Hostname: page-component-7bb8b95d7b-fmk2r Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-09-25T07:47:26.481Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Transdiagnostic indicators predict developmental changes in cognitive control resting-state networks

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  24 August 2023

Giorgia Picci
Affiliation:
Institute for Human Neuroscience, Boys Town National Research Hospital, Boys Town, NE, USA Center for Pediatric Brain Health, Boys Town National Research Hospital, Boys Town, NE, USA Department of Pharmacology & Neuroscience, Creighton University, Omaha, NE, USA
Nathan M. Petro
Affiliation:
Institute for Human Neuroscience, Boys Town National Research Hospital, Boys Town, NE, USA Center for Pediatric Brain Health, Boys Town National Research Hospital, Boys Town, NE, USA
Jake J. Son
Affiliation:
Institute for Human Neuroscience, Boys Town National Research Hospital, Boys Town, NE, USA Center for Pediatric Brain Health, Boys Town National Research Hospital, Boys Town, NE, USA College of Medicine, University of Nebraska Medical Center, Omaha, NE, USA
Oktay Agcaoglu
Affiliation:
Tri-Institutional Center for Translational Research in Neuroimaging and Data Science (TReNDS), Georgia State University, Georgia Institute of technology, and Emory University, Atlanta, GA, USA
Jacob A. Eastman
Affiliation:
Institute for Human Neuroscience, Boys Town National Research Hospital, Boys Town, NE, USA Center for Pediatric Brain Health, Boys Town National Research Hospital, Boys Town, NE, USA
Yu-Ping Wang
Affiliation:
Department of Biomedical Engineering, Tulane University, New Orleans, LA, USA
Julia M. Stephen
Affiliation:
Mind Research Network, Albuquerque, NM, USA
Vince D. Calhoun
Affiliation:
Tri-Institutional Center for Translational Research in Neuroimaging and Data Science (TReNDS), Georgia State University, Georgia Institute of technology, and Emory University, Atlanta, GA, USA
Brittany K. Taylor
Affiliation:
Institute for Human Neuroscience, Boys Town National Research Hospital, Boys Town, NE, USA Center for Pediatric Brain Health, Boys Town National Research Hospital, Boys Town, NE, USA Department of Pharmacology & Neuroscience, Creighton University, Omaha, NE, USA
Tony W. Wilson*
Affiliation:
Institute for Human Neuroscience, Boys Town National Research Hospital, Boys Town, NE, USA Center for Pediatric Brain Health, Boys Town National Research Hospital, Boys Town, NE, USA Department of Pharmacology & Neuroscience, Creighton University, Omaha, NE, USA
*
Corresponding author: T. W. Wilson; Email: tony.wilson@boystown.org
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Over the past decade, transdiagnostic indicators in relation to neurobiological processes have provided extensive insight into youth’s risk for psychopathology. During development, exposure to childhood trauma and dysregulation (i.e., so-called AAA symptomology: anxiety, aggression, and attention problems) puts individuals at a disproportionate risk for developing psychopathology and altered network-level neural functioning. Evidence for the latter has emerged from resting-state fMRI studies linking mental health symptoms and aberrations in functional networks (e.g., cognitive control (CCN), default mode networks (DMN)) in youth, although few of these investigations have used longitudinal designs. Herein, we leveraged a three-year longitudinal study to identify whether traumatic exposures and concomitant dysregulation trigger changes in the developmental trajectories of resting-state functional networks involved in cognitive control (N = 190; 91 females; time 1 Mage = 11.81). Findings from latent growth curve analyses revealed that greater trauma exposure predicted increasing connectivity between the CCN and DMN across time. Greater levels of dysregulation predicted reductions in within-network connectivity in the CCN. These findings presented in typically developing youth corroborate connectivity patterns reported in clinical populations, suggesting there is predictive utility in using transdiagnostic indicators to forecast alterations in resting-state networks implicated in psychopathology.

Type
Regular Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

In recent years, transdiagnostic processes have been highlighted as providing potent links between childhood traumatic experiences and subsequent psychopathology (McLaughlin et al., Reference McLaughlin, Colich, Rodman and Weissman2020). There is consistent evidence that childhood traumatic exposures put individuals at an elevated risk of developing anxiety, depression, substance use disorders, and post-traumatic stress disorders (Buckingham & Daniolos, Reference Buckingham and Daniolos2013; Gur et al., Reference Gur, Moore, Rosen, Barzilay, Roalf, Calkins, Ruparel, Scott, Almasy, Satterthwaite, Shinohara and Gur2019; Mills et al., Reference Mills, Scott, Alati, O’Callaghan, Najman and Strathearn2013). Moreover, an overlapping literature has shown that such traumatic experiences affect resting-state functional connectivity within key neural networks associated with transdiagnostic markers (e.g., cognitive control, emotion dysregulation, irritability; Barch, Reference Barch2017; Beauchaine & Cicchetti, Reference Beauchaine and Cicchetti2019; Demir-Lira et al., Reference Demir-Lira, Voss, O’Neil, Briggs-Gowan, Wakschlag and Booth2016; Klein et al., Reference Klein, Dougherty, Kessel, Silver and Carlson2021; McTeague et al., Reference McTeague, Goodkind and Etkin2016) known to predict poor outcomes (Lu et al., Reference Lu, Gao, Wei, Wang, Hu, Huang, Xu and Li2017; Luo et al., Reference Luo, Yu, Chen, Lin, Wu, Yao, Li, Wu and Peng2022; Stone et al., Reference Stone, Amole, Cyranowski and Swartz2018). By and large, this work has had limitations in sample size, use of cross-sectional designs, and retrospective adult reporting. The current study addressed these limitations by employing a longitudinal design in youth to interrogate whether traumatic experiences and concomitant transdiagnostic processes (i.e., dysregulation) alter developmental trajectories of key resting-state functional networks (e.g., cognitive/executive control network (CCN), default mode network (DMN)).

Much of the literature linking trauma and aberrant functional connectivity has focused on resting-state activity. These studies have yielded crucial insights into trauma-related sequalae in neurodevelopment broadly and functional organization of neural networks specifically. Of particular relevance to the current investigation are altered patterns of within- and between-network connectivity in the CCN (i.e., the executive control network, or the fronto-parietal network). The CCN consists of functional connections between hubs in the frontal and posterior parietal cortices and is critical to adaptive engagement in goal-directed behaviors (Seeley et al., Reference Seeley, Menon, Schatzberg, Keller, Glover, Kenna, Reiss and Greicius2007; Yeo et al., Reference Yeo, Krienen, Sepulcre, Sabuncu, Lashkari, Hollinshead, Roffman, Smoller, Zöllei, Polimeni, Fischl, Liu and Buckner2011). Importantly, disruptions in cognitive control and the underlying neural circuitry have previously been implicated in an array of mental health disorders (Sheffield & Barch, Reference Sheffield and Barch2016; Solomon et al., Reference Solomon, Hogeveen, Libero and Nordahl2017; Sutcubasi et al., Reference Sutcubasi, Metin, Kurban, Metin, Beser and Sonuga-Barke2020; Williams, Reference Williams2016), as well as following childhood trauma (Silveira et al., Reference Silveira, Shah, Nooner, Nagel, Tapert, de Bellis and Mishra2020; Wu et al., Reference Wu, Wu, Wu, Zhan, Peng, Wang, Zhao, Ning, Zheng and She2021), underscoring its potential as a transdiagnostic indicator. In individuals with childhood trauma exposure, patterns of CCN functional connectivity are largely mixed, with reports of some (Rakesh et al., Reference Rakesh, Kelly, Vijayakumar, Zalesky, Allen and Whittle2021) or no longitudinal changes in within- or between-network connectivity as a function of trauma exposure (Chahal et al., Reference Chahal, Miller, Yuan, Buthmann and Gotlib2022; Rakesh et al., Reference Rakesh, Allen and Whittle2021). Other longitudinal work has highlighted that connectivity patterns of key hubs in the CCN (e.g., intraparietal sulcus) as portending subsequent executive dysfunction and substance misuse in youth following trauma exposure, suggesting predictive ability of CCN hubs in long-term outcomes of major risk factors for psychopathology (Silveira et al., Reference Silveira, Shah, Nooner, Nagel, Tapert, de Bellis and Mishra2020). Cross-sectionally and longitudinally, children with trauma exposure exhibiting lower CCN connectivity seem to be at elevated risk of inflammatory reactivity, suggesting that trauma may induce neurobiological alterations to the stress system, including stress-sensitive regions within the CCN (Miller et al., Reference Miller, Chen, Finegood, Lam, Weissman-Tsukamoto, Leigh, Hoffer, Carroll, Brody, Parrish and Nusslock2021; Nusslock et al., Reference Nusslock, Brody, Armstrong, Carroll, Sweet, Yu, Barton, Hallowell, Chen, Higgins, Parrish, Wang and Miller2019).

In addition to childhood trauma exposure, dysregulation has been highlighted as a robust predictor of a multitude of psychopathological diagnoses and comorbidities, making it a relevant transdiagnostic risk factor (Althoff & Ametti, Reference Althoff and Ametti2021). Dysregulation is typically defined as impairments in regulating affect, behavior, and cognition (ABCs), with some evidence for an inverted-U developmental trajectory, with peak symptoms in early adolescence (Deutz et al., Reference Deutz, Vossen, Haan, Deković, Baar and Prinzie2018). As a behavioral phenotype, it does not fit neatly within classic diagnostic classification systems, as it shares features of both internalizing and externalizing symptomology. The Child Behavior Checklist Dysregulation Profile (CBCL-DP) consists of three symptom subscales, including anxiety/depression, attention problems, and aggression, and is among the most commonly used instruments measuring dysregulation in youth. Specifically, prior studies have shown that as early as preschool age, higher dysregulation symptoms are related to persistent impairments in emotional functioning (e.g., self-regulation; Kim et al., Reference Kim, Carlson, Meyer, Bufferd, Dougherty, Dyson, Laptook, Olino and Klein2012). Longitudinal studies have documented that elevated dysregulation in children is highly predictive of internalizing, externalizing, personality, and substance use disorders, as well as suicidality in adolescence and adulthood, up to 20 years later (Althoff et al., Reference Althoff, Verhulst, Rettew, Hudziak and van der Ende2010; Halperin et al., Reference Halperin, Rucklidge, Powers, Miller and Newcorn2011; Holtmann et al., Reference Holtmann, Buchmann, Esser, Schmidt, Banaschewski and Laucht2011; Meyer et al., Reference Meyer, Carlson, Youngstrom, Ronsaville, Martinez, Gold, Hakak and Radke-Yarrow2009), suggesting that CBCL-DP carries meaningful transdiagnostic qualities. Thus, interest has grown across the research community in evaluating dysregulation symptoms as a developmental precursor and marker of general psychopathology (Bellani et al., Reference Bellani, Negri and Brambilla2012). Despite its clinical relevance, few studies have examined associations between dysregulation symptoms and functional connectivity patterns in the developing brain (McGough et al., Reference McGough, McCracken, Cho, Castelo, Sturm, Cowen, Piacentini and Loo2013), particularly in networks subserving the specific behaviors implicated in dysregulation (e.g., self-control, cognitive control). Although limited, extant findings suggest that otherwise healthy youth with elevated emotional and behavioral dysregulation exhibit altered intrinsic connections among networks thought to serve flexible cognitive control (e.g., ventromedial prefrontal cortex (PFC), insula, anterior cingulate; Hwang et al., Reference Hwang, Velanova and Luna2010). During development, greater dysregulation has been shown to relate to weaker resting-state functional connectivity between a number of regions pertinent to cognitive control, including amygdala to medial PFC (Park et al., Reference Park, Leonard, Saxler, Cyr, Gabrieli and Mackey2018), as well as between dorsolateral and ventromedial PFC, (Lopez et al., Reference Lopez, Luby, Belden and Barch2018), and between insular and amygdala regions (Bebko et al., Reference Bebko, Bertocci, Chase, Dwojak, Bonar, Almeida, Perlman, Versace, Schirda, Travis, Gill, Demeter, Diwadkar, Sunshine, Holland, Kowatch, Birmaher, Axelson, Horwitz and Phillips2015). These findings suggest that there are likely alterations in resting-state connectivity within the CCN that relate to dysregulation, but the extent to which dysregulation is associated with longitudinal aberrations in connectivity within the CCN and other key networks is unknown. Thus, we sought to fill this gap in the literature by examining whether dysregulation symptomology predicts changes in resting-state functional connectivity in these networks.

Existing studies using a developmental lens have made important longitudinal links between psychopathology outcomes in youth and within and between CCN connectivity (Chahal et al., Reference Chahal, Miller, Yuan, Buthmann and Gotlib2022; Rakesh et al., Reference Rakesh, Allen and Whittle2021). Despite these contributions, there have yet to be studies examining whether childhood trauma exposure and dysregulation modulate the developmental trajectories of within and between CCN connectivity during development. Moreover, despite associations between trauma and dysregulation, studies modeling these constructs together are limited. In other words, existing work has focused on neurodevelopmental trajectories predicting outcomes, and not how initial levels of potent risk factors forecast changes in CCN connectivity. The current study addressed the latter in an otherwise healthy sample of youth with variability in traumatic exposures and dysregulation behavior using a latent growth modeling approach. In so doing, the present study allowed for rigorous investigation of deviations from typical neurodevelopment associated with two interrelated transdiagnostic indicators of future mental health concerns.

Methods

Participants

A sample of 212 typically developing children and adolescents (106 males) were recruited to participate in the Developmental Chronnecto-Genomics study (Stephen et al., Reference Stephen, Solis, Janowich, Stern, Frenzel, Eastman, Mills, Embury, Coolidge, Heinrichs-Graham, Mayer, Liu, Wang, Wilson and Calhoun2021). Of those, a total of 7 participants were excluded due to poor quality T1 structural MRI data, and 15 had unusable or incomplete resting-state fMRI data. Thus, data from a sample of 190 typically-developing children and adolescents with usable eyes-open resting-state fMRI were examined (8–15 years old; meanage = 11.81 years, SD = 1.73; 91 females). The study was multisite, with 95 participants recruited at the University of Nebraska Medical Center (UNMC) and 95 participants from the Mind Research Network (MRN) for the initial study assessment. Participants were invited back to participate annually for three years (time between visits: meantime1to2 = 1.12 years, SD = 0.20, meantime2to3 = 1.09 years, SD = 0.24). Of the initial sample, 120 and 50 participants completed a usable resting-state fMRI scan for years 2 and 3, respectively. Inclusion criteria included English as a primary language, ages 9–15 at the time of their first visit, and participant and parent willingness to assent/consent. Exclusion criteria were as follows: inability to assent/consent, history of developmental delays and/or psychiatric disorders, history of neurological disorders, history of concussion or head injury, pregnancy, prenatal exposure to drugs, use of medications known to affect brain function, and magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) contraindications.

Ethical considerations

All parents and youth provided written consent and assent, respectively, prior to participating in the study. The appropriate institutional review boards for both study sites approved all study procedures.

Structural and functional MRI data acquisition

Participants underwent a structural T1-weighted magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scan during each visit (i.e., 3 scans total). Children recruited at UNMC were scanned using a Siemens 3T Skyra scanner and those at MRN were scanned using a Siemens 3T TIM Trio. A 32-channel head coil was used at both sites and all sequences were optimized to minimize inter-site differences. Structural MR images at both sites were acquired with an MPRAGE sequence with the following parameters: TR = 2400 milliseconds; TE = 1.94 milliseconds; flip angle = 8°; FoV = 256 mm; slice thickness = 1 mm (no gap); base resolution = 256; 192 slices; voxel size = 1 mm3. Eyes-open resting-state multiband fMRI data were also collected during each visit using a standard echo planar BOLD sequence with the following parameters: 650 volumes, TR = 0.46s, TE = 29 ms, FA = 44°, with a slice thickness of 3 mm (no gap); site 1: 48 sequential axial slices with a FOV of 268 × 268 mm (82 × 82 matrix), and site 2: 56 sequential axial slices with a FoV of 246 × 246 mm (82 × 82 matrix).

Functional network connectivity processing

Complete details of the resting-state functional connectivity (rsFC) preprocessing and analysis are reported in Supplementary Materials and recent publications (Agcaoglu et al., Reference Agcaoglu, Wilson, Wang, Stephen and Calhoun2019, Reference Agcaoglu, Wilson, Wang, Stephen and Calhoun2020; Taylor et al., Reference Taylor, Frenzel, Eastman, Embury, Agcaoglu, Wang, Stephen, Calhoun and Wilson2022). Briefly, scans were corrected for head motion and differences in slice timing, followed by despiking to reduce outliers. Data were warped into Montreal Neurological Institute space, and then rewarped to a study-specific template due to the age range of the participants (Agcaoglu et al., Reference Agcaoglu, Wilson, Wang, Stephen and Calhoun2019, Reference Agcaoglu, Wilson, Wang, Stephen and Calhoun2020). Group independent component analysis (ICA; (Calhoun & Adali, Reference Calhoun and Adali2012) of the preprocessed functional data yielded 150 spatially-independent components, 51 of which were identified as components comprising seven different resting-state networks (RSNs). rsFC was measured as the average Pearson correlation between different RSN time courses. The present study focused on the connectivity within the cognitive control network (CCN) and its connections with default mode network (DMN), sensorimotor (SM), visual (VIS), and auditory (AUD) networks. The CCN was comprised of connectivity among 11 subnetworks across frontal, insular, and parietal regions, as well as several key temporal areas (for complete details of regions, see Supplementary Table S1).

Child behavior checklist

During all three visits, a caregiver completed the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL, (Achenbach et al., Reference Achenbach, Dumenci and Rescorla2001)) to assess their child’s dysregulation behaviors over the past 6 months. The dysregulation profile is a summed score of the attention, aggression, and anxious/depressed subscales (Holtmann et al., Reference Holtmann, Buchmann, Esser, Schmidt, Banaschewski and Laucht2011). Scores from the first visit were used in the primary models and scores from all three visits were used in the alternative models. Raw scores were used in our models, as sex and age were both controlled for in our models.

Trauma history profile

Participants completed the self-report Trauma History Profile (THP), which was derived from the UCLA Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) Reaction Index for DSM IV (Steinberg et al., Reference Steinberg, Brymer, Decker and Pynoos2004) and assessed a variety of trauma types and events. Participants endorsed whether they experienced 12 different types of trauma in their lifetime (No = 0, Yes = 1). Example items include: “was hit, punched, kicked very hard (not play fighting)”, “in a bad accident, like a serious car accident or fall”, “had a painful or scary medical treatment”. A summed score of each participants’ trauma exposure was used (Figure S1). Information regarding the percentage of participants reporting on each trauma subtype can be found in the Supplemental Material (Figure S2).

Data analytic plan

We first computed descriptive statistics on demographics and all variables of interest. Variables entered into subsequent models were examined for violations of normality (i.e., skewness and kurtosis) and were transformed accordingly. We used ANOVAs and chi-square tests to determine whether participants who discontinued participation during time 2 and 3 were demographically (i.e., age, sex, race, and ethnicity) different from those who completed all 3 timepoints. Next, we fit a series of latent growth curve models (LGCM) to evaluate changes in intrinsic connectivity within and between the CCN across time, and whether these changes were associated with dysregulation symptoms and trauma exposure at time 1. Each set of networks was modeled separately, with 5 final models (i.e., CCN-CCN, CCN-AUD, CCN-SM, CCN-DMN, CCN-VIS). We first fit the base LGCM for change in connectivity across the 3-year period, without control variables. The intercept was defined by intrinsic network connectivity at each time point, constrained to 1. The slope was defined by intrinsic network connectivity to 0, 1, and 2 for timepoints 1, 2, and 3. The next set of models added in sex (0=males, 1=females), age at time 1, and data collection site (0 = UNMC site; 1 = MRN site) as control variables. Latent intercept and slope variables were regressed on all control variables to account for demographic and site differences on the network connectivity measures. The third and final set of models added in the dysregulation and trauma exposure scores from time 1. Total raw scores for both scales were regressed on age, sex, and site to account for potential demographic developmental effects and differences between sites. The latent intercept and slope variables were regressed on the dysregulation and trauma exposure scores and previously added control variables. The dysregulation, trauma scales, and control variables were permitted to freely correlate. All parameters were freely estimated. The final models enabled us to discern whether symptoms and trauma exposure (at time 1) were predictive of the baseline (i.e., intercept) and rate of change (i.e., slope) in intrinsic network connectivity across time.

We also fit an alternative set of models testing the opposite effects whereby time 1 connectivity predicted change in dysregulation across the three timepoints. This was done to test whether alterative, plausible models offer a more robust account of brain-symptomology correspondence during this particular developmental window. Note that trauma exposure was assessed in all participants at time 1; only those participants who endorsed a traumatic exposure at time 2 and/or 3 were administered the THP at subsequent time points. Thus, the THP was not amenable to growth curve analyses in the current sample. To ensure that these models were as comparable as possible to the models examining time 1 symptomology predicting changes in connectivity, we included the same set of covariates.

We examined the goodness of fit for each model using standard criteria (Hu & Bentler, Reference Hu and Bentler1999), including root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) < .06, and comparative fit index (CFI) > .95. We also examined the χ2 test of model fit, where a nonsignificant result indicates good model fit. In addition, model fit comparisons were inspected, including absolute fit indices such as Akaike’s Information Criterion and Bayesian Information Criterion (Akaike, Reference Akaike1974; Rissanen, Reference Rissanen1983). To determine model fit improvement, we primarily relied on χ2 difference tests for the nested models (i.e., baseline, covariates, final model) for each network. All models were tested in Mplus (v8.6).

Missing data

Of the 190 children recruited at time 1, not all participants had a resting state, structural scan, or CBCL dysregulation scores (for the alternative models) at times 2 and 3 (reported in Table 1). We conducted each LGCM with and without missing data estimation using full-information maximum likelihood (FIML) and the same conclusions were reached. In order to reduce potential bias from data missing at random, we report results using FIML estimation.

Table 1. Sample demographics and study variables of interest

AI/A = American Indian/Alaskan; A = Asian; AUD = auditory network; B/AA = Black, African American; CCN = cognitive control network; DMN = default mode network; F = female; M = male; M = mixed race; N = not reported; NL = Not Latino/a; SM = sensorimotor network; T1 = time 1, T2 = time 2, T3 = time 3; VIS = visual network; W = White. Dysregulation symptoms are reported as raw values from the CBCL.

Results

Demographic & descriptive statistics

Demographic and descriptive statistics of the final sample are reported in Table 1. Participants who did and did not continue study participation across the 3 time points did not differ by age (F (2,187) = 0.68,p = .50), sex (χ2(2,N = 190) = 1.45,p = .48), ethnicity (χ2(8,N = 182) = 11.58,p = .17), or race (χ2(2,N = 190) = 0.11,p = .95). In addition, participants were demographically well-matched across the 2 study sites with respect to age (t (188) = 0.001,p = .99), sex (χ2(1,N = 190) = 0.53,p = .47), and race (χ2(4,N = 182) = 6.79, p = .15). There were, however, a significantly greater proportion of Latino/a participants at the MRN study site compared to the UNMC site (χ2(1,N = 190) = 19.39,p < .001). There were no site differences in study retention (χ2(2,N = 190) = 2.82,p = .24).

Correlations among study variables of interest are reported in Table S2. An example LGCM is illustrated in Figure 1 and model fit results and comparisons for each step of the analysis are reported in Table S3. With the exception of the CCN-SM and CCN-AUD models (discussed below), the final models for the CCN-CCN, CCN-DMN, and CCN-VIS rsFC trajectories had good to excellent fit. In the next section, we report the LGCM model results from each of the RSNs CCN-CCN, CCN-AUD, CCN-SM, CCN-DMN, CCN-VIS.

Figure 1. Example latent growth curve model of cognitive control connectivity. I = Intercept, S = Slope. Dysreg = dysregulation was measured via the CBCL at time 1. Trauma = trauma exposure was collected via the UCLA Trauma History Profile at Time 1. CCN-CCN = within cognitive control network functional network connectivity. Sex, age at time 1, and study site were included as covariates for the intercept and slope, as well as each of the symptomology predictors.

For the alternative LGCM estimating dysregulation symptoms across time, the baseline model had poor model fit (χ2 (1) = 4.00,p = .045; RMSEA = .10, 90% CI [.02,.26]; CFI = .99). Thus, we did not proceed with modeling or interpreting the time 1 rsFC predicting intercept and slope of dysregulation.

LGCM model results for cognitive control network connectivity

Results describing the base models and models with only the control variables are reported in the Supplement. The final models included age, site, sex, dysregulation symptoms scores, and trauma exposure as predictors of the intercept and slope of change in CCN rsFC. Based upon χ2 difference tests, there were significant decrements in model fit for the CCN-SM (χ2diff = 18.93, p <.001) and CCN-AUD (χ2diff= 11.56, p = .01) when covariates were entered into the model (i.e., age, sex, site), making the parameter estimates uninterpretable. The remaining 3 models had good to excellent fit. An illustrative set of results for the CCN-CCN model are illustrated in Figure 2. Tables S4S6 contain a complete report of model results. In what follows, we report model results for the CCN-CCN, CCN-DMN, and CCN-VIS networks.

Figure 2. Latent growth curve model results for connectivity between cognitive control regions. Final model results in which sex, age at time 1, study site, CBCL dysregulation symptoms, and trauma exposure all predict the latent intercept and slope of change in cognitive control network connectivity. Solid lines indicate statistically significant estimates at p < .05. All estimates are unstandardized. I = intercept, S = slope. Dysreg = dysregulation measured via the CBCL; trauma = trauma measured via the UCLA THP at time 1. CCN = cognitive control network connectivity. Sex, age at time 1, and study site were included as covariates for the intercept and slope, as well as each of the symptomology predictors. For sex, males = 0 and females = 1.

CCN-CCN network connectivity results

In terms of the effects of interest, dysregulation symptoms were significantly associated with baseline rsFC within the CCN network (b = 0.004, p < .001), such that youths with greater dysregulation symptoms tended to have greater baseline CCN-CCN rsFC. In addition, elevated dysregulation symptoms at time 1 related to decreasing CCN-CCN rsFC across time (b = −0.002, p = .03) (Fig. 3a). Trauma exposure did not relate to baseline CCN-CCN rsFC (b = −0.002, p = .57). In addition, trauma exposure at time 1 did not relate to changes in CCN-CCN rsFC across time (b = 0.003, p = .40).

Figure 3. Associations between dysregulation symptomology and trauma at T1 and functional connectivity slope of change. Scatterplots displaying associations between trauma exposure or CBCL symptomology at time 1 (i.e., dysregulation symptoms) and the estimated slope of change in functional network connectivity at rest. Slope values were adjusted by regressing out effects of other variables in the latent growth curve model (e.g., 3a slope was adjusted for age, sex, site, and trauma exposure). Symptoms and trauma exposure were also adjusted for covariates in the model (i.e., age, sex, and site). CCN = cognitive control network, DMN = default mode network; T1 = time 1.

The base model showed that youths tended to have positive within-network CCN rsFC at time 1 (mean = 0.01, p < .001), though there was no systematic change in rsFC over time (mean = 0.01, p = .39; details in supplement). Age at time 1 was not associated with baseline rsFC (b = -0.005, p = .11) or changes in rsFC across time (b = 0.006, p = .07). Age did not correspond with dysregulation symptoms (b = −0.298, p = .27) or trauma exposure (b = −0.075, p = .35). Sex was related to baseline CCN-CCN rsFC (b = 0.024, p = .03), such that females tended to have greater rsFC between regions of the CCN network compared to males. There were no sex differences in change across time in rsFC among CCN network regions (b = −0.011, p = .32). Moreover, males and females did not differ in their number of dysregulation symptoms (b = −0.345, p = .71) or trauma exposures (b = 0.014, p = .96). Study site was not associated with baseline rsFC within the CCN network (b = −0.013, p = .22), nor was it associated with change in rsFC within the CCN network (b = 0.002, p =. 87). Study site was also not related to dysregulation symptoms (b = −0.209, p = .82) or trauma exposure (b = −0.098, p = .72).

CCN-DMN network connectivity results

Dysregulation symptoms were significantly associated with baseline CCN-DMN rsFC (b = 0.003, p = .004), where youths with a greater number of dysregulation symptoms had greater baseline CCN-DMN rsFC. Time 1 dysregulation symptoms did not predict the rate of change in CCN-DMN rsFC across time (b = −0.002, p = .06). Although trauma exposure did not relate to baseline CCN-DMN rsFC (b = −0.003, p = .27), more trauma exposures reported at time 1 predicted increases in CCN-DMN rsFC across time (b = 0.007, p = .036) (Fig. 3b).

In the base model, youths generally exhibited a pattern of positive between-network rsFC in CCN-DMN at time 1 (mean = 0.08, p < .001), though there were no changes in the rate of change in CCN-DMN rsFC over time (mean = 0.01, p = .34). Age at time 1 was not related to baseline levels of CCN-DMN rsFC (b = -0.001, p = .81) nor change in rsFC across time (b = 0.00, p = .95). Age did not correspond with dysregulation symptoms (b = −0.292, p = .28) or trauma exposure (b = −0.077, p = .34). Sex was not related to baseline CCN-DMN rsFC (b = 0.017, p = .12) or the rate of change in CCN-DMN rsFC (b = −0.010, p = .38). Moreover, males and females did not differ in their number of dysregulation symptoms (b = −0.342, p = .72) or trauma exposures (b = 0.019, p = .94). Study site was associated with baseline CCN-DMN rsFC (b = −0.021, p = .05), whereby the MRN site had greater baseline rsFC compared to the UNMC site. Though, study site was not systematically related to changes in CCN-DMN rsFC across time (b = 0.014, p = .21). Study site was also not related to dysregulation symptoms (b = −0.342, p = .72) or trauma exposure (b = −0.100, p = .72).

CCN-VIS network connectivity results

Dysregulation symptoms were significantly associated with baseline levels of CCN-VIS rsFC (b = 0.003, p = .005), where youths with greater dysregulation symptoms had greater baseline CCN-VIS rsFC. Dysregulation symptoms at time 1 did not correspond with the slope of change in CCN-VIS rsFC over time (b = −0.001, p = .08). Similarly, trauma exposure did not relate to baseline CCN-VIS rsFC (b = −0.001, p = .78) nor the rate of change in CCN-VIS rsFC (b = −0.001, p = .75).

The base model showed that youths tended to show a pattern of positive between-network CCN-VIS rsFC at time 1 (mean = 0.07, p < .001), though there were no significant changes in rsFC over time (mean = 0.004, p = .46). Age at time 1 related to baseline CCN-VIS rsFC (b = −0.010, p = .005), such that older participants showed lower levels of CCN-VIS rsFC. Age at time 1 was not associated with fluctuations in CCN-VIS rsFC across time (b = 0.006, p = .09). In addition, age did not relate to dysregulation symptoms (b = −0.292, p = .28) or trauma exposure (b = −0.075, p = .35). Sex was related to baseline CCN-VIS rsFC (b = 0.027, p = .03), such that females tended to have greater rsFC than males. There were also sex differences in the rate of change in CCN-VIS rsFC across time, with females exhibiting more sharply decreasing connectivity relative to males (b = −0.030, p = .007). There were no sex differences in dysregulation symptoms (b = −0.295, p = .75) or trauma exposure (b = 0.013, p = .96). Study site was not associated with baseline CCN-VIS rsFC (b = −0.012, p = .31), nor was it related to the rate of change in CCN-VIS rsFC (b = −0.003, p = .80). Study site was also not related to dysregulation symptoms (b = −0.194, p = .84) or trauma exposure (b = −0.098, p = .72).

Discussion

The present study examined the extent to which key transdiagnostic precursors to psychopathology - trauma exposure and dysregulation symptoms - predict longitudinal fluctuations in rsFC of the cognitive control network (CCN). We report two key findings regarding changes in rsFC across time. First, youths with higher dysregulation generally showed greater within-network CCN rsFC at time 1; those with higher levels of dysregulation tended to have decreasing within CCN rsFC across adolescence. Second, greater levels of trauma exposure at time 1 predicted increasing CCN-DMN connectivity over time. No other resting-state networks demonstrated changes related to these transdiagnostic indicators. In addition, although not a core aim of the study, we did not uncover age-related changes in network connectivity across time, as others have previously reported in intra- and inter-network connectivity patterns of the DMN and CCN (e.g., Sherman et al., Reference Sherman, Rudie, Pfeifer, Masten, McNealy and Dapretto2014). An alternative set of models testing whether rsFC at baseline predicted variability in dysregulation yielded poor model fits. It is plausible that changes in dysregulation over the developmental window examined here tended to be non-linear, which is consistent with work documenting trajectories of the CBCL dysregulation profile in typically developing youth (Deutz et al., Reference Deutz, Vossen, Haan, Deković, Baar and Prinzie2018).

Consistent with prior cross-sectional and longitudinal designs (Bebko et al., Reference Bebko, Bertocci, Chase, Dwojak, Bonar, Almeida, Perlman, Versace, Schirda, Travis, Gill, Demeter, Diwadkar, Sunshine, Holland, Kowatch, Birmaher, Axelson, Horwitz and Phillips2015; Lopez et al., Reference Lopez, Luby, Belden and Barch2018; Park et al., Reference Park, Leonard, Saxler, Cyr, Gabrieli and Mackey2018), our findings show that in an otherwise healthy sample, greater dysregulation is linked to decreasing rsFC among regions of the CCN. It should be noted that the present study is, to the authors’ knowledge, the only systematic investigation of associations between the CBCL dysregulated profile and changes in CCN connectivity. Other studies include nodes commonly attributed to the CCN (e.g., insula, dorsomedial/lateral prefrontal cortices), but not necessarily the traditional fronto-parietal connections that comprise the CCN. In typically developing youth, lower within CCN connectivity has been reported previously, as the CCN is expected to be an incohesive connector during development, meaning it is likely to exhibit higher between-network connectivity and lower within-network connectivity (Gu et al., Reference Gu, Satterthwaite, Medaglia, Yang, Gur, Gur and Bassett2015). However, these patterns shift during adolescence into adulthood, as brain networks become more segregated and modular, within-network rsFC is expected to increase due to functional specialization (Grayson & Fair, Reference Grayson and Fair2017). With these predictions in mind, findings reported here contribute two insights: (1) greater initial within CCN rsFC in youth with more dysregulation may be a marker of atypical trajectories to come and (2) longitudinal trajectories in those with higher dysregulation seem to indicate aberrant decreases in CCN rsFC, as the normative trajectory is characterized by increases in CCN connectivity with development.

These findings offer unique insights into the dynamics of the broader CCN while providing convergent evidence of greater dysregulated behavior being linked to weaker rsFC across key regions in the CCN reported in pairwise approaches (Park et al., Reference Park, Leonard, Saxler, Cyr, Gabrieli and Mackey2018). Results suggest that youth with elevated dysregulation may exhibit a functional de-coupling of regions within the CCN during a developmental window in which there should be enhanced coupling. This functional de-coupling may challenge youth’s ability to effectively self-regulate behavior in concert with the mood instability and attentional problems that are present in most accounts of dysregulation, all of which are generally predictive of subsequent psychopathology (Holtmann et al., Reference Holtmann, Buchmann, Esser, Schmidt, Banaschewski and Laucht2011). Notably, the current set of findings are among the first to show dysregulation predicting longitudinal changes in CCN connectivity in youth, as prior work leveraging longitudinal designs have not examined change in these networks per se (Lopez et al., Reference Lopez, Luby, Belden and Barch2018). Reduced within-network CCN rsFC has been highlighted as a potential transdiagnostic vulnerability, as it seems to be shared across a number of psychiatric disorders, including anxiety (Geiger et al., Reference Geiger, Domschke, Ipser, Hattingh, Baldwin, Lochner and Stein2016), and depression (Stange et al., Reference Stange, Bessette, Jenkins, Peters, Feldhaus, Crane, Ajilore, Jacobs, Watkins and Langenecker2017), as well as subclinical levels of depression in adults (Hwang et al., Reference Hwang, Egorova, Yang, Zhang, Chen, Yang, Hu, Sun, Tu and Kong2015) and emergent depression in youth (Pan et al., Reference Pan, Xu, Zhou, Chen, Wei, Lu, Shang, Wang and Huang2020). Thus, the current study contributes to a burgeoning literature that has largely been conducted in adults, by demonstrating that a transdiagnostic behavioral profile (i.e., dysregulation) that is present during development also corresponds with the emergence of selective rsFC reductions in the CCN and is a potent predictor of future psychopathology and a shared neurobiological feature of multiple psychiatric conditions (Kaiser et al., Reference Kaiser, Andrews-Hanna, Wager and Pizzagalli2015; McTeague et al., Reference McTeague, Goodkind and Etkin2016) (Kaiser et al., Reference Kaiser, Andrews-Hanna, Wager and Pizzagalli2015; McTeague et al., Reference McTeague, Goodkind and Etkin2016). It may be that youth with greater dysregulation are at heightened risk of developing rsFC patterns that typify generalized psychopathology; in concert, dysregulation may represent a malleable set of behaviors that if targeted early in development, could be amenable to preventative interventions to more adaptively engage the CCN.

Another key finding was that greater trauma exposure predicted increasing functional connectivity between CCN-DMN across time. Existing literature linking trauma exposure with CCN-DMN connectivity is largely mixed (Ross & Cisler, Reference Ross and Cisler2020) and sparse in developmental samples (Sheynin et al., Reference Sheynin, Duval, Lokshina, Scott, Angstadt, Kessler, Zhang, Gur, Gur and Liberzon2020). However, findings of greater connectivity between the CCN-DMN have been shown in pediatric samples with PTSD compared to controls (Patriat et al., Reference Patriat, Birn, Keding and Herringa2016; Viard et al., Reference Viard, Mutlu, Chanraud, Guenolé, Egler, Gérardin, Baleyte, Dayan, Eustache and Guillery-Girard2019), suggesting that the pattern of increasing connectivity between these networks may be indicative of emergent neurodevelopmental abnormalities seen in psychiatric samples who are at a similar developmental stage. DMN and CCN are thought to have competing activation patterns, with regions of the DMN engaged during more internally-oriented states, and the CCN engaged during goal-directed tasks (Fox et al., Reference Fox, Zhang, Snyder and Raichle2009). Further, studies have shown that connectivity between the DMN and CCN tends to be negatively correlated (i.e., anticorrelated) in healthy adults during rest (Chai et al., Reference Chai, Castañón, Öngür and Whitfield-Gabrieli2012; Fox et al., Reference Fox, Zhang, Snyder and Raichle2009). Thus, elevated levels of DMN-CCN connectivity at rest likely signify a disruption in the normative patterns of connectivity, and our finding of trauma-predicted increases in connectivity between DMN-CCN across time may indicate that trauma represents a significant risk factor for alterations in neurodevelopment. It is an open question whether such trauma-related increases in connectivity among the DMN and CCN are an indicator that CCN regions are less segregated from the DMN, which would stand in contrast to the expected increasing segregation of large-scale resting-state networks that occurs during adolescence (Grayson & Fair, Reference Grayson and Fair2017; Sherman et al., Reference Sherman, Rudie, Pfeifer, Masten, McNealy and Dapretto2014). As others have shown in prior work, reduced segregation between task-positive networks and the DMN may be a transdiagnostic mechanism by which core behavioral deficits emerge in psychiatric disorders (Owens et al., Reference Owens, Yuan, Hahn, Albaugh, Allgaier, Chaarani, Potter and Garavan2020).

Though this study did not specifically aim to test sex differences, we uncovered several noteworthy patterns. First, the current sample did not have higher levels of dysregulation in females than males, as others have reported (Mbekou et al., Reference Mbekou, Gignac, MacNeil, Mackay and Renaud2014; but see Boomsma et al., Reference Boomsma, Rebollo, Derks, van Beijsterveldt, Althoff, Rettew and Hudziak2006). This may be due to sampling differences, as prior studies have focused on treatment-seeking samples whereas the present sample is community-based. Moreover, prior longitudinal work has shown that there may not be initial sex differences in dysregulation levels, but that elevated levels in males and females confer risk for different psychopathology diagnostic outcomes; females are at elevated risk of mood disorders, while males are at elevated risk of substance use disorders and conduct-related disorders (Althoff et al., Reference Althoff, Verhulst, Rettew, Hudziak and van der Ende2010). Second, findings revealed that females had higher baseline within-CCN and CCN-VIS connectivity compared to males. In addition, females had steeper decreases in CCN-VIS connectivity across time relative to males. There is a scarcity of findings indicating that males and females have divergent resting-state connectivity patterns and brain function in general, with sex accounting for approximately 1% of variance in brain function and structure across modalities, including resting state (Eliot et al., Reference Eliot, Ahmed, Khan and Patel2021). Thus, given that the current sample is within the pubertal window, which is known to onset earlier in females than males, we speculate that the results may reflect differences in pubertal status, which has previously been linked to development of resting-state networks (Gracia-Tabuenca et al., Reference Gracia-Tabuenca, Moreno, Barrios and Alcauter2021). In other words, the sex differences reported here are likely due to puberty, not sex in and of itself. This interpretation should be explicitly followed up in future analyses designed to evaluate pubertal status and/or hormones in youth (e.g., Ladouceur et al., Reference Ladouceur, Henry, Ojha, Shirtcliff and Silk2023; Penhale et al., Reference Penhale, Picci, Ott, Taylor, Frenzel, Eastman, Wang, Calhoun, Stephen and Wilson2022).

The current study has many strengths, but there are several limitations that should be acknowledged. First, the quantification of trauma is based upon a cumulative exposure approach (Evans et al., Reference Evans, Li and Whipple2013), which lacks specificity in terms of identifying subtypes or dimensions of trauma that may lead to unique and dissociable alterations to neurodevelopment. For instance, recent theoretical frameworks have highlighted threat and deprivation experiences as specific early adversity subtypes that predict alterations to neural circuits in importantly different ways (Machlin et al., Reference Machlin, Miller, Snyder, McLaughlin and Sheridan2019; McLaughlin & Sheridan, Reference McLaughlin and Sheridan2016). Future work building upon the current study would benefit from incorporating a more dimensional, rather than a cumulative risk approach to delineating changes in functional connectivity patterns of key resting-state networks following specific trauma exposure types. Relatedly, other transdiagnostic measures that have recently been highlighted as potent predictors of subsequent psychopathology (e.g., dimensional psychopathology or a p-factor; Parkes et al., Reference Parkes, Moore, Calkins, Cook, Cieslak, Roalf, Wolf, Gur, Gur, Satterthwaite and Bassett2021) would be a promising avenue to pursue. Moreover, examining the extent to which neural connectivity patterns mediate associations between earlier transdiagnostic indicators and longer-term psychopathology outcomes would be a crucial next step in this line of inquiry (Rakesh et al., Reference Rakesh, Kelly, Vijayakumar, Zalesky, Allen and Whittle2021). Finally, it is worth noting that the current study focused on deviations from normative developmental trajectories in an otherwise healthy sample, which likely limits generalizability to samples with clinical levels of psychopathology. That said, there is utility in examining subclinical and/or transdiagnostic processes underlying neurobiological responses to trauma experiences (Picci et al., Reference Picci, Christopher-Hayes, Petro, Taylor, Eastman, Frenzel, Wang, Stephen, Calhoun and Wilson2022a; Picci et al., Reference Picci, Taylor, Killanin, Eastman, Frenzel, Wang, Stephen, Calhoun and Wilson2022b; Taylor et al., Reference Taylor, Eastman, Frenzel, Embury, Wang, Stephen, Calhoun, Badura-Brack and Wilson2021), as these efforts may promote eventual discoveries of preclinical biomarkers for psychopathology that may emerge during development.

Taken together, the present study offers novel evidence for two putative predictors of psychopathology forecasting alterations to large-scale neural networks. Findings here suggest that dysregulated behavior and trauma exposure uniquely predict changes in within-network connectivity in the CCN and between-network connectivity between the CCN-DMN, respectively. These findings corroborate prior literature showing comparable connectivity patterns in cross-sectional designs and in samples with clinical levels of psychopathology. This work represents one of the few longitudinal investigations revealing long-term developmental associations between transdiagnostic indicators and emergent, altered connectivity patterns seen in clinical populations in an otherwise healthy sample.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579423001013.

Funding statement

This work was supported by the National Science Foundation (#1539067 to TWW, YPW, JMS, and VDC and #2112455 to VDC), the National Institutes of Health (R01-MH121101, R01-MH116782, and P20-GM144641 to TWW; R01-EB020407 and R01-MH118695 to VDC), and At Ease, USA. Funding agencies had no part in the study design or the writing of this report.

Competing interests

None.

References

Achenbach, T. M., Dumenci, L., & Rescorla, L. A. (2001). Ratings of relations between DSM-IV diagnostic categories and items of the CBCL/6-18, TRF, and YSR. University of Vermont Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families.Google Scholar
Agcaoglu, O., Wilson, T. W., Wang, Y., Stephen, J., & Calhoun, V. D. (2019). Resting state connectivity differences in eyes open versus eyes closed conditions. Human Brain Mapping, 40(8), 24882498. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24539 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Agcaoglu, O., Wilson, T. W., Wang, Y.-P., Stephen, J. M., & Calhoun, V. D. (2020). Dynamic resting-state connectivity differences in eyes open versus eyes closed conditions. Brain Connectivity, 10(9), 504519. https://doi.org/10.1089/brain.2020.0768 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Akaike, H. (1974). A new look at the statistical model identification. IEEE Transactions on Automatic Control, 19(6), 716723. https://doi.org/10.1109/TAC.1974.1100705 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Althoff, R. R., & Ametti, M. (2021). Measurement of dysregulation in children and adolescents. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 30(2), 321333. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chc.2020.10.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Althoff, R. R., Verhulst, F. C., Rettew, D. C., Hudziak, J. J., & van der Ende, J. (2010). Adult outcomes of childhood dysregulation: A 14-year follow-up study. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 49(11), 11051116.e1. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2010.08.006 Google ScholarPubMed
Barch, D. M. (2017). The neural correlates of transdiagnostic dimensions of psychopathology. American Journal of Psychiatry, 174(7), 613615. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2017.17030289 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beauchaine, T. P., & Cicchetti, D. (2019). Emotion dysregulation and emerging psychopathology: A transdiagnostic, transdisciplinary perspective. Development and Psychopathology, 31(3), 799804. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579419000671 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bebko, G., Bertocci, M., Chase, H., Dwojak, A., Bonar, L., Almeida, J., Perlman, S. B., Versace, A., Schirda, C., Travis, M., Gill, M. K., Demeter, C., Diwadkar, V., Sunshine, J., Holland, S., Kowatch, R., Birmaher, B., Axelson, D., Horwitz, S.Phillips, M.L. (2015). Decreased amygdala-insula resting state connectivity in behaviorally and emotionally dysregulated youth. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 231(1), 7786. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2014.10.015 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bellani, M., Negri, G. A. L., & Brambilla, P. (2012). The dysregulation profile in children and adolescents: A potential index for major psychopathology? Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences, 21(2), 155159. https://doi.org/10.1017/S2045796011000849 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boomsma, D. I., Rebollo, I., Derks, E. M., van Beijsterveldt, T. C. E. M., Althoff, R. R., Rettew, D. C., & Hudziak, J. J. (2006). Longitudinal stability of the CBCL-juvenile bipolar disorder phenotype: A study in dutch twins. Biological Psychiatry, 60(9), 912920. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.02.028 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buckingham, E. T., & Daniolos, P. (2013). Longitudinal outcomes for victims of child abuse. Current Psychiatry Reports, 15(2), 342. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-012-0342-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calhoun, V. D., & Adali, T. (2012). Multisubject independent component analysis of fMRI: A decade of intrinsic networks, default mode, and neurodiagnostic discovery. IEEE Reviews in Biomedical Engineering, 5, 6073. https://doi.org/10.1109/RBME.2012.2211076 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chahal, R., Miller, J. G., Yuan, J. P., Buthmann, J. L., & Gotlib, I. H. (2022). An exploration of dimensions of early adversity and the development of functional brain network connectivity during adolescence: Implications for trajectories of internalizing symptoms. Development and Psychopathology, 34(2), 557571. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579421001814 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chai, X. J., Castañón, A. N., Öngür, D., & Whitfield-Gabrieli, S. (2012). Anticorrelations in resting state networks without global signal regression. NeuroImage, 59(2), 14201428. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.048 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Demir-Lira, Ö.E., Voss, J. L., O’Neil, J. T., Briggs-Gowan, M. J., Wakschlag, L. S., & Booth, J. R. (2016). Early-life stress exposure associated with altered prefrontal resting-state fMRI connectivity in young children. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 107114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2016.02.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deutz, M. H. F., Vossen, H. G. M., Haan, A. D. D., Deković, M., Baar, A. L. V., & Prinzie, P. (2018). Normative development of the child behavior checklist dysregulation profile from early childhood to adolescence: Associations with personality pathology. Development and Psychopathology, 30(2), 437447. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579417000955 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eliot, L., Ahmed, A., Khan, H., & Patel, J. (2021). Dump the “dimorphism”: Comprehensive synthesis of human brain studies reveals few male-female differences beyond size. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 125, 667697. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.02.026 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, G. W., Li, D., & Whipple, S. S. (2013). Cumulative risk and child development. Psychological Bulletin, 139(6), 13421396. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031808 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fox, M. D., Zhang, D., Snyder, A. Z., & Raichle, M. E. (2009). The global signal and observed anticorrelated resting state brain networks. Journal of Neurophysiology, 101(6), 32703283. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.90777.2008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geiger, M. J., Domschke, K., Ipser, J., Hattingh, C., Baldwin, D. S., Lochner, C., & Stein, D. J. (2016). Altered executive control network resting-state connectivity in social anxiety disorder. The World Journal of Biological Psychiatry, 17(1), 4757. https://doi.org/10.3109/15622975.2015.1083613 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gracia-Tabuenca, Z., Moreno, M. B., Barrios, F. A., & Alcauter, S. (2021). Development of the brain functional connectome follows puberty-dependent nonlinear trajectories. NeuroImage, 229, 117769. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.117769 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grayson, D. S., & Fair, D. A. (2017). Development of large-scale functional networks from birth to adulthood: A guide to the neuroimaging literature. NeuroImage, 160, 1531. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.01.079 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gu, S., Satterthwaite, T. D., Medaglia, J. D., Yang, M., Gur, R. E., Gur, R. C., & Bassett, D. S. (2015). Emergence of system roles in normative neurodevelopment. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(44), 1368113686. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1502829112 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gur, R. E., Moore, T. M., Rosen, A. F. G., Barzilay, R., Roalf, D. R., Calkins, M. E., Ruparel, K., Scott, J. C., Almasy, L., Satterthwaite, T. D., Shinohara, R. T., Gur, R. C. (2019). Burden of environmental adversity associated with psychopathology, maturation, and brain behavior parameters in youths. JAMA Psychiatry, 76(9), 966975. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.0943 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Halperin, J. M., Rucklidge, J. J., Powers, R. L., Miller, C. J., & Newcorn, J. H. (2011). Childhood CBCL bipolar profile and adolescent/young adult personality disorders: A 9-year follow-up. Journal of Affective Disorders, 130(1-2), 155161. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2010.10.019 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holtmann, M., Buchmann, A. F., Esser, G., Schmidt, M. H., Banaschewski, T., & Laucht, M. (2011). The child behavior checklist-dysregulation profile predicts substance use, suicidality, and functional impairment: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52(2), 139147. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02309.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6(1), 155. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hwang, J. W., Egorova, N., Yang, X. Q., Zhang, W. Y., Chen, J., Yang, X. Y., Hu, L. J., Sun, S., Tu, Y., Kong, J. (2015). Subthreshold depression is associated with impaired resting-state functional connectivity of the cognitive control network. Translational Psychiatry, 5(11), e683e683. https://doi.org/10.1038/tp.2015.174 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hwang, K., Velanova, K., & Luna, B. (2010). Strengthening of top-down frontal cognitive control networks underlying the development of inhibitory control: A functional magnetic resonance imaging effective connectivity study. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(46), 1553515545. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2825-10.2010 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaiser, R. H., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Wager, T. D., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2015). Large-scale network dysfunction in major depressive disorder: A meta-analysis of resting-state functional connectivity. JAMA Psychiatry, 72(6), 603611. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.0071 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, J., Carlson, G. A., Meyer, S. E., Bufferd, S. J., Dougherty, L. R., Dyson, M. W., Laptook, R. S., Olino, T. M., & Klein, D. N. (2012). Correlates of the CBCL-dysregulation profile in preschool-aged children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53(9), 918926. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02546.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klein, D. N., Dougherty, L. R., Kessel, E. M., Silver, J., & Carlson, G. A. (2021). A transdiagnostic perspective on youth irritability. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 30(5), 437443. https://doi.org/10.1177/09637214211035101 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ladouceur, C. D., Henry, T., Ojha, A., Shirtcliff, E. A., & Silk, J. S. (2023). Fronto-amygdala resting state functional connectivity is associated with anxiety symptoms among adolescent girls more advanced in pubertal maturation. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 60, 101236. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2023.101236 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lopez, K. C., Luby, J. L., Belden, A. C., & Barch, D. M. (2018). Emotion dysregulation and functional connectivity in children with and without a history of major depressive disorder. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 18(2), 232248. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-018-0564-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lu, S., Gao, W., Wei, Z., Wang, D., Hu, S., Huang, M., Xu, Y., & Li, L. (2017). Intrinsic brain abnormalities in young healthy adults with childhood trauma: A resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging study of regional homogeneity and functional connectivity. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 51(6), 614623. https://doi.org/10.1177/0004867416671415 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luo, Q., Yu, H., Chen, J., Lin, X., Wu, Z., Yao, J., Li, Y., Wu, H., & Peng, H. (2022). Altered variability and concordance of dynamic resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging indices in patients with major depressive disorder and childhood trauma. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 16, 852799. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2022.852799 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Machlin, L., Miller, A. B., Snyder, J., McLaughlin, K. A., & Sheridan, M. A. (2019). Differential associations of deprivation and threat with cognitive control and fear conditioning in early childhood. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 13, 80. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00080 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mbekou, V., Gignac, M., MacNeil, S., Mackay, P., & Renaud, J. (2014). The CBCL dysregulated profile: An indicator of pediatric bipolar disorder or of psychopathology severity? Journal of Affective Disorders, 155, 299302. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2013.10.033 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGough, J. J., McCracken, J. T., Cho, A. L., Castelo, E., Sturm, A., Cowen, J., Piacentini, J., & Loo, S. K. (2013). A potential electroencephalography and cognitive biosignature for the child behavior checklist-dysregulation profile. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 52(11), 11731182. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2013.08.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLaughlin, K. A., Colich, N. L., Rodman, A. M., & Weissman, D. G. (2020). Mechanisms linking childhood trauma exposure and psychopathology: A transdiagnostic model of risk and resilience. BMC Medicine, 18(1), 96. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12916-020-01561-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLaughlin, K. A., & Sheridan, M. A. (2016). Beyond cumulative risk: A dimensional approach to childhood adversity. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 25(4), 239245. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721416655883 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McTeague, L. M., Goodkind, M. S., & Etkin, A. (2016). Transdiagnostic impairment of cognitive control in mental illness. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 83, 3746. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2016.08.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meyer, S., Carlson, G., Youngstrom, E., Ronsaville, D., Martinez, P., Gold, P., Hakak, R., & Radke-Yarrow, M. (2009). Long-term outcomes of youth who manifested the CBCL-pediatric bipolar disorder phenotype during childhood and/or adolescence. Journal of Affective Disorders, 113(3), 227235. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2008.05.024 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, G. E., Chen, E., Finegood, E. D., Lam, P. H., Weissman-Tsukamoto, R., Leigh, A. K. K., Hoffer, L., Carroll, A. L., Brody, G. H., Parrish, T. B., Nusslock, R. (2021). Resting-state functional connectivity of the central executive network moderates the relationship between neighborhood violence and proinflammatory phenotype in children. Biological Psychiatry, 90(3), 165172. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2021.03.008 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mills, R., Scott, J., Alati, R., O’Callaghan, M., Najman, J. M., & Strathearn, L. (2013). Child maltreatment and adolescent mental health problems in a large birth cohort. Child Abuse & Neglect, 37(5), 292302. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2012.11.008 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nusslock, R., Brody, G. H., Armstrong, C. C., Carroll, A. L., Sweet, L. H., Yu, T., Barton, A. W., Hallowell, E. S., Chen, E., Higgins, J. P., Parrish, T. B., Wang, L., Miller, G. E. (2019). Higher peripheral inflammatory signaling associated with lower resting-state functional brain connectivity in emotion regulation and central executive networks. Biological Psychiatry, 86(2), 153162. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2019.03.968 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Owens, M. M., Yuan, D., Hahn, S., Albaugh, M., Allgaier, N., Chaarani, B., Potter, A., & Garavan, H. (2020). Investigation of psychiatric and neuropsychological correlates of default mode network and dorsal attention network anticorrelation in children. Cerebral Cortex, 30(12), 60836096. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhaa143 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pan, F., Xu, Y., Zhou, W., Chen, J., Wei, N., Lu, S., Shang, D., Wang, J., & Huang, M. (2020). Disrupted intrinsic functional connectivity of the cognitive control network underlies disease severity and executive dysfunction in first-episode, treatment-naive adolescent depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 264, 455463. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2019.11.076 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Park, A. T., Leonard, J. A., Saxler, P. K., Cyr, A. B., Gabrieli, J. D. E., & Mackey, A. P. (2018). Amygdala-medial prefrontal cortex connectivity relates to stress and mental health in early childhood. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 13(4), 430439. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsy017 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parkes, L., Moore, T. M., Calkins, M. E., Cook, P. A., Cieslak, M., Roalf, D. R., Wolf, D. H., Gur, R. C., Gur, R. E., Satterthwaite, T. D., Bassett, D. S. (2021). Transdiagnostic dimensions of psychopathology explain individuals’ unique deviations from normative neurodevelopment in brain structure. Translational Psychiatry, 11(1), 1. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-021-01342-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patriat, R., Birn, R. M., Keding, T. J., & Herringa, R. J. (2016). Default-mode network Abnormalities in pediatric posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 55(4), 319327. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2016.01.010 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Penhale, S. H., Picci, G., Ott, L. R., Taylor, B. K., Frenzel, M. R., Eastman, J. A., Wang, Y.-P., Calhoun, V. D., Stephen, J. M., Wilson, T. W. (2022). Impacts of adrenarcheal DHEA levels on spontaneous cortical activity during development. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 57, 101153. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2022.101153 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Picci, G., Christopher-Hayes, N. J., Petro, N. M., Taylor, B. K., Eastman, J. A., Frenzel, M. R., Wang, Y.-P., Stephen, J. M., Calhoun, V. D., Wilson, T. W. (2022a). Amygdala and hippocampal subregions mediate outcomes following trauma during typical development: Evidence from high-resolution structural MRI. Neurobiology of Stress, 18, 100456. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ynstr.2022.100456 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Picci, G., Taylor, B. K., Killanin, A. D., Eastman, J. A., Frenzel, M. R., Wang, Y.-P., Stephen, J. M., Calhoun, V. D., & Wilson, T. W. (2022b). Left amygdala structure mediates longitudinal associations between exposure to threat and long-term psychiatric symptomatology in youth. Human Brain Mapping, 43(13), 40914102. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.25904 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rakesh, D., Allen, N. B., & Whittle, S. (2021). Longitudinal changes in within-salience network functional connectivity mediate the relationship between childhood abuse and neglect, and mental health during adolescence. Psychological Medicine, 53(4), 113. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721003135 Google ScholarPubMed
Rakesh, D., Kelly, C., Vijayakumar, N., Zalesky, A., Allen, N. B., & Whittle, S. (2021). Unraveling the consequences of childhood maltreatment: Deviations from typical functional neurodevelopment mediate the relationship between maltreatment history and depressive symptoms. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 6(3), 329342. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2020.09.016 Google ScholarPubMed
Rissanen, J. (1983). A universal prior for integers and estimation by minimum description length. The Annals of Statistics, 11(2), 416431. https://doi.org/10.1214/aos/1176346150 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ross, M. C., & Cisler, J. M. (2020). Altered large-scale functional brain organization in posttraumatic stress disorder: A comprehensive review of univariate and network-level neurocircuitry models of PTSD. NeuroImage: Clinical, 27, 102319. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2020.102319 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seeley, W. W., Menon, V., Schatzberg, A. F., Keller, J., Glover, G. H., Kenna, H., Reiss, A. L., & Greicius, M. D. (2007). Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(9), 23492356. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5587-06.2007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheffield, J. M., & Barch, D. M. (2016). Cognition and resting-state functional connectivity in schizophrenia. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 61, 108120. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.12.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sherman, L. E., Rudie, J. D., Pfeifer, J. H., Masten, C. L., McNealy, K., & Dapretto, M. (2014). Development of the default mode and central executive networks across early adolescence: A longitudinal study. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 148159. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2014.08.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheynin, J., Duval, E. R., Lokshina, Y., Scott, J. C., Angstadt, M., Kessler, D., Zhang, L., Gur, R. E., Gur, R. C., Liberzon, I. (2020). Altered resting-state functional connectivity in adolescents is associated with PTSD symptoms and trauma exposure. NeuroImage: Clinical, 26, 102215. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2020.102215 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silveira, S., Shah, R., Nooner, K. B., Nagel, B. J., Tapert, S. F., de Bellis, M. D., & Mishra, J. (2020). Impact of childhood trauma on executive function in adolescence—Mediating functional brain networks and prediction of high-risk drinking. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 5(5), 499509. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2020.01.011 Google ScholarPubMed
Solomon, M., Hogeveen, J., Libero, L., & Nordahl, C. (2017). An altered scaffold for information processing: Cognitive control development in adolescents with autism. Biological Psychiatry. Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 2(6), 464475. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.06.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stange, J. P., Bessette, K. L., Jenkins, L. M., Peters, A. T., Feldhaus, C., Crane, N. A., Ajilore, O., Jacobs, R. H., Watkins, E. R., Langenecker, S. A. (2017). Attenuated intrinsic connectivity within cognitive control network among individuals with remitted depression: Temporal stability and association with negative cognitive styles: Cognitive control network connectivity in rMDD. Human Brain Mapping, 38(6), 29392954. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23564 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steinberg, A. M., Brymer, M. J., Decker, K. B., & Pynoos, R. S. (2004). The university of california at Los Angeles post-traumatic stress disorder reaction index. Current Psychiatry Reports, 6(2), 96100. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-004-0048-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stephen, J. M., Solis, I., Janowich, J., Stern, M., Frenzel, M. R., Eastman, J. A., Mills, M. S., Embury, C. M., Coolidge, N. M., Heinrichs-Graham, E., Mayer, A., Liu, J., Wang, Y. P., Wilson, T. W., Calhoun, V. D. (2021). The developmental chronnecto-genomics (Dev-coG) study: A multimodal study on the developing brain. NeuroImage, 225, 117438. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117438 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stone, L. B., Amole, M. C., Cyranowski, J. M., & Swartz, H. A. (2018). History of childhood emotional abuse predicts lower resting-state high-frequency heart rate variability in depressed women. Psychiatry Research, 269, 681687. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2018.08.106 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sutcubasi, B., Metin, B., Kurban, M. K., Metin, Z. E., Beser, B., & Sonuga-Barke, E. (2020). Resting-state network dysconnectivity in ADHD: A system-neuroscience-based meta-analysis. The World Journal of Biological Psychiatry, 21(9), 662672. https://doi.org/10.1080/15622975.2020.1775889 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, B. K., Eastman, J. A., Frenzel, M. R., Embury, C. M., Wang, Y.-P., Stephen, J. M., Calhoun, V. D., Badura-Brack, A. S., & Wilson, T. W. (2021). Subclinical anxiety and posttraumatic stress influence cortical thinning during adolescence. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 60(10), 12881299. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2020.11.020 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, B. K., Frenzel, M. R., Eastman, J. A., Embury, C. M., Agcaoglu, O., Wang, Y. P., Stephen, J. M., Calhoun, V. D., & Wilson, T. W. (2022). Individual differences in amygdala volumes predict changes in functional connectivity between subcortical and cognitive control networks throughout adolescence. NeuroImage, 247, 118852. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118852 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Viard, A., Mutlu, J., Chanraud, S., Guenolé, F., Egler, P.-J., Gérardin, P., Baleyte, J.-M., Dayan, J., Eustache, F., Guillery-Girard, B. (2019). Altered default mode network connectivity in adolescents with post-traumatic stress disorder. NeuroImage: Clinical, 22, 101731. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2019.101731 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, L. M. (2016). Precision psychiatry: A neural circuit taxonomy for depression and anxiety. The Lancet. Psychiatry, 3(5), 472480. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(15)00579-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wu, H., Wu, C., Wu, F., Zhan, Q., Peng, H., Wang, J., Zhao, J., Ning, Y., Zheng, Y., She, S. (2021). Covariation between childhood-trauma related resting-state functional connectivity and affective temperaments is impaired in individuals with major depressive disorder. Neuroscience, 453, 102112. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2020.08.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yeo, B. T. T., Krienen, F. M., Sepulcre, J., Sabuncu, M. R., Lashkari, D., Hollinshead, M., Roffman, J. L., Smoller, J. W., Zöllei, L., Polimeni, J. R., Fischl, B., Liu, H., Buckner, R. L. (2011). The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. Journal of Neurophysiology, 106(3), 11251165. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00338.2011 Google ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Sample demographics and study variables of interest

Figure 1

Figure 1. Example latent growth curve model of cognitive control connectivity. I = Intercept, S = Slope. Dysreg = dysregulation was measured via the CBCL at time 1. Trauma = trauma exposure was collected via the UCLA Trauma History Profile at Time 1. CCN-CCN = within cognitive control network functional network connectivity. Sex, age at time 1, and study site were included as covariates for the intercept and slope, as well as each of the symptomology predictors.

Figure 2

Figure 2. Latent growth curve model results for connectivity between cognitive control regions. Final model results in which sex, age at time 1, study site, CBCL dysregulation symptoms, and trauma exposure all predict the latent intercept and slope of change in cognitive control network connectivity. Solid lines indicate statistically significant estimates at p < .05. All estimates are unstandardized. I = intercept, S = slope. Dysreg = dysregulation measured via the CBCL; trauma = trauma measured via the UCLA THP at time 1. CCN = cognitive control network connectivity. Sex, age at time 1, and study site were included as covariates for the intercept and slope, as well as each of the symptomology predictors. For sex, males = 0 and females = 1.

Figure 3

Figure 3. Associations between dysregulation symptomology and trauma at T1 and functional connectivity slope of change. Scatterplots displaying associations between trauma exposure or CBCL symptomology at time 1 (i.e., dysregulation symptoms) and the estimated slope of change in functional network connectivity at rest. Slope values were adjusted by regressing out effects of other variables in the latent growth curve model (e.g., 3a slope was adjusted for age, sex, site, and trauma exposure). Symptoms and trauma exposure were also adjusted for covariates in the model (i.e., age, sex, and site). CCN = cognitive control network, DMN = default mode network; T1 = time 1.

Supplementary material: File

Picci et al. supplementary material
Download undefined(File)
File 119.4 KB