on the role of parental reflective functioning and parenting stress in the development of socio-emotional problems in internationally adopted children

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 November 2022

Saskia Malcorps
Affiliation:
Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
Nicole Vliegen
Affiliation:
Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
Peter Fonagy
Affiliation:
Research Department of Clinical, Educational and Health Psychology, University College London, London, UK
Patrick Luyten*
Affiliation:
Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium Research Department of Clinical, Educational and Health Psychology, University College London, London, UK
*
Corresponding author: Patrick Luyten, email: patrick.luyten@kuleuven.be
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Parental reflective functioning (PRF) plays a protective role in the development of children with histories of early adversity, including adopted children. This is the first study to investigate the developmental trajectories of PRF and children’s socio-emotional problems in the first 4 years after international adoption (N = 48 families, mean age (T1) = 20.7 months) and to examine the mediating role of parenting stress in the relation between PRF and child socio-emotional problems. Multilevel modeling indicated that age at adoption and parent gender moderated the development of PRF and child socio-emotional problems. Moreover, decreases in PRF were associated with more socio-emotional problems in the children. These relations were mediated by parenting stress, and particularly feelings of incompetence and marital dissatisfaction.

Type
Regular Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Reflective functioning (RF), or mentalizing, refers to an individual’s ability to hold another mind in mind, that is, to be able to understand oneself and others in terms of intentional mental states such as feelings, desires, wishes, and attitudes. This capacity allows individuals to perceive both the self and others in terms of mental states, thereby making them meaningful, understandable, and predictable (Fonagy et al., Reference Fonagy, Campbell, Constantinou, Higgitt, Allison and Luyten2021). Parental reflective functioning (PRF) is a specific expression of RF, as it entails the parent’s capacity to envision the child as being motivated by internal mental states, and to be able to reflect on their own internal mental experiences, and how they are shaped and changed by interactions with the child (Slade, Reference Slade2005). PRF has been shown to play an important role in establishing secure attachment relationships between parent and child, and in the socio-emotional development of children more generally (Luyten et al., Reference Luyten, Campbell, Allison and Fonagy2020; Zeegers et al., Reference Zeegers, Colonnesi, Stams and Meins2017). Studies suggest that PRF may be particularly important in buffering the effects of early adversity (Ensink et al., Reference Ensink, Berthelot, Bernazzani, Normandin and Fonagy2014; Huth-Bocks et al., Reference Huth-Bocks, Muzik, Beeghly, Earls and Stacks2014). These findings have led several authors to suggest that the capacity for PRF can have specific relevance for parents of adopted children, as adopted children by definition have a history of early separation from attachment figures, often compounded by additional traumatic experiences related to abuse or neglect (Adkins et al., Reference Adkins, Luyten and Fonagy2018; Midgley et al., Reference Midgley, Alayza, Lawrence and Bellew2018). However, to date, research on PRF and the socio-emotional development of adopted children is scarce (Midgley et al., Reference Midgley, Alayza, Lawrence and Bellew2018).

There is evidence indicating that parenting stress has a negative impact on PRF (Håkansson et al., Reference Håkansson, Watten, Söderström and Øie2019; McMahon & Meins, Reference McMahon and Meins2012). It is a central assumption of the mentalizing approach to psychological development that the ability to mentalize tends to decrease with increasing stress levels (Luyten & Fonagy, Reference Luyten and Fonagy2015). Impairments in mentalizing are considered to typically manifest in high-stress and -arousal contexts, particularly in the context of close attachment relationships such as parent–child relationships. Research has indicated that with increasing stress the neural regions underlying the ability for “controlled” mentalizing tend to become deactivated, and more posterior brain regions responsible for “automatic”, yet biased, processing of social information are activated. Hence, it is hypothesized that with increasing arousal, there is a biobehavioral switch from more controlled mentalizing, characterized by slow, conscious reflection, to more automatic mentalizing, characterized by rapid and often biased assumptions about self and others. Furthermore, early adversity, and insecure attachment experiences more generally, are assumed to influence the point at which an individual switches from controlled to automatic mentalizing. Parenting, and adoptive parenthood in particular, may be an important source of stress for young parents, particularly when parents have the feeling that their child is more “difficult” than expected, leading to negative feelings concerning themselves, their role as parents, their partner relationship, and their adoptive child (Deater-Deckard, Reference Deater-Deckard1998). Consistent with these assumptions, research in adoptive families suggests the existence of bidirectional associations between child development and parenting stress. Several studies have indicated that parenting stress in adoptive parents is negatively associated with the socio-emotional development of adopted children (Melançon et al., Reference Melançon, Cossette, Smith, Beauvais-Dubois, Cyr and Smolla2019). Similarly, features of adopted children (i.e. age at adoption, children’s behavioral difficulties) may contribute to more pronounced parenting stress (Canzi et al., Reference Canzi, Ranieri, Barni and Rosnati2019). However, given the dearth of longitudinal studies, more research in this area is needed to better understand potential associations between parenting stress and child development in adopted children and their caregivers.

In what follows, we first provide an overview of empirical research on the relation between PRF and children’s socio-emotional development. Next, we review evidence on the relation between parenting stress and PRF. This is followed by a discussion of the potential role of PRF and parenting stress in the socio-emotional development of adopted children. We conclude this introduction with the rationale and aims of the present study.

Parental reflective functioning and child socio-emotional development

PRF refers to the parent’s capacity to think about the child as having an inner psychological world, and hence to treat the child as a psychological agent with their own feelings, wishes, and desires (Slade, Reference Slade2005). The concept of PRF originated in developmental psychopathology research (Slade, Reference Slade2005) and can be seen as a specific operationalization of the umbrella concept of “parental mentalizing”, which can be broadly defined as the parent’s ability to represent and hold in mind the internal states of their child (Zeegers et al., Reference Zeegers, Colonnesi, Stams and Meins2017). Other research traditions in this area have focused on conceptually related notions such as parental mind-mindedness (Meins et al., Reference Meins, Centifanti, Fernyhough and Fishburn2013), parental mental-state talk (Tompkins et al., Reference Tompkins, Benigno, Kiger Lee and Wright2018), and parental insightfulness (Oppenheim & Koren-Karie, Reference Oppenheim and Koren-Karie2002). While each of these different approaches uses different operationalizations to assess parental mentalizing (see Schiborr et al. (Reference Schiborr, Lotzin, Romer, Schulte-Markwort and Ramsauer2013) for a comprehensive review) and emphasizes somewhat different aspects of parental mentalizing, they share a focus on parents’ capacities to be interested in and reflect on the internal mental world of their child (Zeegers et al., Reference Zeegers, Colonnesi, Stams and Meins2017), and their ability to appropriately mirror the child’s affective states (Luyten, Nijssens, et al., Reference Luyten, Nijssens, Fonagy and Mayes2017).

There is now good evidence to suggest that higher levels of parental mentalizing are prospectively related to better child social outcomes and less internalizing and externalizing problems (see Camoirano (Reference Camoirano2017) for a review). In addition, recent meta-analyses underline the importance of parental mentalizing for parent–child attachment and child mentalizing. For instance, Zeegers et al. (Reference Zeegers, Colonnesi, Stams and Meins2017) found robust associations between parental mentalizing, infant attachment security (pooled correlation of r = .30 based on 20 effect sizes), and parental sensitivity (pooled correlation of r = .24 based on 24 effect sizes). Similarly, meta-analyses by Aldrich et al. (Reference Aldrich, Chen and Alfieri2021), Devine and Hughes (Reference Devine and Hughes2018), and Tompkins et al. (Reference Tompkins, Benigno, Kiger Lee and Wright2018) revealed consistent modest but significant relations (pooled correlations between .16 and .21) between parental mentalizing and child theory-of-mind development (assessed in terms of false-belief understanding, emotion labeling, and perspective taking), which is considered an important component of child mentalizing.

As noted, high levels of parental mentalizing may be an important protective factor, particularly in contexts characterized by psychological or social risk (Ensink et al., Reference Ensink, Bégin, Normandin and Fonagy2016; Meins et al., Reference Meins, Centifanti, Fernyhough and Fishburn2013). In community samples, high levels of PRF have been shown to mitigate the impact of negative parenting behaviors such as high parental control and low parental warmth or involvement (Benbassat & Priel, Reference Benbassat and Priel2012). In low-socioeconomic status (SES) families, lower income was associated with lower autonomy support in fathers with low PRF, but not in fathers with moderate to high PRF (Buttitta et al., Reference Buttitta, Smiley, Kerr, Rasmussen, Querdasi and Borelli2019). In a prospective study, children of parents with high levels of mind-mindedness were also found to exhibit fewer externalizing and internalizing behaviors over time (Meins et al., Reference Meins, Centifanti, Fernyhough and Fishburn2013) compared with children of parents with lower levels of mind-mindedness. Research in mothers and children with a history of early adversity such as childhood abuse and neglect similarly indicates that parental mentalizing may have an important protective effect and may potentially play a role in breaking intergenerational cycles often associated with trauma. For instance, studies have shown that among mothers who themselves experienced severe early adversity, higher levels of PRF were associated with higher levels of attachment security in their children compared with mothers with low reflective functioning (Huth-Bocks et al., Reference Huth-Bocks, Muzik, Beeghly, Earls and Stacks2014; Schechter et al., Reference Schechter, Coots, Zeanah, Davies, Coates, Trabka and Myers2005). In children with histories of child sexual abuse, higher PRF was related to fewer externalizing problems (Ensink et al., Reference Ensink, Bégin, Normandin and Fonagy2016). More recent studies have further demonstrated that parents’ reflective functioning with regard to their own traumatic history (so-called trauma reflective functioning) is an important predictor of higher engagement with pregnancy and motherhood (Ensink et al., Reference Ensink, Berthelot, Bernazzani, Normandin and Fonagy2014) and a lower likelihood of their children being exposed to sexual abuse (Borelli et al., Reference Borelli, Cohen, Pettit, Normandin, Target, Fonagy and Ensink2019).

These studies also raise questions about the temporal stability of PRF. While PRF is considered to consist of both trait and state components (Luyten, Nijssens, et al., Reference Luyten, Nijssens, Fonagy and Mayes2017), currently no studies to date have investigated the temporal stability of PRF.

Parenting stress and PRF

Although parental mentalizing may protect against the impact of early childhood adversity, at the same time, research has amply shown that high levels of stress typically inhibit parents’ capacity for reflective functioning and facilitate a switch to more rapid, automatic, and often biased assumptions about their child’s mind (Mayes, Reference Mayes2000), in which non-reflective and negative assumptions dominate thinking about the self and others (Luyten, Nijssens, et al., Reference Luyten, Nijssens, Fonagy and Mayes2017). Studies suggest that parenting itself may be an important source of stress for young parents (McMahon & Meins, Reference McMahon and Meins2012). When parents have the feeling that their child is much more “difficult” than expected, or, more generally, that parenthood is much more challenging than they anticipated, this may lead to considerable distress and negative, or even aversive, feelings concerning themselves, their role as parents, their partner relationship, and their child (Deater-Deckard, Reference Deater-Deckard1998). Research has demonstrated a strong association between parenting stress and various negative outcomes in parents, children, and families, such as poor parental mental health, negative parenting behaviors, and child internalizing and externalizing problems (Holly et al., Reference Holly, Fenley, Kritikos, Merson, Abidin and Langer2019).

Studies have consistently found that that higher levels of stress are associated with lower levels of parental mentalizing (Håkansson et al., Reference Håkansson, Watten, Söderström and Øie2019; McMahon & Meins, Reference McMahon and Meins2012), and that this association is specific to stress related to the child or parenting, and not to general stress (Håkansson et al., Reference Håkansson, Watten, Söderström and Øie2019; Rutherford et al., Reference Rutherford, Booth, Luyten, Bridgett and Mayes2015). Evidence for the assumption that parenting stress negatively impacts on mentalizing, which in turn has a negative impact on parenting behaviors, also comes from experimental research. Rutherford et al. (Reference Rutherford, Booth, Luyten, Bridgett and Mayes2015), for instance, asked recent mothers to comfort a simulated baby until it stopped crying. In reality, the simulator was inconsolable. Mothers with high levels of prementalizing modes (assessed using the Parental Reflective Functioning Questionnaire (PRFQ) before the task) were less persistent in trying to console the simulated baby and had lower self-awareness of their own tolerance of distress compared with mothers with low levels of prementalizing modes. Prementalizing modes refer to a non-mentalizing stance in which parents have difficulties reflecting on their child’s inner mental world, and their attempts to do so are characterized by markedly negatively biased thoughts and feelings about their child’s mind. In a study by Krink et al. (Reference Krink, Muehlhan, Luyten, Romer and Ramsauer2018), psychosocial stress was experimentally induced in infants and their postpartum depressed mothers using a still-face paradigm, in which a mother must remain completely unresponsive to the infant for 1 or 2 min. The still-face paradigm is known to cause stress in both the caregiver and the child. Results showed that mothers with high levels of prementalizing modes demonstrated a greater decrease in observed sensitivity toward the child after completing a still-face paradigm compared with mothers with low levels of prementalizing modes. Similarly, in a study by McMahon and Meins (Reference McMahon and Meins2012), parental stress mediated the negative association between mind-mindedness and observed maternal hostility during a free-play interaction with the child, suggesting that low mind-mindedness was associated with higher parenting stress, leading to more hostility of mothers toward their child. These results seem to indicate that parenting stress may negatively influence PRF, which in turn may negatively affect child socio-emotional development.

However, it is again important to note that relations between parenting stress, parental mentalizing, and child functioning are most likely to be reciprocal. Child features such as temperament, early childhood trauma, or emotional problems may lead to increased parenting stress and impaired parental mentalizing (Rutherford et al., Reference Rutherford, Booth, Luyten, Bridgett and Mayes2015). Yet, no study to date has investigated potential reciprocal associations between parental mentalizing, parenting stress, and child features.

Parental mentalizing and parenting stress in adoptive families

Although the majority of internationally adopted children adjust fairly well over time (Van IJzendoorn et al., Reference Van IJzendoorn, Bakermans-Kranenburg and Juffer2007), a substantial group is at increased risk for psychological problems in later life, as is evidenced by elevated levels of insecure attachment, particularly disorganized attachment (van den Dries et al., Reference van den Dries, Juffer, van Ijzendoorn and Bakermans-Kranenburg2009), higher levels of externalizing problems in adolescence (Askeland et al., Reference Askeland, Hysing, La Greca, Aarø, Tell and Sivertsen2017), and the overrepresentation of adolescent adoptees in mental health care services (Miller et al., Reference Miller, Fan, Grotevant, Christensen, Coyl and Van Dulmen2000). Adopted children often have a history of substantial early adversity marked by separation of their biological parents, inadequate care from early caregivers or orphanages, abuse, and/or disruptive changes of context and culture. These difficulties may lead to underdeveloped emotional and relational capacities, which make it more challenging to parent these children than normally developing children (Midgley et al., Reference Midgley, Alayza, Lawrence and Bellew2018), as is also evidenced in high levels of placement breakdown (Palacios et al., Reference Palacios, Rolock, Selwyn and Barbosa-Ducharne2019). The capacity for PRF may play an important role in buffering the effects of early adversity (Adkins et al., Reference Adkins, Luyten and Fonagy2018), as it allows parents to keep accessing the thoughts and feelings that may underlie challenging behavior by the child, which can result in a more adequate and attuned response instead of reacting in a non-mentalizing and insensitive manner (Meins et al., Reference Meins, Centifanti, Fernyhough and Fishburn2013).

Very few studies have directly investigated the assumption that PRF may play an important role in buffering the effects of early adversity, and the majority of the studies conducted so far are cross-sectional. In one study with 43 adoptive families, higher levels of positive/reflective parenting, rated on an adaptation of the Parent Development Interview for use in adoptive parents 3 months after placement, were associated with less aggressive themes in children’s narratives of attachment-related story stem tasks assessed at the same time point (Steele et al., Reference Steele, Henderson, Hodges, Kaniuk, Hillman, Steele, Mayes, Fonagy and Target2007). In a cross-sectional study of 50 adoptive families, higher levels of maternal reflective functioning, assessed using the Parental Self-Reflectiveness Scale scored on the Parental Awareness Interview, were related to significantly fewer child externalizing problems (Priel et al., Reference Priel, Melamed-Hass, Besser and Kantor2000). Fishburn et al. (Reference Fishburn, Meins, Greenhow, Jones, Hackett, Biehal and Wade2017) found, in a sample of 36 adoptive families, that mind-mindedness showed a small but negative association with child behavioral difficulties measured at the same time point. Finally, in a longitudinal study, Tarullo et al. (Reference Tarullo, Youssef, Frenn, Wiik, Garvin and Gunnar2016) examined the relationship between Parental Mental State Talk and emotion understanding at the ages of 3 and 5.5 years in internationally adopted (N = 50) and non-adopted (N = 36) children. Results showed that Parental Mental State Talk at 3 years was positively associated with emotion understanding at 5.5 years in the internationally adopted children but not in the non-adopted children, suggesting once again that PRF may be particularly protective in children with a history of adversity.

However, more research in this area is needed, especially given the paucity of longitudinal studies. Moreover, no study to date has investigated potential evocative child-to-parent effects; parenting a child who is more challenging due to their past history of neglect and relational trauma may strain parents’ mentalizing capacities (Midgley et al., Reference Midgley, Alayza, Lawrence and Bellew2018), which may then in turn negatively impact child socio-emotional development.

Parenting stress might play an important role in such reciprocal relations, as prior studies in adoptive families have shown the existence of bidirectional associations between child development and parenting stress, with clear evidence on the one hand that certain child characteristics lead to more parenting stress (Canzi et al., Reference Canzi, Ranieri, Barni and Rosnati2019), and on the other hand that parenting stress has an adverse impact on children’s emotional and behavioral problems (Smith et al., Reference Smith, Cossette, Melançon, Beauvais-Dubois, Smolla, Gagnon-Oosterwaal and Bégin2018).

Child characteristics such as age at adoption (Canzi et al., Reference Canzi, Ranieri, Barni and Rosnati2019) and behavioral difficulties (McKay et al., Reference McKay, Ross and Goldberg2010; Palacios & Sánchez-Sandoval, Reference Palacios and Sánchez-Sandoval2006) have indeed been associated with greater parenting stress, even when controlling for other stressors such as parental well-being, quality of the parents’ partner relationship, and social support (Canzi et al., Reference Canzi, Ranieri, Barni and Rosnati2019). Age at adoption is an often-used proxy for early adversity, because later placement is associated with a longer exposure to disadvantageous situations such as problematic parenting conditions, more changes of residence and caregivers, or longer institutionalization. Research consistently shows that later age at adoption is related to negative outcomes in various developmental areas (Canzi et al., Reference Canzi, Ranieri, Barni and Rosnati2019; Julian, Reference Julian2013).

Other studies have demonstrated the maladaptive effects of parenting stress, as it is prospectively associated with higher levels of internalizing and externalizing problems in adopted children (Melançon et al., Reference Melançon, Cossette, Smith, Beauvais-Dubois, Cyr and Smolla2019; Smith et al., Reference Smith, Cossette, Melançon, Beauvais-Dubois, Smolla, Gagnon-Oosterwaal and Bégin2018). However, no study to date has examined the possible mediating role of parenting stress in the relation between PRF and child difficulties in adoptive families.

The present study

The present study aims to fill important gaps in our knowledge concerning PRF, parenting stress, and children’s socio-emotional problems in internationally adopted children and their adoptive parents. This paper presents the first prospective study investigating the temporal stability of PRF and its association with child socio-emotional problems in the first 4 years after adoption in a sample of internationally adopted children and their adoptive parents. In addition, this study investigated the potential mediating role of parenting stress in the relation between PRF and child socio-emotional problems by using a multilevel mediational approach. The aims of this study were therefore threefold.

The first aim was to examine the temporal stability of adoptive parents’ PRF in the first 4 years after adoption. Using multilevel growth analyses, we estimated growth trajectories of the three dimensions of PRF (i.e., prementalizing modes [PM], certainty about mental states [CM], and interest and curiosity [IC]) over five data waves for adoptive mothers and fathers separately. As the current sample consisted of a fairly homogeneous group of relatively highly educated, middle- to high-SES parents, we did not expect to find differences in the trajectories of PRF of the adoptive mothers and fathers. However, we did anticipate that the trajectories of parents’ PRF over time would differ as a function of child age at adoption. As children who are adopted later in life typically experience more emotional and relational difficulties, and these problems can become more pronounced with increasing age, we expected that, over time, late adoptees would challenge the PRF capacities of their adoptive parents more than early adoptees. In the adoption literature there is currently no consensus regarding a cutoff for “late adoption” (Julian, Reference Julian2013), and cutoffs used in research range from 12 months to 4 years. Yet, as there is increasing consensus that age of adoption is an important proxy for adversity, a case can be made for the cutoff for late adoption being earlier rather than later. For the current study, we based our cutoff on a study by Merz and McCall (Reference Merz and McCall2010), who found that among adopted children (with similar pre-adoptive experiences to those of the children in the current study), those adopted after the age of 18 months had significantly higher levels of social-emotional problems than children adopted at a younger age. Therefore, we hypothesized that, over time, parents of late adoptees (≥18 months of age, based on Merz and McCall (Reference Merz and McCall2010)) would (a) experience a stronger increase in PM, (b) gain less certainty about what their children are thinking or feeling (a less steady increase in CM), and (c) become less interested in what goes on in their child’s mind (a stronger decrease in levels of IC) compared with parents of early adoptees (<18 months).

A second aim of this study was to examine the trajectories of internationally adopted children’s socio-emotional problems in the first 4 years after adoption as a function of children’s gender and age at adoption as possible moderating variables. In line with evidence of a negative “age at adoption” effect across multiple domains of developmental outcomes (Julian, Reference Julian2013), we expected that children’s socio-emotional problems would be higher at baseline and would show a greater increase for children who were adopted at an older age (≥18 months) than for children who were younger at adoption. We did not expect to find differences between boys and girls (Juffer & van Ijzendoorn, Reference Juffer and van Ijzendoorn2005).

Finally, we investigated reciprocal relations between the PRF dimensions and children’s socio-emotional problems, and whether these relations were mediated by parenting stress as assessed by the Parenting Stress Index, and in particular by parental feelings of incompetence, role restriction, marital dissatisfaction, and social isolation. As PRF is expected to foster children’s socio-emotional development, we expected that higher levels of both CM and IC and lower levels of PM in both mothers and fathers would be associated with less severe child socio-emotional problems. Finally, as parenting stress can be expected to negatively influence both PRF and child socio-emotional outcomes, we hypothesized that parenting stress would mediate the relations between the different dimensions of PRF and child socio-emotional problems.

Method

Participants and procedures

This study is part of the Leuven Adoption Study, a multi-wave, multi-method, and multi-informant study aimed at identifying the developmental trajectories of internationally adopted children and their families in Flanders (Belgium). Recruitment for the study was done through adoption agencies, social media, and meetings of prospective adoptive parents. Couples who were interested in participating in the study met with a research assistant, who provided detailed information about the study. After providing informed consent, participants completed assessments at 2 weeks, 6 months, and 1 year after the arrival of their child, and, from then on, annually. Assessments included interviews, behavioral observations, experimental tasks, and questionnaires to assess the psychological functioning of the children and their mothers and fathers. This study used data collected at 6 months (Time 1, T1), 1 year (T2), 2 years (T3), 3 years (T4), and 4 years (T5) after child arrival.

Participants were 48 adoptive families, each consisting of the mother, father, and adopted child. Heterosexual couples who had applied for the international adoption of a first child and did not have biological children of their own were included in the study. Furthermore, to participate, the adopted children had to be younger than 2.5 years on arrival in Belgium and had to learn Dutch as the main language in their adoptive families. Owing to the age restriction, 10 of the initially screened 58 families were no longer eligible to participate in the study.

All the couples had Belgian nationality and spoke Dutch as their first language. The couples were all married (being married is often a prerequisite for the countries from which children are adopted), and the duration of their relationship ranged from 4.25 to 20.41 years (M = 10.83, SD = 3.56). The majority of couples reported infertility problems (72%) or chronic diseases (e.g., endometriosis, severe heart failure; 12%) that meant they were unable to have a biological child. The remaining 16% of parents indicated that they mainly wanted to adopt in order to give a child a better future. Overall, the couples represented a relatively older, relatively highly educated, middle- to high-SES group of parents. The mean age of the parents was 33.7 years (SD = 3.59) with a range of 27 to 46 years. For 20.8% of parents, the highest obtained level of education was a high school diploma; the other 79.2% of parents had participated in higher education, of whom 41.7% had a bachelor’s degree and 37.5% a master’s degree. Moreover, at the start of the study all couples had already gone through an extensive psychosocial screening procedure (carried out by social services of the Flemish agency for Public Health, Welfare and Family) before being allowed to adopt a child, the aim of which is to minimize the risk of placement breakdown.

The group of adopted children consisted of 34 boys (70.8%) and 14 girls (29.2%). The mean age at arrival was 13.43 months (SD = 6.56), with a range of 4 to 30 months (2.5 years). The children were adopted from eight countries: Ethiopia (27), South Africa (10), Kazakhstan (6), Nigeria (1), Sri Lanka (1), China (1), Burkina Faso (1), and Uganda (1). Parents reported that the children had lived in a range of placements, going from between one and four different placements before being adopted.

Instruments

Parental Reflective Functioning Questionnaire

The Parental Reflective Functioning Questionnaire (PRFQ; Luyten, Mayes, et al., Reference Luyten, Mayes, Nijssens and Fonagy2017; Luyten, Nijssens, et al., Reference Luyten, Nijssens, Fonagy and Mayes2017) was developed as a brief, multidimensional measure of PRF. The PRFQ consists of three subscales assessing three key dimensions of PRF: (a) the prementalizing modes (PM) subscale assesses a non-mentalizing stance of parents toward their child consisting of severely biased and often malevolent assumptions about the internal world of their child (e.g., “My child cries around strangers to embarrass me.”); (b) the certainty of mental states (CM) subscale focuses on parents’ capacity to recognize the opacity of mental states (e.g., “I always know why I do what I do to my child.”). Low scores on CM reflect a stance characterized by a lack of certainty about the child’s mind, also referred to as hypomentalizing. Very high scores on CM may reflect a stance in which parents are overly certain about the mental states of their child, which may give rise to hypermentalizing and which may be experienced by the child as intrusive or overcontrolling (e.g., “I always know what my child wants”) (Luyten, Mayes, et al., Reference Luyten, Mayes, Nijssens and Fonagy2017); and (c) the interest and curiosity (IC) subscale assesses the presence or absence of active curiosity about and willingness to understand the mental states of the child (e.g., “I am often curious to find out how my child feels.”). While low IC scores reflect a lack of interest in the child’s mind, very high scores reflect an excessive interest in the child’s mind. All items are scored on a 7-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (completely disagree) to 7 (completely agree). Regarding internal consistency in this sample, Cronbach’s alpha of PM ranged from .56 to .81, CM from .84 to .86, and IC from .62 to .73 over the five times of measurement. Concerning stability, Tables 35 of the online data supplement present correlations for each PRF dimension over the five measurement times (for mothers and fathers).

Parenting Stress Index

Parenting stress was assessed by the Dutch translation of the Parenting Stress Index (PSI; de Brock et al., Reference de Brock, Vermulst, Gerris and Abidin1992). The PSI is a self-report questionnaire that consists of 14 subscales assessing parenting stress and parent and child functioning, of which we included the four parenting stress subscales for the current study: incompetence (13 items; e.g., “I can't make a decision without help”), role restriction (7 items; e.g., “I feel restricted by my obligations as a parent”), social isolation (6 items; e.g., “I am less interested in other people than before”), and marital relationship (7 items; e.g., “Raising a child has given more relational conflicts than expected”). Items are rated on a Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 6 (strongly agree). Studies have supported the reliability and validity of the PSI (de Brock et al., Reference de Brock, Vermulst, Gerris and Abidin1992). In this study, Cronbach’s alpha ranged from .85 to .91 for incompetence, .85 to .89 for role restriction, .66 to .71 for social isolation, and .69 to .79 for marital relationship over the five times of measurement. In addition, Tables 69 of the online data supplement present correlations for each PSI dimension over the five measurement times (for mothers and fathers).

Children’s socio-emotional problems

At the different measurement times, age-appropriate measures were used to assess the children’s socio-emotional problems: the Brief Infant-Toddler Social Emotional Assessment (BITSEA) was administered when the children were between 1.5 and 3 years old, the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL; version 1.5–5 years) was used when the children were between 3 and 5 years, and the CBCL (version 5–18 years) was used when the children were more than 5 years old. Both the BITSEA and the CBCL involve parents rating various features of their children on a three-point Likert scale: 0 (absent), 1 (occurs sometimes), 2 (occurs often). To be able to compare scores and model developmental trajectories, scores of each of these measures were standardized using norms (means and standard deviations) as published in an important European validation study of the BITSEA (Wendland et al., Reference Wendland, Danet, Gacoin, Didane, Bodeau, Saïas and Guédeney2014) and in the Multicultural Supplement to the Manual for the ASEBA Preschool Forms and Profiles (Achenbach & Rescorla, Reference Achenbach and Rescorla2010) and School-Age Forms & Profiles (Achenbach & Rescorla, Reference Achenbach and Rescorla2007). Regarding internal consistency in this sample, Cronbach’s alpha of the BITSEA problem scale ranged from .64 to .72, and that of the CBCL Total Scale ranged from .73 to .78. Table 10 of the online data supplement presents correlations of the child socio-emotional scores over the five measurement times (for mothers and fathers).

Statistical analyses

Given that the data are hierarchically structured, with five measurement times (i.e., Level 1) being nested within 96 parents (i.e., Level 2), who are nested within 48 couples or families (i.e., Level 3), dependencies within families and within persons were expected. Therefore, multilevel modeling was used to investigate potential changes in PRF dimensions and child socio-emotional problems over time (Singer & Willett, Reference Singer and Willett2009). All analyses were done using SPSS (version 27, 2020) and R version 4.0.2 (R Core Team, 2018), with the lme4 (version 1.1-25), lmertest (version 3.1-3), and lavaan (version 0.6-7) packages.

First, unconditional means models without predictors were fitted for all target variables to evaluate the extent to which variability was attributable to within-person (Level 1), between-person (Level 2), or between-couple (Level 3) variation. Levels that did not contain a significant proportion of the variability (<10% based on Preacher et al. (Reference Preacher, Zyphur and Zhang2010)) were not included in further analyses.

Next, we used random intercept, random slope, multilevel growth models to investigate trajectories for each of the PRF dimensions and child socio-emotional problems over the five measurement times. Time points were coded −4, −3, −2, −1, and 0, implying that regression coefficients involving time measured the rate of change from 6 months to 4 years after adoption and regression intercepts referenced group differences at 4-year follow-up. For each multilevel growth model, in a first step an unconditional growth model representing the trajectories of BITSEA/CBCL or PRFQ as a linear function of time were run. As a second step, quadratic and cubic time variables were added to the model if likelihood ratio tests showed a significant improvement in fit. In a third step, conditional growth models were fitted including the time-invariant parameters of (a) gender of parent and child, (b) child age at arrival (<18 months, hereafter referred to as early adoptees, versus ≥18 months, referred to as late adoptees), and (c) the interaction between the two parameters. The significance of regression coefficients and intercepts was tested using Wald tests, and improvement of model fit when introducing new parameters was tested using likelihood ratio tests. The full taxonomy of models for each outcome variable can be found in the online data supplement of this article (see Tables 1114 in the online data supplement). Here, we present only the tables and figures for the final models (see Tables 1 and 2, and Figure 1). If parameters are not included in the final model, this means that the parameters’ Wald test was not significant (p > 0.05) and the addition of the parameter did not improve the model fit.

Figure 1. Growth trajectories of PRF (PM, CM, and IC) and child socio-emotional (SE) problems in the first 4 years after international adoption.

Table 1. Multilevel growth models for trajectories of PRF

Note. SE = standard error, CI: 95% confidence interval, LRT = likelihood ratio test.

Table 2. Multilevel model of growth child socio-emotional problems

Note. Children’s socio-emotional problems (measured using BITSEA problem score and CBCL Total Score), SE = standard error, CI: 95% confidence interval, LRT = likelihood ratio test.

Finally, we examined the relations between the PRF dimensions and children’s socio-emotional problems, and investigated whether these relations could be accounted for by parenting stress, and more specifically by parental feelings of (a) incompetence, (b) role restriction, (c) marital dissatisfaction, and (d) social isolation. To this end, we tested a series of multilevel mediation models in which the direct and indirect relations between PRF and child socio-emotional outcomes were simultaneously analyzed over the five measurement times. As all variables in the mediation models were measured at the within-person level (Level 1), rather than at the within-couple or the between-couple level, this kind of model is often referred to as a 1-1-1 model (Tofighi et al., Reference Tofighi, West and MacKinnon2013). As we wanted to investigate both parent-to-child and child-to-parent effects, each time we ran two models: one with the PRF dimension as independent variable and child socio-emotional problems as dependent variable (parent-to-child), and the other with child socio-emotional problems as independent variable and PRF dimension as dependent variable (child-to-parent).

Results

Preliminary analyses

There were no significant differences between mothers and fathers on most of the variables, with two exceptions (see Table 1 in the online data supplement): at T1 mothers reported significantly lower levels of prementalizing modes (PM) and higher levels of interest and curiosity (IC) than fathers.

As expected, parents of late adoptees reported stronger feelings of marital dissatisfaction and role restriction, and higher levels of child socio-emotional problems, than parents of early adoptees.

Developmental trajectories of PRF

Results of unconditional means models are presented in Table 2 of the online data supplement. While intraclass correlations showed significant variability at each level for certainty about mental states (CM) and all Parenting Stress Index subscales, for PM and IC no substantial variability was situated at the between-couple level (Level 3). Owing to the lack of variance in two of the three PRF dimensions, we decided not to include the between-couple level in further analyses. Although for child socio-emotional problems only 8% of the variance was due to within-couple variability, we decided to keep the within-couple level in all subsequent models to be able to compare the trajectories of child socio-emotional problems between mothers and fathers.

Next, we examined the growth models of each of the PRF dimensions and child socio-emotional problems (Table 1, Figure 1.1). First, a quadratic random intercepts and slopes model best fitted the change in PM over the 4-year follow-up. Regarding moderation, as expected, the rate of change in PM was significantly different as a function of child age at arrival in fathers; however, contrary to expectations, this was not the case for mothers. As shown in Figure 1, PM of fathers of late adoptees increased over time, while PM of fathers of early adoptees decreased. In contrast, PM of mothers remained stable over time, regardless of child age at arrival.

Second, the trajectory of CM followed a linear pattern from baseline to 4-year follow-up. As expected, the rate of change differed significantly as a function of child age at adoption in both mothers and fathers. As Figure 1.2 shows, CM increased less in parents of late adoptees than parents of early adoptees. However, there was no significant difference in the regression intercepts between early and late adoptees at the 4-year follow-up.

The trajectory of IC also followed a linear pattern. Results further showed that the regression intercept at 4 years post-adoption differed significantly between mothers and fathers, but that the rate of change did not. As can be seen in Figure 1.3, fathers exhibited lower levels of IC than mothers at T1 (see also Table 1 of the online data supplement), and this difference was maintained over time. Furthermore, the level of IC of both mothers and fathers decreased slightly but significantly over the 4 years of follow-up. Contrary to expectations, we found no effect of child age at arrival on IC.

Finally, the growth model for child socio-emotional problems (see Table 2 and Figure 1.4) indicated that the development of child socio-emotional problems followed a quadratic trajectory. Furthermore, both the rate of change and the regression intercept at 4 years post-adoption differed as a function of child age at arrival. As expected, late adoptees showed a significant increase in socio-emotional problems over time and a higher level of socio-emotional problems at 4 years post-adoption, whereas early adoptees showed a small, but significant, decrease of socio-emotional problems over time, resulting in lower levels of socio-emotional problems at 4-year follow-up.

Multilevel mediation models

Results of parent-to-child multilevel mediation models are presented in Figure 2 (see also Tables 15 and 16 in the online data supplement). First, in line with our hypotheses, PM was strongly related to child socio-emotional problems in both mothers and fathers, with the total effect (c) indicating that this relation was substantially stronger in mothers (Z(c) = 8.02) than in fathers (Z(c) = 2.24). For both mothers and fathers, the strength of this relation was reduced substantially after controlling for the different dimensions of parenting stress. However, the direct relation between PM and child socio-emotional problems remained significant only in mothers (Z(c’) = 4.74), indicating partial mediation by parenting stress between PM and child socio-emotional problems in mothers, and full mediation in fathers. In both mothers and fathers, there was a significant indirect effect only via parental incompetence (B(ID) = 0.17, SE = 0.09, p = .043, Z = 2.02, and B(ID) = 0.11, SE = 0.05, p = 0.025, Z = 2.24, respectively), indicating that an increase in PM was related to increased feelings of incompetence, which in turn was associated with an increase in child socio-emotional problems. The indirect paths via marital relationship, role restriction, and social isolation were not significant

Figure 2. Multilevel mediation models (parent to child). SE Problems: child socio-emotional problems, Bold line = indirect effect p < 0.05, dashed line = indirect effect p < 0.10.

Second, contrary to our expectations, the relation between CM and child socio-emotional problems was not significant in mothers. In fathers, there was a trend for CM to be negatively related to socio-emotional problems, but this trend did not reach significance (p = .06). However, there was a significant indirect relation between paternal CM and child socio-emotional problems through paternal feelings of incompetence (B(ID) = –0.07, SE = 0.04, p = .044, Z = –2.018). In mothers, there was a similar trend for an indirect effect of CM on child socio-emotional problems (B(ID) = –0.06, SE = 0.03, p = .064, Z = –1.85), but this trend did not reach significance.

Finally, and also contrary to expectations, there was no total effect (c) between IC and child socio-emotional problems in mothers or fathers. After including the parenting stress subscales as mediators, there was a positive direct association (c’) between maternal IC and child socio-emotional problems, and a negative indirect relation between maternal IC and child socio-emotional problems via parental incompetence (B(ID) = –0.12, SE = 0.024, p = .024, CI = –0.23, –0.02). In fathers, there was no direct or indirect association between IC and child socio-emotional problems.

Child-to-parent effects

As child socio-emotional problems may also influence the PRF of parents, we also analyzed reversed models in which we investigated whether child socio-emotional problems predicted PRF, with the parenting stress subscales as potential mediators (see Figure 3 and Tables 17 and 18 in the online data supplement). As expected, these child-to-parent models revealed the same total and direct effects between child socio-emotional problems and PM, CM, and IC as the parent-to-child models. The only exception was that the relation between child socio-emotional problems and paternal CM (Z = –1.97) reached significance (p = .049), whereas this was not the case in the parent-to-child model. Hence, as expected, in fathers, more child socio-emotional problems were significantly associated with less certainty about what the child is thinking or feeling.

Figure 3. Multilevel mediation models (child to parent). SE Problems: child socio-emotional problems, Bold line = indirect effect p < 0.05, dashed line = indirect effect p < 0.10.

There were significant differences between the child-to-parent and parent-to-child models testing for mediation, particularly in the models concerning fathers. First, the association between child socio-emotional problems and parental PM was in mothers partially mediated by parental incompetence (B(ID1) = 0.05, SE = 0.02, Z = 2.12, p = .034), while in fathers this relationship was fully mediated by marital relationship difficulties (B(ID3) = 0.05, SE = 0.02, Z = 2.44, p = .015). For role restriction (B(ID3) = 0.03, SE = 0.02, Z = 1.76, p = .08), the latter indirect effect showed only a trend toward significance. Second, by accounting for the different dimensions of parenting stress, the relation between child socio-emotional problems and paternal CM was reduced from B(c) = –0.13, SE = 0.07, Z = –1.97, p = .049 to B(c’) = –0.06, SE = 0.07, Z = –0.91, p = .36, indicating full mediation. The only indirect effect that reached a trend toward significance was again marital relationship difficulties, B(ID3) = –0.04, SE = 0.02, Z = –1.70, p = .09. For mothers, changes in child socio-emotional problems were not related to changes in maternal CM directly or indirectly through any of the subscales of parenting stress. Finally, the total effect (c) did not show an association between child socio-emotional problems and IC for mothers or fathers. However, after including the parenting stress dimensions as mediators, a positive direct relation between child socio-emotional problems and maternal IC, and a negative indirect effect via incompetence, emerged. In fathers, there was no significant direct relation between child socio-emotional problems and IC, and the only mediator that reached marginal significance was role restriction (B(ID2) = 0.04, SE = 0.02, Z = 1.68, p = .093).

Discussion

In light of recent evidence on the importance of parental mentalizing for the socio-emotional development of children who have experienced early adversity (Huth-Bocks et al., Reference Huth-Bocks, Muzik, Beeghly, Earls and Stacks2014), the current study reported results on the developmental trajectories of PRF and child socio-emotional problems in the first 4 years after adoption, and the possible mediating role of parenting stress in the relation between PRF and child socio-emotional problems, in a sample of internationally adopted children and their adoptive parents. As previous studies have convincingly shown that children who were adopted at a later age are at increased risk for a variety of behavioral and psychological problems (Julian, Reference Julian2013), we also examined the moderating impact of age at adoption throughout the different growth analyses. Overall, three sets of key findings emerged concerning (a) the development of socio-emotional problems in the first 4 years after adoption, (b) stability and change in PRF in the first years after adoption as a function of child features, and (c) the role of parenting stress in explaining relations between PRF and socio-emotional problems in adoptive children.

The development of socio-emotional problems in the first 4 years after adoption

The first set of findings relates to the development of socio-emotional problems in adopted children in the first 4 years after adoption. Compared with population norms, the adopted children in this study presented average levels of socio-emotional problems in the first 4 years after adoption (Achenbach & Rescorla, Reference Achenbach and Rescorla2007). These results are in line with the meta-analyses of Juffer and van Ijzendoorn (Reference Juffer and van Ijzendoorn2005) and Van IJzendoorn et al. (Reference Van IJzendoorn, Bakermans-Kranenburg and Juffer2007), who showed that most internationally adopted children develop fairly well and do not differ from children who were not adopted in terms of their levels of physical, social, and emotional outcomes.

Furthermore, as expected, children who were adopted at or after the age of 18 months had a significantly higher level of socio-emotional problems, both at baseline and at 4-year follow-up, compared with children who were adopted before 18 months of age. These results are consistent with a broader “age-at-adoption” effect, that is, a step-like relationship between age at adoption and various developmental outcomes, whereby children adopted before a certain cutoff generally show similar levels of socio-emotional problems compared with children raised by their birth parents, but children adopted after the cutoff show elevated socio-emotional problems (Julian, Reference Julian2013). Currently, there is no consensus in the literature with regard to the age at which this cutoff or “step” occurs, but it is generally assumed that it occurs at an earlier age for children who have experienced more severe levels of adversity. For example, in children adopted from Romanian orphanages, significant developmental differences were found in children adopted before or after the age of 6 months (Julian, Reference Julian2013). In contrast, in children adopted from Russian institutions (where they had received appropriate physical but poor psychosocial care), no differences on the CBCL were found between children adopted before 9 months of age and children adopted between 9 and 17 months of age. Only children older than 18 months at adoption showed significantly higher levels on both internalizing and externalizing problems (Merz & McCall, Reference Merz and McCall2010).

Moreover, the trajectory of socio-emotional problems differed as a function of age at adoption as well. While the level of socio-emotional problems of younger children tended to significantly decrease over time, that of the older children tended to increase significantly. These trajectories are consistent with previous findings that behavioral and internalizing problems worsened over 3 years post-adoption in children adopted at school age (Helder et al., Reference Helder, Mulder and Gunnoe2016) and that lower levels of pre-adoptive risk were associated with an accelerated decrease in internalizing problems in the first 3 years after adoption (Paine et al., Reference Paine, Perra, Anthony and Shelton2021). Based on evidence that age-at-adoption effects are more likely to be detected in adolescence than in childhood (Julian, Reference Julian2013) and that the impact of pre-adoption adversity on socio-emotional problems does not substantially decrease from childhood to adulthood (van der Vegt et al., Reference van der Vegt, van der Ende, Ferdinand, Verhulst and Tiemeier2009), further follow-up research is necessary to investigate whether this increasing trend in socio-emotional problems continues or becomes more pronounced in later developmental phases.

Stability and change in PRF

In line with our hypothesis that late adoptees possibly challenge parental mentalizing more than early adoptees, we found that age at adoption moderated the development of paternal prementalizing modes (PM) and certainty about mental states (CM) in both fathers and mothers. Although CM increased over time in mothers and fathers of both early and late adoptees, this increase was significantly less steep for parents of late adoptees compared with early adoptees. Results also showed that levels of PM decreased slightly over time for fathers of early adoptees, but there was a significant increase in PM for fathers of late adoptees. Contrary to our expectations, these age-at-adoption effects were not found in the development of maternal PM. In mothers, the levels of negative mentalizing and non-mentalizing remained low and stable over the first 4 years of adoption for both early and late adoptees. In addition, levels of parents’ interest and curiosity (IC) were moderated only by the gender of the parent and were not affected by the age of the child at arrival. At baseline (i.e., 6 months after adoption), mothers showed a significantly higher level of IC than fathers, and this difference was maintained over the first years after adoption. Additionally, there was a slight but significant decrease in IC in both mothers and fathers over time.

These results indicate that the impact of age at adoption on PRF remains relatively limited in this sample. In the group of late adoptees, adoptive parents remained very interested in what their child is thinking and feeling, and mothers in particular typically refrained from making non-mentalizing, negative attributions regarding their child’s mind. We hypothesize that these consistently high levels of PRF are at least in part attributable to the extensive screening process adoptive parents must go through before being able to adopt in Flanders, in which specific attention is paid to the reflective and emotion-regulating capacities of prospective adoptive parents. Previous studies by Palacios et al. (Reference Palacios, Román, Moreno and León2009) and León et al. (Reference León, Steele, Palacios, Román and Moreno2018) similarly suggest that the preparation and training process that Spanish adoptive parents must undergo may explain why adoptive mothers show more sophisticated and positive levels of PRF compared with non-adoptive mothers.

Nevertheless, there was also evidence for child-to-parent effects, whereby children seem to influence the development of mentalizing dimensions in their parents over time. Specifically, late adoptees seem to challenge their parents’ mentalizing capacities, as is evidenced by significantly smaller increases in levels of CM over time in both fathers and mothers of late adoptees compared with early adoptees. Late adoptees also seem to challenge the mentalizing capacities of fathers more than early adoptees, as evidenced by a significant increase of PM in fathers in the late adoptees group. Further research is needed to better understand these age-at-adoption effects. It is possible that these evocative effects reflect the consequences of longer exposure to early adversity, as expressed in higher levels of emotional problems, in late adoptees.

Exploratory post hoc analyses in the current study showed that the increase in PM occurred specifically in fathers of the late adoptees who demonstrated the highest levels of socio-emotional problems, suggesting that more “difficult” child behavior may impede paternal mentalizing. Evidence of children’s behavioral difficulties challenging parental mentalizing was previously also found in a study by Fishburn et al. (Reference Fishburn, Meins, Greenhow, Jones, Hackett, Biehal and Wade2017), who compared the levels of mind-mindedness, which can be considered a proxy of PRF, of adoptive and biological parents. Whereas adoptive parents appeared to have lower levels of mind-mindedness than biological parents in that study, further analyses showed that these differences stemmed from the fact that adopted children demonstrated more behavioral difficulties than the non-adopted children. After accounting for differences in behavioral problems, the difference in mind-mindedness between adoptive and biological parents was no longer significant. Moreover, studies focusing on adoptive mothers’ representations of their adopted children have also shown that older age at adoption was associated with more maternal disappointment and hostility and perceptions of the child as less affectionate, more aggressive and rejecting (Steele et al., Reference Steele, Henderson, Hodges, Kaniuk, Hillman, Steele, Mayes, Fonagy and Target2007; Steele, Reference Steele2006), and an overall less positive perception of the child (León et al., Reference León, Steele, Palacios, Román and Moreno2018).

The role of parenting stress

A final set of important findings concerns the relation between PRF and adopted children’s socio-emotional problems, and the possible mediating role of parenting stress. Of the three PRF dimensions, PM was most strongly related to children’s socio-emotional problems. As expected, in both mothers and fathers, a tendency for hypomentalizing and attributing negative mental states to the child was strongly associated with more pronounced child socio-emotional problems. Analyses further demonstrated that feelings of parental inadequacy play an important role in this relation, as, for mothers, feelings of parental incompetence were the only significant mediator in both the parent-to-child and child-to-parent models. In fathers, two potential mechanisms seemed to be at play. Feelings of incompetence fully accounted for the parent-to-child relation between PM and the children’s socio-emotional problems, but marital dissatisfaction was the only significant mediating mechanism in the child-to-parent model. Finally, whereas parenting stress in fathers fully explained the relationship between PM and child socio-emotional problems, this was not the case in mothers. Even when we accounted for parenting stress, there remained a very strong relation between maternal PM and child socio-emotional problems.

Next, contrary to our expectations, there was no association between maternal CM and child socio-emotional problems. However, higher levels of paternal CM were significantly associated with lower levels of child socio-emotional problems. Once again, paternal feelings of incompetence and of marital dissatisfaction appeared to be important mediating mechanisms: paternal feelings of incompetence fully explained the parent-to-child relation, whereas marital dissatisfaction explained the child-to-parent relation.

Finally, the relation between parental IC, parenting stress, and child socio-emotional problems also differed between mothers and fathers. In fathers, there was no relation between IC and child socio-emotional problems. At first sight this also seemed to be the case for mothers, but subsequent analyses showed that there were opposite direct and indirect relations between maternal IC and child socio-emotional problems. Importantly, when in mediation analyses a direct and a mediated effect of an independent variable on a dependent variable have opposite signs, this may point to a suppression effect (MacKinnon et al., Reference MacKinnon, Krull and Lockwood2000), meaning that the initial lack of an association between maternal IC and child socio-emotional problems was due to the fact that the positive direct effect (Z = 2.29) and negative indirect effect (Z = –2.26) canceled each other out. These contrasting findings suggest that maternal IC is related to both more child socio-emotional problems and to less child socio-emotional problems, as more IC was also associated with lower feelings of incompetence, which led to less pronounced child socio-emotional problems.

In summary, these results showed that decreases in PRF over time (expressed in more PM, more IC, and less CM) are associated with more pronounced child socio-emotional problems. The results regarding PM and CM were in line with our expectations and previous evidence that has demonstrated how more PM is associated with more hostility and less sensitivity toward the child, and that less CM is related to less parental competence and efficacy (De Roo et al., Reference De Roo, Wong, Rempel and Fraser2019). However, the positive direct relation between IC and child socio-emotional problems was unexpected. A first possible explanation is that mothers in this study had very high levels of IC (M = 5.90, SD = 0.50), and both theoretical accounts and research findings suggest that such very high IC scores may reflect intrusiveness rather than genuine adaptive interest in the inner world of the child. Prior studies have shown that high levels of IC are related to insecure child attachment and anxious-resistant attachment more specifically (Luyten, Mayes, et al., Reference Luyten, Mayes, Nijssens and Fonagy2017).

However, as Luyten, Nijssens, et al. (Reference Luyten, Nijssens, Fonagy and Mayes2017, p. 177) previously warned against “a simplistic and linear understanding of the relationship between PRF and child development”, it may be that child socio-emotional problems drive these relations, as more behavioral difficulties challenge the adoptive parents’ capacity for PRF. Indeed, more dysregulated and difficult interactions with the child can elicit a more frequent switch to a more automatic, emotional, non-mentalizing stance, as reflected in the increase in PM in fathers. Furthermore, results of this study suggest that in such circumstances, fathers are more likely to “give up”, whereas mothers appear to persist in trying to make sense of their child’s mind, which may be experienced as intrusive and/or overcontrolling by the child. The latter is the second possible explanation for the unexpected positive relation between IC and child socio-emotional problems.

Moreover, analyses of the indirect relations demonstrate that children’s behavioral difficulties have a differential impact on their mothers and fathers. Indeed, the child-to-parent models indicate that, especially for fathers, a stress spillover effect, or a transfer of negative affect from one relationship directly to another within a family system (Nelson et al., Reference Nelson, O'Brien, Blankson, Calkins and Keane2009) seems to take place, in which negative affect or frustrations due to the child’s socio-emotional problems seem to transfer into the marital relationship. While fathers thus seem to externalize the negative feelings elicited by difficult interactions with the child, mothers seem to internalize these feelings. Indeed, in mothers, a more self-critical tendency becomes apparent, in which more difficult behavior of the child leads to stronger feelings of maternal incompetence. This is in line with a previous study by Priel et al. (Reference Priel, Melamed-Hass, Besser and Kantor2000), who found that while adoptive mothers were highly reflective and positive about their children, they were less able to reflect about themselves and their own competences as mothers, with mothers of late adoptees scoring even lower on the latter than mothers of early adoptees. By contrast, no difference between self and child mentalizing was found for biological parents.

Clinical implications

This study has important clinical implications. While support for adoptive parents often focuses on the pre-adoption process, in particular the preparation and screening of prospective adoptive parents, there is much less focus on continued care and support for adoptive parents after the arrival of the child. However, this study shows that difficult dynamics can arise between adopted children’s behavioral difficulties, breakdowns in parental mentalizing, and parents’ feelings about their own incompetence and marital dissatisfaction. It is clear that when tensions rise, adoptive families might benefit from professional help that supports parents to understand the mental states underlying the difficult and unpredictable behavior of their child and to endure the relational consequences of early adversity, while also communicating their own feelings, vulnerabilities, and needs to their partner. Specific programs have been developed to scaffold the mentalizing capacity of adoptive (and foster) parents, such as the psychoeducational “Family Minds” program (Adkins et al., Reference Adkins, Luyten and Fonagy2018, Reference Adkins, Reisz, Hasdemir and Fonagy2022), the “Adopting Minds” therapy program for families in a post-adoption support service (Midgley et al., Reference Midgley, Alayza, Lawrence and Bellew2018), and a more extensive treatment guideline for adopted children who have experienced complex trauma and their parents (Vliegen et al., in press). We argue that the implementation of such programs in post-adoption support could be of great value for the well-being of both adoptive parents and children.

Limitations and strengths

An important limitation of the current study is the sample size. Although in the current work we selected multilevel modeling approaches that have been shown to be quite robust in relatively small samples with repeated measures, the study is underpowered to run more complex models that would allow us to effectively disentangle the bidirectional child and parent effects over time, for instance, through random intercept cross-lagged panel modeling.

A second limitation is that we were not able to investigate the trajectories of internalizing and externalizing problems separately over time, due to methodological complexities (i.e., the aggregation of two age-sensitive measures of child socio-emotional problems, one of which does not distinguish internalizing and externalizing problems). This means that we could not make any statements about possible deviations from the normative development of both types of problems we would expect in this preschool-to-latency phase.

Additionally, while the current study focuses on the trajectory of socio-emotional problems and PRF throughout the preschool years, continued research concerning these processes throughout the latency period and in the run-up to puberty would provide extremely valuable information, as in these periods of development external social, emotional, and academic expectations increase, and identity formation and the impact of hormonal changes will play an increasingly important role.

Finally, PRF was assessed in this study using a brief self-report measure, that is, the PRFQ. Future research is therefore needed to replicate these findings using more narrative-based measures of PRF, such as the Reflective Functioning Scale (Fonagy et al., Reference Fonagy, Target, Steele and Steele1998), which can be scored on semi-structured interviews about the parent–child relationship, such as the Parent Development Interview (Slade, Reference Slade2005) or the Working Model of the Child Interview (Schechter et al., Reference Schechter, Coots, Zeanah, Davies, Coates, Trabka and Myers2005).

This study also has a number of strengths. This is the first paper to report on the evolution of socio-emotional problems of internationally adopted children in Belgium using high-quality, age-sensitive measures. Additionally, while previous research has provided evidence on the reliability, validity, and predictive ability of the PRFQ, this is the first to study the evolution of the three PRF dimensions over time. This study shows that it is important to investigate not just the overall capacity for PRF because the investigation of the different dimensions of PRF provides complementary insight, consistent with the view that PRF is a multidimensional capacity.

Moreover, whereas developmental research has often focused on the mother–child relationship, the current study demonstrates that relations and mediating mechanisms differ between mothers and fathers. Therefore, these findings once again demonstrate the importance of including both mothers and fathers in developmental research, and of studying the differential impact of maternal and paternal reflective functioning on child development (Buttitta et al., Reference Buttitta, Smiley, Kerr, Rasmussen, Querdasi and Borelli2019).

A final strength of this study is its target group, as families with internationally adopted children provide a unique opportunity to study the impact of parenting abilities on child development without confounding genetic overlap (Devine & Hughes, Reference Devine and Hughes2018).

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579422001171

Acknowledgements

We would like to express our gratitude to the adoptive families and research assistants who have been part of the Leuven Adoption Study for more than 10 years. We also thank Nele Flamant for her help with the multilevel mediation analyses.

Funding statement

The Leuven Adoption Study is funded in part by the Flemish agency Kind en Gezin (https://www.kindengezin.be/) with an annual grant of €7,000 rewarded to NV from 2009 to 2021. The funders had no role in the study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. The study received no other external funding.

Conflicts of interest

None.

References

Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. (2007). Multicultural supplement to the manual for the ASEBA school-age forms & profiles: Child Behavior Checklist for ages 6-18. ASEBA.Google Scholar
Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2010). Multicultural supplement to the manual for the ASEBA preschool forms & profiles: Child Behavior Checklist for ages 1 1/2-5. ASEBA.Google Scholar
Adkins, T., Luyten, P., & Fonagy, P. (2018). Development and preliminary evaluation of Family Minds: A mentalization-based psychoeducation program for foster parents. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 27(8), 25192532. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-018-1080-x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adkins, T., Reisz, S., Hasdemir, D., & Fonagy, P. (2022). Family Minds: A randomized controlled trial of a group intervention to improve foster parents’ reflective functioning. Development and Psychopathology, 34(3), 11771191. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457942000214X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aldrich, N. J., Chen, J., & Alfieri, L. (2021). Evaluating associations between parental mind-mindedness and children’s developmental capacities through meta-analysis. Developmental Review, 60, 100946. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2021.100946 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Askeland, K. G., Hysing, M., La Greca, A. M., Aarø, L. E., Tell, G. S., & Sivertsen, B. (2017). Mental health in internationally adopted adolescents: A meta-analysis. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 56(3), 203213. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2016.12.009 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benbassat, N., & Priel, B. (2012). Parenting and adolescent adjustment: The role of parental reflective function. Journal of Adolescence, 35(1), 163174. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2011.03.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borelli, J. L., Cohen, C., Pettit, C., Normandin, L., Target, M., Fonagy, P., & Ensink, K. (2019). Maternal and child sexual abuse history: An intergenerational exploration of children’s adjustment and maternal trauma-reflective functioning. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1062. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01062 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buttitta, K. V., Smiley, P. A., Kerr, M. L., Rasmussen, H. F., Querdasi, F. R., & Borelli, J. L. (2019). In a father’s mind: Paternal reflective functioning, sensitive parenting, and protection against socioeconomic risk. Attachment & Human Development, 21(5), 445466. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2019.1582596 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Camoirano, A. (2017). Mentalizing makes parenting work: A review about parental reflective functioning and clinical interventions to improve it. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 14. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00014 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Canzi, E., Ranieri, S., Barni, D., & Rosnati, R. (2019). Predictors of parenting stress during early adoptive parenthood. Current Psychology, 38(3), 811820. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-017-9657-x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Brock, A., Vermulst, A., Gerris, J., & Abidin, R. R. (1992). NOSI. Nijmeegse Ouderlijke Stress Index. Handleiding experimentele versie. Swets & Zeitlinger.Google Scholar
De Roo, M., Wong, G., Rempel, G. R., & Fraser, S. N. (2019). Advancing optimal development in children: Examining the construct validity of a Parent Reflective Functioning Questionnaire. JMIR Pediatrics and Parenting, 2(1), e11561. https://doi.org/10.2196/11561 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deater-Deckard, K. (1998). Parenting stress and child adjustment: Some old hypotheses and new questions. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 5(3), 314332. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2850.1998.tb00152.x Google Scholar
Devine, R. T., & Hughes, C. (2018). Family correlates of false belief understanding in early childhood: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 89(3), 971987. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12682 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ensink, K., Bégin, M., Normandin, L., & Fonagy, P. (2016). Maternal and child reflective functioning in the context of child sexual abuse: Pathways to depression and externalising difficulties. European Journal of Psychotraumatology, 7(1), 30611. https://doi.org/10.3402/ejpt.v7.30611 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ensink, K., Berthelot, N., Bernazzani, O., Normandin, L., & Fonagy, P. (2014). Another step closer to measuring the ghosts in the nursery: Preliminary validation of the Trauma Reflective Functioning Scale. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1471. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01471 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fishburn, S., Meins, E., Greenhow, S., Jones, C., Hackett, S., Biehal, N., & Wade, J. (2017). Mind-mindedness in parents of looked-after children. Developmental Psychology, 53(10), 19541965. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000304 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fonagy, P., Campbell, C., Constantinou, M., Higgitt, A., Allison, E., & Luyten, P. (2021). Culture and psychopathology: An attempt at reconsidering the role of social learning. Development and Psychopathology, 34(4), 12051220. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579421000092 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fonagy, P., Target, M., Steele, H., & Steele, M. (1998). Reflective Functioning Scale manual version 5.0, for application to adult attachment interviews [Unpublished manuscript]. University College London.Google Scholar
Håkansson, U., Watten, R. G., Söderström, K., & Øie, M. G. (2019). The association between executive functioning and parental stress and psychological distress is mediated by parental reflective functioning in mothers with substance use disorder. Stress and Health, 35(4), 407420. https://doi.org/10.1002/smi.2868 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Helder, E. J., Mulder, E., & Gunnoe, M. L. (2016). A longitudinal investigation of children internationally adopted at school age. Child Neuropsychology, 22(1), 3964. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2014.967669 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holly, L. E., Fenley, A. R., Kritikos, T. K., Merson, R. A., Abidin, R. R., & Langer, D. A. (2019). Evidence-base update for parenting stress measures in clinical samples. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 48(5), 685705. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2019.1639515 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huth-Bocks, A. C., Muzik, M., Beeghly, M., Earls, L., & Stacks, A. M. (2014). Secure base scripts are associated with maternal parenting behavior across contexts and reflective functioning among trauma-exposed mothers. Attachment & Human Development, 16(6), 535556. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2014.967787 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Juffer, F., & van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (2005). Behavior problems and mental health referrals of international adoptees: A meta-analysis. Journal of the American Medical Association, 293(20), 25012515. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.293.20.2501 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Julian, M. M. (2013). Age at adoption from institutional care as a window into the lasting effects of early experiences. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 16(2), 101145. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10567-013-0130-6 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krink, S., Muehlhan, C., Luyten, P., Romer, G., & Ramsauer, B. (2018). Parental reflective functioning affects sensitivity to distress in mothers with postpartum depression. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 27(5), 16711681. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-017-1000-5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
León, E., Steele, M., Palacios, J., Román, M., & Moreno, C. (2018). Parenting adoptive children: Reflective functioning and parent-child interactions. A comparative, relational and predictive study. Children and Youth Services Review, 95, 352360. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2018.11.009 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luyten, P., Campbell, C., Allison, E., & Fonagy, P. (2020). The mentalizing approach to psychopathology: State of the art and future directions. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 16(1), 297325. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-071919-015355 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luyten, P., & Fonagy, P. (2015). The neurobiology of mentalizing. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research and Treatment, 6(4), 366379.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luyten, P., Mayes, L. C., Nijssens, L., & Fonagy, P. (2017). The parental reflective functioning questionnaire: Development and preliminary validation. PLOS ONE, 12(5), e0176218. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0176218 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luyten, P., Nijssens, L., Fonagy, P., & Mayes, L. C. (2017). Parental reflective functioning: Theory, research, and clinical applications. Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 70(1), 174199. https://doi.org/10.1080/00797308.2016.1277901 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacKinnon, D. P., Krull, J. L., & Lockwood, C. M. (2000). Equivalence of the mediation, confounding and suppression effect. Prevention Science, 1(4), 173181. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1026595011371 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mayes, L. C. (2000). A developmental perspective on the regulation of arousal states. Seminars in Perinatology, 24(4), 267279. https://doi.org/10.1053/sper.2000.9121 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McKay, K., Ross, L. E., & Goldberg, A. E. (2010). Adaptation to parenthood during the post-adoption period: A review of the literature. Adoption Quarterly, 13(2), 125144. https://doi.org/10.1080/10926755.2010.481040 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McMahon, C. A., & Meins, E. (2012). Mind-mindedness, parenting stress, and emotional availability in mothers of preschoolers. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 27(2), 245252. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2011.08.002 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meins, E., Centifanti, L. C. M., Fernyhough, C., & Fishburn, S. (2013). Maternal mind-mindedness and children’s behavioral difficulties: Mitigating the impact of low socioeconomic status. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41(4), 543553. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-012-9699-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Melançon, F., Cossette, L., Smith, C., Beauvais-Dubois, C., Cyr, C., & Smolla, N. (2019). Parenting stress of adoptive mothers, mother-child conflict, and behavior problems during adolescence among international adoptees. Journal of Family Psychology, 33(8), 988993. https://doi.org/10.1037/fam0000542 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Merz, E. C., & McCall, R. B. (2010). Behavior problems in children adopted from psychosocially depriving institutions. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 38(4), 459470. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-009-9383-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Midgley, N., Alayza, A., Lawrence, H., & Bellew, R. (2018). Adopting Minds — a mentalization-based therapy for families in a post-adoption support service: Preliminary evaluation and service user experience. Adoption & Fostering, 42(1), 2237. https://doi.org/10.1177/0308575917747816 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, B. C., Fan, X., Grotevant, H. D., Christensen, M., Coyl, D., & Van Dulmen, M. (2000). Adopted adolescents' overrepresentation in mental health counseling: Adoptees' problems or parents' lower threshold for referral? Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39(12), 15041511. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-200012000-00011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, J. A., O'Brien, M., Blankson, A. N., Calkins, S. D., & Keane, S. P. (2009). Family stress and parental responses to children’s negative emotions: Tests of the spillover, crossover, and compensatory hypotheses. Journal of Family Psychology, 23(5), 671679. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015977 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oppenheim, D., & Koren-Karie, N. (2002). Mothers' insightfulness regarding their children’s internal worlds: The capacity underlying secure child-mother relationships. Infant Mental Health Journal, 23(6), 593605. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.10035 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paine, A. L., Perra, O., Anthony, R., & Shelton, K. H. (2021). Charting the trajectories of adopted children’s emotional and behavioral problems: The impact of early adversity and postadoptive parental warmth. Development and Psychopathology, 33(3), 922936. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579420000231 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palacios, J., Rolock, N., Selwyn, J., & Barbosa-Ducharne, M. (2019). Adoption breakdown: Concept, research, and implications. Research on Social Work Practice, 29(2), 130142. https://doi.org/10.1177/1049731518783852 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Palacios, J., Román, M., Moreno, C., & León, E. (2009). Family context for emotional recovery in internationally adopted children. International Social Work, 52(5), 609620. https://doi.org/10.1177/0020872809337679 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Palacios, J., & Sánchez-Sandoval, Y. (2006). Stress in parents of adopted children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 30(6), 481487. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025406071492 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Preacher, K. J., Zyphur, M. J., & Zhang, Z. (2010). A general multilevel SEM framework for assessing multilevel mediation. Psychological Methods, 15(3), 209233. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020141 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Priel, B., Melamed-Hass, S., Besser, A., & Kantor, B. (2000). Adjustment among adopted children: The role of maternal self-reflectiveness. Family Relations, 49(4), 389396. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1741-3729.2000.00389.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
R Core Team. (2018). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna. https://www.R-project.org Google Scholar
Rutherford, H. J. V., Booth, C. R., Luyten, P., Bridgett, D. J., & Mayes, L. C. (2015). Investigating the association between parental reflective functioning and distress tolerance in motherhood. Infant Behavior and Development, 40, 5463. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2015.04.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schechter, D. S., Coots, T., Zeanah, C. H., Davies, M., Coates, S. W., Trabka, K. A., & Myers, M. M. (2005). Maternal mental representations of the child in an inner-city clinical sample: Violence-related posttraumatic stress and reflective functioning. Attachment & Human Development, 7(3), 313331. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616730500246011 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schiborr, J., Lotzin, A., Romer, G., Schulte-Markwort, M., & Ramsauer, B. (2013). Child-focused maternal mentalization: A systematic review of measurement tools from birth to three. Measurement, 46(8), 24922509. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.measurement.2013.05.007 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singer, J. D., & Willett, J. B. (2009). Applied longitudinal data analysis: Modeling change and event occurrence. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Slade, A. (2005). Parental reflective functioning: An introduction. Attachment & Human Development, 7(3), 269281. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616730500245906 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, C., Cossette, L., Melançon, F., Beauvais-Dubois, C., Smolla, N., Gagnon-Oosterwaal, N., & Bégin, J. (2018). Behavior problems in adolescence among international adoptees, pre-adoption adversity, and parenting stress. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 57, 5361. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2018.04.005 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steele, M. (2006). Changing minds: Attachment representations and adoption outcome in a maltreated sample. Paper presented at the International Conference on Adoption Research (ICAR 2), Norwich, England.Google Scholar
Steele, M., Henderson, K., Hodges, J., Kaniuk, J., Hillman, S., & Steele, H. (2007). In the best interests of the late-placed child: A report from attachment representations and adoption outcome study. In Mayes, L., Fonagy, P., & Target, M. (Eds.), Developmental science and psychoanalysis: Integration and innovation (pp. 159182). Karnac Books.Google Scholar
Tarullo, A. R., Youssef, A., Frenn, K. A., Wiik, K., Garvin, M. C., & Gunnar, M. R. (2016). Emotion understanding, parent mental state language, and behavior problems in internationally adopted children. Development and Psychopathology, 28(2), 371383. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457941500111X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tofighi, D., West, S. G., & MacKinnon, D. P. (2013). Multilevel mediation analysis: The effects of omitted variables in the 1-1-1 model. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 66(2), 290307. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8317.2012.02051.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tompkins, V., Benigno, J. P., Kiger Lee, B., & Wright, B. M. (2018). The relation between parents' mental state talk and children’s social understanding: A meta-analysis. Social Development, 27(2), 223246. https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12280 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van den Dries, L., Juffer, F., van Ijzendoorn, M. H., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2009). Fostering security? A meta-analysis of attachment in adopted children. Children and Youth Services Review, 31(3), 410421. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2008.09.008 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van der Vegt, E. J., van der Ende, J., Ferdinand, R. F., Verhulst, F. C., & Tiemeier, H. (2009). Early childhood adversities and trajectories of psychiatric problems in adoptees: Evidence for long lasting effects. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37(2), 239249. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-008-9272-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van IJzendoorn, M. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & Juffer, F. (2007). Plasticity of growth in height, weight, and head circumference: Meta-analytic evidence of massive catch-up after international adoption. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 28(4), 334343. https://doi.org/10.1097/DBP.0b013e31811320aa CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vliegen, N., Tang, E., Midgley, N., Luyten, P., & Fonagy, P. (in press). Therapeutic work for children with complex trauma: A three-track psychodynamic approach. Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wendland, J., Danet, M., Gacoin, E., Didane, N., Bodeau, N., Saïas, T., & Guédeney, A. (2014). French version of the Brief Infant-Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment questionnaire-BITSEA. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 39(5), 562575. https://doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/jsu016 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zeegers, M. A. J., Colonnesi, C., Stams, G. J. M., & Meins, E. (2017). Mind matters: A meta-analysis on parental mentalization and sensitivity as predictors of infant-parent attachment. Psychological Bulletin, 143(12), 12451272. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000114 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. Growth trajectories of PRF (PM, CM, and IC) and child socio-emotional (SE) problems in the first 4 years after international adoption.

Figure 1

Table 1. Multilevel growth models for trajectories of PRF

Figure 2

Table 2. Multilevel model of growth child socio-emotional problems

Figure 3

Figure 2. Multilevel mediation models (parent to child). SE Problems: child socio-emotional problems, Bold line = indirect effect p < 0.05, dashed line = indirect effect p < 0.10.

Figure 4

Figure 3. Multilevel mediation models (child to parent). SE Problems: child socio-emotional problems, Bold line = indirect effect p < 0.05, dashed line = indirect effect p < 0.10.

Supplementary material: File

Malcorps et al. supplementary material

Tables S1-S18

Download Malcorps et al. supplementary material(File)
File 94.6 KB