Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-7cvxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-24T23:47:15.942Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

What works for whom? Genetic moderation of intervention efficacy

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 February 2015

Jay Belsky*
Affiliation:
University of California, Davis
Marinus H. van Ijzendoorn*
Affiliation:
Leiden University
*
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Jay Belsky, Department of Human Ecology, University of California, Davis, 1331 Hart Hall, One Shields Avenue, Davis, CA 95616; E-mail: jbelsky@ucdavis.edu; or Marinus H. van IJzendoorn, Graduate School of Education and Child Studies, Centre for Child and Family Studies, Leiden University, Pieter de la Court Gebouw, Wassenaarseweg 52, Leiden 2333 AK, The Netherlands; E-mail: vanijzen@fsw.leidenuniv.nl.
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Jay Belsky, Department of Human Ecology, University of California, Davis, 1331 Hart Hall, One Shields Avenue, Davis, CA 95616; E-mail: jbelsky@ucdavis.edu; or Marinus H. van IJzendoorn, Graduate School of Education and Child Studies, Centre for Child and Family Studies, Leiden University, Pieter de la Court Gebouw, Wassenaarseweg 52, Leiden 2333 AK, The Netherlands; E-mail: vanijzen@fsw.leidenuniv.nl.
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Special Section Editorial
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

The big question in human development intervention, just like in modern medicine, is What works for whom? Although it is well appreciated that interventions do not succeed with all whom they reach, a common presumption as to why they often prove less effective than anticipated focuses on poor implementation. There can be no doubt that fidelity to program model matters greatly. Nevertheless, an expanding body of experimental-intervention research (e.g., Beach, Brody, Lei, & Philibert, Reference Beach, Brody, Lei and Philibert2010; Kegel, Bus, & van IJzendoorn, Reference Kegel, Bus and van IJzendoorn2011), building directly on theory regarding differential susceptibility to environmental influences and observational evidence to this effect (Belsky, Bakermans-Kranenburg, & van IJzendoorn, Reference Belsky, Bakermans-Kranenburg and van IJzendoorn2007; Belsky & Pluess, Reference Belsky and Pluess2009, Reference Belsky and Pluess2013; Ellis, Boyce, Belsky, Bakermans-Kranenburg, & van IJzendoorn, Reference Ellis, Boyce, Belsky, Bakermans-Kranenburg and van IJzendoorn2011), makes it clear that characteristics of individuals involved in interventions matters as well when it comes to explaining “what works for whom.” Answering the question of which individual characteristics matter holds the promise of enabling intervention programmers to tailor interventions to individuals, thereby improving the efficiency of program delivery and maximizing impact. The main goal of this Special Section of Development and Psychopathology is to share the latest experimental intervention research addressing this issue that focuses on the genetic makeup of individuals as determinants (or moderators) of variation in intervention efficacy.

The differential susceptibility model has important clinical and practical implications. The average effects of preventive or therapeutic interventions in human development are only modest, with effect sizes barely larger that Cohen's criterion for a weak effect (d = 20), reflecting the standardized difference between the control and intervention group on the core outcome the intervention is designed to impact. Failure to consider variation in susceptibility, however, means that such intervention effects may be much larger for more susceptible individuals and much smaller for the less susceptible (Belsky & Pluess, Reference Belsky and Pluess2009, Reference Belsky and Pluess2013; van IJzendoorn, Bakermans-Kranenburg, Belsky, et al., Reference van IJzendoorn, Bakermans-Kranenburg, Belsky, Beach, Brody and Dodge2011). Weak intervention effects might be taken as a sign of a cul-de-sac, not only in research on intervention efficacy but also in terms of how well current thinking about developmental psychopathology (and human development enhancement) can inform efforts to prevent or remediate problematic functioning or enhance well-being. Policymakers and funding agencies might be less inclined to support efforts to document intervention efficacy or to roll out evidence-based interventions on a larger scale given concerns that impact might be limited and/or not cost effective. Being aware of differential susceptibility of participants in preventive or therapeutic interventions creates more realistic expectations of intervention efficacy while illuminating the hidden efficacy of interventions targeting groups composed of a mixture of both more and less susceptible individuals (Bakermans-Kranenburg & van IJzendoorn, in press). It is important to appreciate that a focus on genetically based differential susceptibility can raise ethical issues concerning stigma, discrimination, and equity of service provision (Ellis et al., Reference Ellis, Boyce, Belsky, Bakermans-Kranenburg and van IJzendoorn2011), but this should not lead to ignoring replicated evidence that some individuals are more open to environmentally induced change, for better and for worse, due to their genetic makeup.

Should it prove the case that genetic characteristics of individuals account for and perhaps even causally influence the efficacy of particular interventions, as multiple reports in this Special Section indicate, a second issue arises beyond “what works for whom.” That involves the mechanisms accounting for why a particular intervention proves more effective for some than for others. Although some of the research to be reported in the Special Section will focus on traditional cognitive, emotional, and behavioral mechanisms, here we call attention to the potential role of epigenetic processes. There is increasing appreciation that experiences, including perhaps intervention-related ones, can affect gene expression and thereby cognitive, emotional, and behavioral functioning (Meaney, Reference Meaney2010; van IJzendoorn, Bakermans-Kranenburg, & Ebstein, Reference van IJzendoorn, Bakermans-Kranenburg and Ebstein2011). Thus, in addition to heralding cutting-edge research identifying genotypic markers of intervention efficacy, when we planned this Special Section we expected to be in a position to include work devoted to epigenetic mediation in an effort to illuminate how intervention effects “get under the skin” and come to affect human development. Unfortunately, our editorial ambitions exceeded the evidence base. Thus, even though we can raise this issue of epigenetic mediation, and include one paper in the Special Section examining intervention effects on methylation in rodents (van der Doelen, Arnoldussen, Homberg & Kozicz, Reference Van der Doelen, Arnoldussen, Ghareh, van Och, Homberg and Kozicz2015 [this issue]), as well as a human observational study addressing the same general issue (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Pan, Tuan, Teh, Mah and Li2015 [this issue]), while calling attention to prior studies documenting epigenetic mediation of intervention efficacy (Perroud et al., Reference Perroud, Salzmann, Prada, Nicastro, Hoeppli and Furrer2013; Roberts et al., Reference Roberts, Lester, Hudson, Rapee, Creswell and Cooper2014; Yehuda et al., Reference Yehuda, Daskalakis, Desarnaud, Makotkine, Lehrner and Koch2013), additional evidence of the latter kind must await future empirical progress.

Given these introductory comments, in the remainder of this introductory essay, we delineate the theoretical and empirical foundations of the work that makes up the Special Section and provide overviews the papers that are included before drawing some general conclusions.

Theoretical Foundations: Differential Susceptibility to Environmental Influences

A differential-susceptibility perspective on environmental influences differs fundamentally from the traditional approach to conceptualizing Person × Environment interaction. Whereas the classic diathesis–stress framework focused on those most “vulnerable” to contextual adversity (e.g., poverty, maternal depression, and family conflict; Zuckerman, Reference Zuckerman1999), differential susceptibility highlights that those most likely to be adversely affected by some negative environmental exposure or developmental experience will be those most likely to benefit from contextual support and/or enrichment as well (Belsky, Reference Belsky1997, Reference Belsky, Ellis and Bjorklund2005; Belsky et al., Reference Belsky, Bakermans-Kranenburg and van IJzendoorn2007; Ellis et al., Reference Ellis, Boyce, Belsky, Bakermans-Kranenburg and van IJzendoorn2011). Numerous results from observational studies provide support for the latter proposition (for reviews, see Belsky & Pluess, Reference Belsky and Pluess2011, Reference Belsky and Pluess2013), highlighting, among other factors, the role of genotype in predicting for whom positive and negative experiences and exposures affect, respectively, positive and negative functioning and development.

Consider, in this regard, evidence documenting such for better and for worse results in the case of those carrying one or more serotonin transporter linked polymorphic region gene (5-HTTLPR) short alleles when the rearing predictor and child outcome were, respectively, maternal responsiveness and moral internalization (Kochanska, Kim, Barry, & Philibert, Reference Kochanska, Kim, Barry and Philibert2011), child maltreatment and antisocial behavior (Cicchetti, Rogosch, & Thibodeau, Reference Cicchetti, Rogosch and Thibodeau2012), supportive parenting and positive affect (Hankin et al., Reference Hankin, Nederhof, Oppenheimer, Jenness, Young and Abela2011), and, in the case of African American adolescents, perceived racial discrimination and conduct problems (Brody et al., Reference Brody, Beach, Chen, Obasi, Philibert and Kogan2011). Consider, as well, evidence showing heightened (or exclusive) susceptibility of individuals carrying the dopamine receptor D4 gene (DRD4) seven-repeat (7R) allele when the environmental predictor and developmental outcome were, respectively, maternal positivity and prosocial behavior (Knafo, Israel, & Ebstein, Reference Knafo, Israel and Ebstein2011), early nonfamilial childcare and social competence (Belsky & Pluess, Reference Belsky and Pluess2013), contextual stress/support and adolescent negative arousal (Beach et al., Reference Beach, Lei, Brody, Simons, Cutrona and Philibert2012), and childhood adversity and young-adult persistent alcohol dependence (Park, Sher, Todorov, & Heath, Reference Park, Sher, Todorov and Heath2011). Of special importance perhaps is that two recent meta-analyses of children and/or adolescents reveal that those carrying certain dopamine genes, including DRD4 7R, prove to be more affected by positive and negative environmental exposures than do others (van IJzendoorn & Bakermans-Kranenburg, Reference van IJzendoorn, Bakermans-Kranenburg, Belsky, Beach, Brody and Dodge2011), with the same being true, at least for Caucasian children, for those carrying 5-HTTLPR short alleles (van IJzendoorn, Belsky, & Bakermans-Kranenburg, Reference van IJzendoorn, Belsky and Bakermans-Kranenburg2012).

Implications for Intervention

One of the major consequences of differential-susceptibility theorizing has been to direct attention toward variation in response to positive/supportive environmental influences, a focus that was neglected for many years due to the influence of diathesis–stress thinking, which highlighted variation in response to adversity only. It is of interest to note that whereas diathesis–stress research long focused on characteristics of individuals to explain their susceptibility to negative experiences and exposures, including genetic ones, only limited attention was paid to such factors when it came to illuminating variation in response to supportive experiences and exposures. Perhaps the best evidence of this is that most thinking about variation in intervention efficacy highlighted the importance of program implementation, with little if any attention paid to characteristics of program recipients (beyond perhaps demographic ones or preintervention levels of functioning).

As it turns out, there is ever-growing experimental evidence, which this Special Section expands and extends, that genetic factors matter when it comes to illuminating what works for whom. Recent genetically informed intervention evaluations indicate that alleles long presumed to place individuals at risk in the face of adversity (i.e., 5-HTTLPR short and DRD4 7R) or to promote resilience (i.e., not short/not 7R) are associated with them being, respectively, susceptible or not to the benefits of intervention (for the first experimental genetic differential susceptibility evidence, see Bakermans-Kranenburg, van IJzendoorn, Pijlman, Mesman, & Juffer, Reference Bakermans-Kranenburg, van IJzendoorn, Pijlman, Mesman and Juffer2008). Consider in this regard Drury et al.’s (Reference Drury, Gleason, Theall, Smyke, Nelson and Fox2012) data showing that it was only the children growing up in Romanian orphanages who carried 5-HTTLPR short alleles who benefited from being randomly assigned to high-quality foster care, in terms of reductions in the display of indiscriminant friendliness (but for counterevidence in the case of an extended, intensive intervention with nonmoderated high effectiveness, see Cicchetti, Rogosch, & Toth, Reference Cicchetti, Rogosch and Toth2011). Eley et al. (Reference Eley, Hudson, Creswell, Tropeano, Lester and Cooper2012) also documented intervention benefits restricted to short-allele carriers, although their design included only treated children. Turning to DRD4, Kegel et al. (Reference Kegel, Bus and van IJzendoorn2011) tested and found support for the hypothesis that it would be 7R carriers who would benefit from specially designed computer games promoting phonemic awareness and, thereby, early literacy. Other randomized-controlled trial results point in the same direction with regard to DRD4 7R, including research on African American teens in which substance use was the outcome examined (Beach et al., Reference Beach, Brody, Lei and Philibert2010; Brody, Chen, & Beach, in press; Brody et al., Reference Brody, Chen, Beach, Kogan, Yu and DiClemente2014). What makes these diverse results so interesting is that they concern not only quite diverse means of intervention (parent training, cognitive behavioral therapy, and computer-assisted learning) but also have as their targets prevention, remediation, and enhancement.

Special Section Papers

When it comes to this Special Section and research on genetic moderation of intervention efficacy, the papers included focus exclusively on randomized control trial experiments and genetic differential susceptibility. The main reason is that experiments have at least three distinct advantages compared to observational/correlational studies of Gene × Environment (G × E) interaction (van IJzendoorn & Bakermans-Kranenburg, Reference van IJzendoorn and Bakermans-Kranenburg2015 [this issue]). First, the genetic marker of differential susceptibility cannot be correlated with the manipulated environment, because randomization implies independence of (change in) the environment and genetic makeup. This obviates any concern about gene–environment correlation accounting for a detected statistical G × E interaction, a problem that plagues virtually all observational G × E research. Second, many correlational G × E studies assess genes in a very precise way but fail to measure the environmental component in an equally precise manner. Experimental manipulation of the environment often reduces measurement error in the environment, as long as the intervention is done in a standardized way. Third, correlational G × E studies have been criticized for their lack of statistical power and risk of spurious findings (Duncan & Keller, Reference Duncan and Keller2011). Randomized G × E experiments have considerably more power, estimated by some to be equivalent to correlational studies with 10 times more subjects (for details, see Bakermans & van IJzendoorn, in press).

The first eight articles of this Special Section address the issue of whether intervention effects under consideration vary as a function of child genotype, usually with a priori expectations as to which children should benefit most as a result of their genetic makeup. Zöe Brett et al. do so when investigating how children with extensive exposure to severely depriving Romanian orphanages respond to being randomized to receive high-quality foster care (or not), focusing on externalizing behavior and the 5-HTTLPR polymorphism. Dante Cicchetti, Sheree L. Toth, and Elizabeth D. Handley also focus on the serotonin transporter gene when evaluating whether the effect of interpersonal psychotherapy proves effective in preventing major depressive disorder in a sample of economically impoverished mothers of infants; but they extend Gene × Intervention (G × I) inquiry by also examining the moderating effect of genotypic variation in corticotropin releasing hormone receptor 1. The results prove interesting, even somewhat surprising, with respect to which genetic subgroup benefits the most, perhaps due to the racial composition of the sample, which was disproportionately African American or biracial.

In the third paper, Gene H. Brody, Tianyi Yu, and Steven R. H. Beach test the proposition that rural African American youth who are carriers of DRD4 long alleles would benefit most from a family-oriented intervention targeting both parents and adolescents in order to prevent adolescent drug use. It is important that these investigators not only document genetic moderation of intervention efficacy but also address what they refer to as a “second-generation” question of G × I research by illuminating cognitive processes responsible for detected prevention effects in an effort to understand not just for whom the intervention proved effective but how its effect was instantiated. Of note, the aforementioned paper by Cicchetti et al. in this Special Section extends G × I work in exactly the same way, but with a focus on social rather than cognitive mediational processes. The work of H. Harrington Cleveland et al., which comes next, also focuses on DRD4 as a genetic moderator, in this case of a multifaceted intervention designed to prevent underage alcohol use (measured in ninth grade) that was implemented in some 28 communities. This work chronicles genetic moderation of intervention efficacy that is itself dependent on level of maternal involvement when children were in sixth grade. In contrast to all the work just cited, the fifth paper, by Rachel D. Plak, Cornelia A. T. Kegel, and Adriana G. Bus, focuses on an intervention designed to enhance functioning, not just prevent problems. Thus, these investigators report that a computerized instructional program designed to promote text comprehension and thereby literacy proves effective, but only in the case of those carrying long DRD4 alleles.

Not all papers included in the Special Section focus on “the usual suspects” of DRD4 or 5-HTTLPR. The sixth paper, by Dustin Albert et al., extends an earlier analysis of genetic moderation of the well-known Fast Track Prevention program. Having already reported that this multiple-year, multifaceted program, which began when children were in kindergarten, proved effective in preventing externalizing psychopathology at the age of 25 depending on the glucocorticoid receptor gene, this report addresses whether the genetically moderated prevention effect was evident much earlier in life and whether it mediated the effect detected later in life. In the seventh report, Joni Sasaki, Taraneh Mojaverian, and Heejung J. Kim turn attention to the oxytocin receptor gene polymorphism, testing the proposition that GG homozygotes would prove especially sensitive to social conditions, which they evaluate in a social psychology experiment that attempts to foster self-control by means of religion priming. In the eighth paper, Rachelle J. Musci et al. move beyond a focus on single candidate-gene moderators to examine the moderational effect of a polygenic score. The results revealed that their composite moderator, based on more than 12,000 single nucleotide polymorphisms identified in prior tobacco-related research, moderated the effects of a school-based intervention implemented in kindergarten on age of first tobacco use.

The final three papers differ from those just highlighted, all of which present new evidence pertaining to genetic moderation of intervention efficacy. The contribution by Rick Van der Doelen et al. relies on a rodent model to examine intervention effects on epigenetic processes of methylation, a presumed mediator of the environmental manipulation on behavior. The paper by Li Chen et al., also examines, in the context of an observational study, how the brain-derived neurotropic factor moderates not only the effects of prenatal anxiety on gene expression but also the effects of such gene expression on the endophenotype of neonatal brain volume. As already noted, future work will hopefully extend research of this kind to determine, whether as presumed by these investigators, and many contributors to the Special Section, epigenetic processes are a critical pathway by which intervention effects become instantiated.

The final paper of the Special Section seeks to empirically examine and summarize emerging knowledge about the moderational effects of intervention effects when the focus is on perhaps the two most recent frequently used candidate genes in G × E and G × I research, DRD4 and 5-HTTLPR. Thus, Marinus H. van IJzendoorn and Marian L. Bakerman-Kranenburg report a meta-analysis of all existing G × I studies, whether prevention, remediation, or enhancement oriented, or resulting from social-psychological experiments like that of Sasaki et al., although not of pharmacological interventions. The results prove most interesting.

Conclusion

Because research on the genetic moderation of environmental influences, that is, G × E research, has exerted such a powerful effect on thinking about the interaction of nature and nurture since the publication of Caspi, Moffitt, and associates’ (Reference Caspi, McClay, Moffitt, Mill, Martin and Craig2002, Reference Caspi, Sugden, Moffitt, Taylor, Craig and Harrington2003) papers on this subject, it is somewhat surprising that it has taken as long as it has for intervention-oriented investigators to address the core issue on which this Special Section is focused: whether intervention efficacy varies as a function of the target individual's genetic makeup. Although it is not entirely clear why that has been the case, we are most pleased to see the ever-growing interest in this important subject. We suspect that G × I research has lagged behind G × E research for two reasons. The first reason is that until the emergence of differential-susceptibility theorizing, there was an absence of consideration that some individuals might be more developmentally plastic, for better and for worse, than others, even though diathesis–stress thinking has long acknowledged that some are likely to be more vulnerable to adversity than others.

The second reason may have to do with the intellectual and ethical discomfort that some feel when considering the implications of differential-susceptibility thinking for intervention: if only some are likely to benefit from an intervention, whether prevention, remediation, or enhancement in character, should it only be provided to such individuals? Our experience teaches that some who value equity especially highly believe that the answer to this question is “no,” whereas others who privilege efficacy, and cost effectiveness, regard the answer to this question as “yes.” For several reasons we think both of these answers are premature, at least given the present state of our knowledge.

If research continues to reveal, as the meta-analysis by van IJzendoorn and Bakermans-Kranenburg in this Special Section begins to suggest, that we can identify those who do and do not benefit from many diverse interventions, then it would seem questionable to provide services that the community pays for but that are unlikely to prove effective simply so that everyone is treated, or seems to be treated, in the same way. At the same time, even if the evidence indicates that some do not benefit whereas others do from particular interventions, it does not follow that those who prove relatively unsusceptible will be unsusceptible to any and all interventions. It may just be the case, and this must be the working hypothesis: that they are simply unresponsive to the interventions being administered. If this is the case, the imperative would be to expand the duration, intensity, or range of interventions and thereby determine just how generally or specifically unsusceptible are those who appear not to benefit from interventions being implemented. As one reads the discussion sections of many of the papers included in the Special Section, it is clear that the view we are advocating is widely shared.

We are currently far from a point where we can claim that we should be providing interventions to some and not others due to their genetic makeup. With the current state of knowledge, genetic screening for differential susceptibility would invariably produce too many false negatives as well as false positives. Furthermore, every individual has the basic human right to grow up in good-enough rearing environments and to be enabled to contribute to society according to his or her potentials. This is why we have a system of basic education that promotes literacy and numeracy without wanting to leave any child behind. As another example of the same principle, even though some children with a specific genetic makeup seem more vulnerable to the detrimental effects of institutional care whereas others seem relatively immune to many of these (Brett et al., Reference Brett, Humphreys, Smyke, Gleason, Nelson and Zeanah2015 [this issue]; Nelson, Fox, & Zeanah, Reference Nelson, Fox and Zeanah2014), institutional care reflects structural neglect; as such, it qualifies as lower quality care than any child deserves. All children, according to United Nations Convention on the Rights of the Child (1989), are entitled to (foster or adoptive) family-based care. In other words, just because care that is regarded as neglectful, or worse, may not adversely affect a child in a measurable manner, this is is not a basis for regarding it as sufficient or acceptable.

These comments notwithstanding, we are clearly in a position where we should be asking, based on differential-susceptibility theorizing and the evidence presented in this Special Section and elsewhere, whether some are more likely to benefit from intervention than others because of their genetic makeup (and/or other personal characteristics, like temperament and stress physiology) and, if so, just how broadly susceptible or not are individuals who vary in the benefit they derive from particular interventions. It is in the spirit of such understanding that we are most excited about the work presented in this Special Section and its implications for future preventive and therapeutic intervention research.

References

Albert, D., Belsky, D. W., Crowley, D. M., Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group, Bates, J. E., Pettit, G. S., et al. (2015). Developmental mediation of genetic variation in response to the Fast Track Prevention Program. Development and Psychopathology, 27, 8195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (in press). The hidden efficacy of interventions: Gene × Environment experiments from a differential susceptibility perspective. Annual Review of Psychology.Google Scholar
Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Pijlman, F. T., Mesman, J., & Juffer, F. (2008). Experimental evidence for differential susceptibility: Dopamine D4 receptor polymorphism (DRD4 VNTR) moderates intervention effects on toddlers’ externalizing behavior in a randomized controlled trial. Developmental Psychology, 44, 293300.Google Scholar
Beach, S. R. H., Brody, G. H., Lei, M.-K., & Philibert, R. A. (2010). Differential susceptibility to parenting among African American youths: Testing the DRD4 hypothesis. Journal of Family Psychology, 24, 513521. doi:10.1037/a0020835CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beach, S. R. H., Lei, M. K., Brody, G. H., Simons, R. L., Cutrona, C., & Philibert, R. A. (2012). Genetic moderation of contextual effects on negative arousal and parenting in African American parents. Journal of Family Psychology, 26, 4655. doi:10.1037/a0026236Google Scholar
Belsky, J. (1997). Variation in susceptibility to rearing influences: An evolutionary argument. Psychological Inquiry, 8, 182186.Google Scholar
Belsky, J. (2005). Differential susceptibility to rearing influences: An evolutionary hypothesis and some evidence. In Ellis, B. & Bjorklund, D. (Eds.), Origins of the social mind: Evolutionary psychology and child development (pp. 139163). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Belsky, J., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2007). For better and for worse: Differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 300304.Google Scholar
Belsky, J., & Pluess, M. (2009). Beyond diathesis–stress: Differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 885908.Google Scholar
Belsky, J., & Pluess, M. (2013). Beyond risk, resilience and dysregulation: Phenotypic plasticity and human development. Development and Psychopathology, 25, 12431261.Google Scholar
Brett, Z. H., Humphreys, K. L., Smyke, A. T., Gleason, M. M., Nelson, A., Zeanah, C. H., et al. (2015). Serotonin transporter linked polymorphic region (5-HTTLPR) genotype moderates the longitudinal impact of early caregiving on externalizing behavior. Development and Psychopathology, 27, 718.Google Scholar
Brody, G. H., Beach, S. R. H., Chen, Y.-F., Obasi, E., Philibert, R. A., Kogan, S. M., et al. (2011). Perceived discrimination, serotonin transporter linked polymorphic region status, and the development of conduct problems. Development and Psychopathology, 23, 617627. doi:10.1017/s0954579411000046Google Scholar
Brody, G. H., Chen, Y., & Beach, S. R. H. (in press). Differential susceptibility to prevention: GABAergic, dopaminergic and multilocus effects. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry.Google Scholar
Brody, G. H., Chen, Y.-F., Beach, S. R. H., Kogan, S. M., Yu, T., DiClemente, R. J., et al. (2014). Differential sensitivity to prevention programming: A dopaminergic polymorphism-enhanced prevention effect on protective parenting and adolescent substance use. Health Psychology, 33, 182191.Google Scholar
Brody, G. H., Yu, T., & Beach, S. R. H. (2015). A differential susceptibility analysis reveals the “who and how” about adolescents’ responses to preventive interventions: Tests of first- and second-generation Gene × Intervention hypotheses. Development and Psychopathology, 27, 3749.Google Scholar
Caspi, A., McClay, J., Moffitt, T. E., Mill, J., Martin, J., Craig, I. W., et al. (2002). Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in maltreated children. Science, 297, 851853.Google Scholar
Caspi, A., Sugden, K., Moffitt, T. E., Taylor, A., Craig, I. W., Harrington, H., et al. (2003). Influence of life stress on depression: Moderation by a polymorphism of the 5-HTT gene. Science, 301, 386389.Google Scholar
Chen, L., Pan, H., Tuan, T. A., Teh, A. L., Mah, S. M., Li, Y., et al. (2015). Brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) Val66Met influences the association of the methylome with maternal anxiety and neonatal brain volumes. Development and Psychopathology, 27, 137150.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F. A., & Thibodeau, E. L. (2012). The effects of child maltreatment on early signs of antisocial behavior: Genetic moderation by tryptophan hydroxylase, serotonin transporter, and monoamine oxidase A genes. Development and Psychopathology, 24, 907928.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F. A., & Toth, S. L. (2011). The effects of child maltreatment and polymorphisms of the serotonin transporter and dopamine D4 receptor genes and infant attachment and intervention efficacy. Development and Psychopathology, 23, 357372.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., Toth, S. L., & Handley, E. D. (2015). Genetic moderation of interpersonal psychotherapy efficacy for low-income mothers with major depressive disorder: Implications for differential susceptibility. Development and Psychopathology, 27, 1935.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cleveland, H. H., Schlomer, G. L., Vandenbergh, D. J., Feinberg, M., Greenberg, M., Spoth, R., et al. (2015). The conditioning of intervention effects on early adolescent alcohol use by maternal involvement and dopamine receptor D4 (DRD4) and serotonin transporter linked polymorphic region (5-HTTLPR) candidate genes. Development and Psychopathology, 27, 5167.Google Scholar
Drury, S. S., Gleason, M. M., Theall, K. P., Smyke, A. T., Nelson, C. A., Fox, N. A., et al. (2012). Genetic sensitivity to the caregiving context: The influence of 5HTTLPR and BDNF val66met on indiscriminate social behavior. Physiology & Behavior, 106, 728735. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.11.014CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duncan, L. E., & Keller, M. C. (2011). A critical review of the first 10 years of candidate gene-by-environment interaction research in psychiatry. American Journal of Psychiatry, 168, 10411049.Google Scholar
Eley, T. C., Hudson, J. L., Creswell, C., Tropeano, M., Lester, K. J., Cooper, P., et al. (2012). Therapygenetics: The 5HTTLPR and response to psychological therapy. Molecular Psychiatry, 17, 236241. doi:10.1038/mp.2011.132Google Scholar
Ellis, B. J., Boyce, W. T., Belsky, J., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2011). Differential susceptibility to the environment: An evolutionary–neurodevelopmental theory. Development and Psychopathology, 23, 728. doi:10.1017/s0954579410000611Google Scholar
Hankin, B. L., Nederhof, E., Oppenheimer, C. W., Jenness, J., Young, J. F., Abela, J. R. Z., et al. (2011). Differential susceptibility in youth: Evidence that 5-HTTLPR × Positive Parenting is associated with positive affect “for better and worse.” Translational Psychiatry, 1, e44. doi:10.1038/tp.2011.44CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kegel, C. A. T., Bus, A. G., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2011). Differential susceptibility in early literacy instruction through computer games: The role of the dopamine D4 receptor gene (DRD4). Mind, Brain, and Education, 5, 7178. doi:10.1111/j.1751-228X.2011.01112.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Knafo, A., Israel, S., & Ebstein, R. P. (2011). Heritability of children's prosocial behavior and differential susceptibility to parenting by variation in the dopamine receptor D4 gene. Development and Psychopathology, 23, 5367. doi:10.1017/s0954579410000647Google Scholar
Kochanska, G., Kim, S., Barry, R. A., & Philibert, R. A. (2011). Children's genotypes interact with maternal responsive care in predicting children's competence: Diathesis–stress or differential susceptibility? Development and Psychopathology, 23, 605616.Google Scholar
Meaney, M. J. (2010). Epigenetics and the biological definition of Gene × Environment interactions. Child Development, 81, 4179.Google Scholar
Musci, R. J., Masyn, K. E., Uhl, G., Maher, B., Kellam, S. G., & Ialongo, N. S. (2015). Polygenic Score × Intervention Moderation: An application of discrete-time survival analysis to modeling the timing of first tobacco use among urban youth. Development and Psychopathology, 27, 111122.Google Scholar
Nelson, C. A., Fox, N. A., & Zeanah, C. H. (2014). Romania's abandoned children: Deprivation, brain development, and the struggle for recovery. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Park, A., Sher, K. J., Todorov, A. A., & Heath, A. C. (2011). Interaction between the DRD4 VNTR polymorphism and proximal and distal environments in alcohol dependence during emerging and young adulthood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 120, 585595. doi:10.1037/a0022648Google Scholar
Perroud, N., Salzmann, A., Prada, P., Nicastro, R., Hoeppli, M. E., Furrer, S., et al. (2013). Response to psychotherapy in borderline personality disorder and methylation status of the BDNF gene. Translational Psychiatry, 3, e207.Google Scholar
Plak, R. D., Kegel, C. A. T., & Bus, A. G. (2015). Genetic differential susceptibility in literacy delayed children: A randomized controlled trial on emergent literacy in kindergarten. Development and Psychopathology, 27, 6779.Google Scholar
Roberts, S., Lester, K. J., Hudson, J. L., Rapee, R. M., Creswell, C., Cooper, P., et al. (2014). Serotonin transporter methylation and response to cognitive behaviour therapy in children with anxiety disorders. Translational Psychiatry, 4, e444.Google Scholar
Sasaki, J. Y., Mojaverian, T., & Kim, H. J. (2015). Religion priming and an oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) polymorphism interact to affect self-control in a social context. Development and Psychopathology, 27, 97109.Google Scholar
United Nations General Assembly (UNGA). (1989). Convention on the rights of the child (United Nations, Treaty Series, Vol. 1577, p. 3). New York: Author.Google Scholar
Van der Doelen, R. H. A., Arnoldussen, I. A., Ghareh, H., van Och, L., Homberg, J. R., & Kozicz, T. (2015). Early life adversity and serotonin transporter gene variation interact to affect DNA methylation of the corticotropin-releasing factor gene promoter region in the adult rat brain. Development and Psychopathology, 27, 123135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2012). Differential susceptibility experiments: Going beyond correlational evidence. Comment on beyond mental health, differential susceptibility articles. Developmental Psychology, 48, 769774. doi:10.1037/a0027536CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2015). Genetic differential susceptibility on trial: Meta-analytic support from randomized controlled experiments. Development and Psychopathology, 27, 151162.Google Scholar
van IJzendoorn, M. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Belsky, J., Beach, S., Brody, G., Dodge, K. A., et al. (2011). Gene-by-environment experiments: A new approach to finding the missing heritability. Nature Reviews Genetics, 12, 881.Google Scholar
van IJzendoorn, M. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & Ebstein, R. P. (2011). Methylation matters in child development: Toward developmental behavioral epigenetics. Child Development Perspectives, 4, 305310. doi:10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00202.xGoogle Scholar
van IJzendoorn, M. H., Belsky, J., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2012). Serotonin transporter genotype 5HTTLPR as a marker of differential susceptibility? A meta-analysis of child and adolescent gene-by-environent studies. Translational Psychiatry, e147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yehuda, R., Daskalakis, N. P., Desarnaud, F., Makotkine, I., Lehrner, A., Koch, E., et al. (2013). Epigenetic biomarkers as predictors and correlates of symptom improvement following psychotherapy in combat veterans with PTSD. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 4, 114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zuckerman, M. (1999). Vulnerability to psychopathology: A biosocial model. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar