Introduction
War has long been understood to have an influence on mental health outcomes in populations (Murthy & Lakshminarayana, Reference Murthy and Lakshminarayana2006); impacts of the stress from war have been studied in multiple regions, including the Balkans (Jovanovic et al. Reference Jovanovic, Kuwert, Sarajlic Vukovic, Poredos Lavor, Medved, Kovac, Spitzer, Dudeck, Vogel, Freyberger and Grabe2010; Priebe et al. Reference Priebe, Bogic, Ashcroft, Franciskovic, Galeazzi, Kucukalic, Lecic-Tosevski, Morina, Popovski, Roughton, Schützwohl and Ajdukovic2010b ), Afghanistan (Cardozo et al. Reference Cardozo, Bilukha, Crawford, Shaikh, Wolfe, Gerber and Anderson2004; Scholte et al. Reference Scholte, Olff, Ventevogel, de Vries, Jansveld, Cardozo and Crawford2004), South Asia (Siriwardhana et al. Reference Siriwardhana, Adikari, Pannala, Roberts, Siribaddana, Abas, Sumathipala and Stewart2015), sub-Saharan Africa (Mugisha et al. Reference Mugisha, Muyinda, Malamba and Kinyanda2015) and the Middle East (Neria et al. Reference Neria, Bravova and Halper2010). While all of society bears a burden from conflict, children are particularly vulnerable (Ghosh et al. Reference Ghosh, Mohit and Murthy2004). In 2014, UNICEF reported that 230 million children live in areas of armed conflict; this number includes victims and witnesses of violence, child soldiers, and displaced children, among others (UNICEF, 2014).
Many studies have investigated mental health impacts of conflict on youth (Panter-Brick et al. Reference Panter-Brick, Eggerman, Gonzalez and Safdar2009; Schiff et al. Reference Schiff, Pat-Horenczyk, Benbenishty, Brom, Baum and Astor2012; Okello et al. Reference Okello, De Schryver, Musisi, Broekaert and Derluyn2014; Kangaslampi et al. Reference Kangaslampi, Garoff and Peltonen2015), though the majority have explored affective and anxiety-related pathologies – particularly depression and posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (Panter-Brick et al. Reference Panter-Brick, Eggerman, Gonzalez and Safdar2009; Kimhi et al. Reference Kimhi, Eshel, Zysberg and Hantman2010; Okello et al. Reference Okello, De Schryver, Musisi, Broekaert and Derluyn2014; Tol et al. Reference Tol, Komproe, Jordans, Ndayisaba, Ntamutumba, Sipsma, Smallegange, Macy and de Jong2014; Kangaslampi et al. Reference Kangaslampi, Garoff and Peltonen2015). A series of studies (Silove et al. Reference Silove, Bateman, Brooks, Fonseca, Steel, Rodger, Soosay, Fox, Patel and Bauman2008, Reference Silove, Ivancic, Rees, Bateman-Steel and Steel2014; Soosay et al. Reference Soosay, Silove, Bateman-Steel, Steel, Bebbington, Jones, Chey, Ivancic and Marnane2012) explored relationships between trauma exposure, PTSD symptomatology and psychotic-like experiences (PLEs) in post-conflict Timor. In one study, 12% screened positive for significant PLEs (Soosay et al. Reference Soosay, Silove, Bateman-Steel, Steel, Bebbington, Jones, Chey, Ivancic and Marnane2012). However, these studies focused primarily on adults. The dearth of research on conflict exposure and psychosis in youth is significant because of the connections between early-life stress and psychosis development. This elevated risk has been observed in child abuse victims (Choi et al. Reference Choi, Choi, Kim, Lee, Gim and Park2015), various forms of maltreatment (Read et al. Reference Read, Fink, Rudegeair, Felitti and Whitfield2008) and other childhood traumas (Hammersley et al. Reference Hammersley, Read, Woodall and Dillon2008; Bentall et al. Reference Bentall, Wickham, Shevlin and Varese2012). Research has shown early life trauma and major adversities before age 18 are predictive of later psychosis and can increase the risk of developing psychotic illness by up to threefold later in life (Varese et al. Reference Varese, Smeets, Drukker, Lieverse, Lataster, Viechtbauer, Read, van Os and Bentall2012; Russo et al. Reference Russo, Stochl, Painter, Dobler, Jackson, Jones and Perez2014).
One of the most devastating recent conflicts was the Rwandan Civil War, beginning in 1990 and culminating in the genocide targeted towards the Tutsi people in 1994. During the genocidal period alone, an estimated 500 000–1 000 000 Rwandans were killed, with many more affected by displacement, abuse, torture, rape, forced combat or directly witnessing atrocities (Des Forges, Reference Des Forges1999; Rieder & Elbert, Reference Rieder and Elbert2013). The effects of this conflict were also felt by other countries due to refugee migration and proxy wars (Prunier, Reference Prunier2009). The impacts of the war on mental health have been studied in Uganda. Research there has described depressive and anxiety symptoms, behavioural disturbances and other psychological impacts on child soldiers (Klasen et al. Reference Klasen, Oettingen, Daniels and Adam2010; Moscardino et al. Reference Moscardino, Scrimin, Cadei and Altoe2012) and abducted individuals (Okello et al. Reference Okello, Onen and Musisi2007). Another study of child soldiers also showed connections between war experience, PTSD and psychosis (Amone-P'Olak et al. Reference Amone-P'Olak, Ovuga, Croudace, Jones and Abbott2014); while relationships between PTSD and PLEs were similar to those seen in Timor, investigation was not extended to the general youth population. Though there has been some work on the effects of the war on the Rwandan population, much of it has focused on affective symptoms in adults (e.g. Lacasse et al. Reference Lacasse, Forgeard, Jayawickreme and Jayawickreme2014; Eytan et al. Reference Eytan, Munyandamutsa, Nkubamugisha and Gex-Fabry2015). There has been no research to our knowledge looking at the interrelationships between armed conflict and psychosis in Rwanda; indeed, there has not been significant published research regarding psychosis in any context in the country before.
Early treatment of schizophrenia is linked to better outcomes (Marshall et al. Reference Marshall, Lewis, Lockwood, Drake, Jones and Croudace2005) and accurately identifying individuals before illness onset holds promise for prevention (Mrazek & Haggerty, Reference Mrazek and Haggerty1994; McGorry & Killackey, Reference McGorry and Killackey2002). This is especially relevant in sub-Saharan Africa, where resources for managing psychotic disorders are extremely limited (Patel et al. Reference Patel, Boardman, Prince and Bhugra2006; Saxena et al. Reference Saxena, Sharan, Garrido and Saraceno2006; Ndetei et al. Reference Ndetei, Ongecha and Mutiso2007). Assessing clinical risk early has significantly advanced the possibility of indicated prevention of full-blown psychotic disorders (Yung et al. Reference Yung, McGorry, McFarlane, Jackson, Patton and Rakkar1996, Reference Yung, Phillips, Yuen and McGorry2004; Miller et al. Reference Miller, McGlashan, Woods, Stein, Driesen, Corcoran, Hoffman and Davidson1999). Our group has conducted multiple studies in Kenya, investigating the prevalence of PLEs and risk for developing illness using self-report questionnaires (Mamah et al. Reference Mamah, Mbwayo, Mutiso, Barch, Constantino, Nsofor, Khasakhala and Ndetei2012, Reference Mamah, Owoso, Mbwayo, Mutiso, Muriungi, Khasakhala, Barch and Ndetei2013; Ndetei et al. Reference Ndetei, Muriungi, Owoso, Mutiso, Mbwayo, Khasakhala, Barch and Mamah2012; Owoso et al. Reference Owoso, Ndetei, Mbwayo, Mutiso, Khasakhala and Mamah2014). The Washington Early Recognition Center Affectivity and Psychosis (WERCAP) Screen (Mamah et al. Reference Mamah, Owoso, Sheffield and Bayer2014) was developed to overcome some of the challenges in assessing risk of developing psychotic disorders, guided by the existing literature on risk assessment and our previous efforts in Africa and the USA. Using the instrument, the primary aims of our study were to: (1) compare psychotic and affective risk symptoms between adolescents in Rwanda and Kenya – a geographically close country without the similar recent history of genocide, civil war or being directly affected by the Rwandan conflict – and (2) compare risk symptoms in Rwandan youth born before and after the war.
Methods
Participants
A total of 2800 Kenyan secondary school students from 10th through 12th grades (ages 14 and older) were recruited in September and October, 2013. These students came from a relatively rural area with families present there for generations and no known history of organised mass violence. In Rwanda, 1628 secondary school students from the final three grades (ages 14 and older) were recruited from ten secondary schools in Kigali and surrounding areas. A total of 627 Rwandan college students aged 19 and older were recruited from the former Kigali Health Institute (now part of the University of Rwanda, College of Medicine and Health Sciences) as well. Rwandan participants were recruited in February, 2014. All assessments were performed in the classroom and individually administered; participation was voluntary with all questions verbally answered before consent was signed and the study begun. The study was approved by the Institutional Review Boards (IRBs) of Washington University School of Medicine (no. 201306026), the Kenyatta National Hospital Ethics and Research Committee (no. KNH-ERC/A/137) and the Kigali Health Institute (no. KHI/IRB/21/2013).
Assessment
All participants were administered the WERCAP Screen – an instrument which we have described previously (Mamah et al. Reference Mamah, Owoso, Sheffield and Bayer2014) and used in both the USA and Kenya (Mamah et al. Reference Mamah, Musau, Mutiso, Owoso, Abdallah, Cottler, Striley, Walker and Ndetei2016). The instrument is divided into two sections, one that estimates psychosis risk (pWERCAP) and the other the risk for developing bipolar disorder (termed ‘affectivity’-aWERCAP). The WERCAP Screen (psychosis portion) has previously been validated in a US sample against the SIPS (Structured Interview for Psychosis-Risk Syndromes) – an interview that utilises five psychotic-risk symptom subscales (Miller et al. Reference Miller, McGlashan, Rosen, Cadenhead, Cannon, Ventura, McFarlane, Perkins, Pearlson and Woods2003); a pWERCAP score >30 was consistent with meeting threshold for psychosis-risk state (sensitivity = 0.89; specificity = 1.0) (Mamah et al. Reference Mamah, Owoso, Sheffield and Bayer2014). The WERCAP consists of 16 question items: the first eight assess bipolar-risk and the latter eight psychosis-risk. Examples of screen questions are provided in a previous study (Mamah et al. Reference Mamah, Owoso, Sheffield and Bayer2014). The severity of symptom items is quantified by rating their frequency of occurrence and, if symptoms are present, their effect on functioning at home , work or school, or with other people . Options for frequency are ‘ none ’, ‘ once ’, ‘ rarely ’ (<yearly), ‘ sometimes ’ (>yearly-monthly), ‘ often ’ (>monthly-weekly) or ‘ almost always ’ (>weekly-daily). Five items in the affectivity section do not have an option for rating symptom effects on functioning, as estimating functionality could not be done accurately or they were considered to not be typically associated with readily identifiable dysfunction. Four options for rating functionality are provided: ‘ not at all ’, ‘ a little ’, ‘ moderately ’ and ‘ severely ’. Total scores are generated by a sum of frequency ratings (0–5) and functionality ratings (0–3) to give a measure of total risk symptom burden. Thus, the maximum score is 49 for the aWERCAP and 64 for the pWERCAP.
For the Rwandan participants, the WERCAP Screen was translated to Kinyarwandan , the language universally spoken and understood in Rwanda. Translation involved initial consensus by three Rwandan researchers fluent in English and Kinyarwandan , followed by a back-translation by an independent research assistant also fluent in both languages who had no knowledge of the original questionnaire's content. There were no issues with understanding of the translated version among Rwandan students. The original version of the instrument was used in Kenya, where English is well understood and used in schools.
Data analysis
General statistical analyses were performed with IBM SPSS Statistics, Version 22.0 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY). Students were compared using analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) and multivariate analysis of covariance (MANCOVA), controlled for age. For multivariate analyses, individual item scores (i.e. frequency + functionality, when applicable) on either the aWERCAP or pWERCAP were used as the dependent variable. Post hoc univariate analyses of aWERCAP or pWERCAP items following respective MANCOVAs were done using Bonferroni correction, with significance set at p < 0.0063 (i.e. 0.05/8). Chi-square (χ 2) analysis assessed differences in symptom prevalence frequencies among the four groups outlined in the aims above.
Results
Demographic information
A Levene's test showed equality of variances (p > 0.05) across the four groups (Rwandans born pre-conflict, Rwandans born post-conflict (after 1994), Rwandan secondary school students and Kenyan secondary school students). Demographic information of the groups is displayed in Table 1. Among the 2800 Kenyans recruited, 2737 reported their gender. Of these, 1467 (53.6%) were female and 1270 (46.4%) were male. Among Rwandans, 1568 reported gender, with 672 (42.9%) females and 896 (57.1%) males. For Rwandan students born in or before 1994, 36.6% of those reporting gender were female with 63.4% male; Rwandans born after 1994 had an even gender report of 50% for both genders.
a Comparison between Rwanda and Kenya secondary school students.
b Comparison between pre/in-1994 and post-1994 born Rwandan students.
Age, aWERCAP and pWERCAP scores are given in means (standard deviation). Gender is given in absolute numbers (percentages).
CID = 95% Confidence Interval of the difference in scores.
Affectivity and Psychosis scores were derived from the total scores from affectivity (#1–8) and psychosis (#9–16) items on the WERCAP Screen.
WERCAP score comparisons
Older v. Younger Rwandan Students
Rwandan students born in or before 1994 had significantly higher total psychosis (p < 0.001) and affectivity (p < 0.001) scores compared with Rwandan students born later (Table 1). Older Rwandan males had higher aWERCAP and pWERCAP scores than younger males (p’s < 0.001). However, while older Rwandan females had higher aWERCAP scores than younger females (p < 0.001), there was no significant pWERCAP difference seen in females. Age-corrected z-scores from univariate analyses for individual WERCAP items are shown in Fig. 1a ; older Rwandans had significantly higher scores on multiple items across both affective and psychotic domains. For older students, 5.35% met the positive pWERCAP threshold while only 3.19% of the younger Rwandan cohort did – a statistically significant finding (χ 2 = 5.36; p = 0.02).
Kenyan v. Rwandan Secondary Schoolers
Table 1 shows the mean total scores across Rwandan and Kenyan students. Total pWERCAP and males’ pWERCAP scores did not differ between countries; differences in pWERCAP in females did not remain after age correction. Similarly, any differences in aWERCAP scores between Kenyan and Rwandan secondary schoolers did not remain after controlling for age.
We also performed MANCOVAs to allow for the between-country comparison of affective and psychosis items as a group as a way to more finely detect differences. A MANCOVA using the eight affective items as dependent variables resulted in a significant difference between Rwandan and Kenyan secondary schoolers (Wilks's λ = 0.905; p < 0.001). Age-corrected affective item z-scores are shown in Fig. 1b . Post hoc univariate analyses showed significant group effects for ‘ hypersomnia and sadness ’ (p < 0.001), ‘ decreased need for sleep ’ (p < 0.001) and ‘ grandiosity ’ (p < 0.001). The largest effect on overall MANCOVA (as determined by observed power and effect size in the corrected model) was for ‘ grandiosity ’, followed by ‘ decreased need for sleep ’ – both of which were significantly higher in Rwandan compared with Kenyan secondary schoolers.
Using psychosis-risk items, a MANCOVA here also revealed a significant group effect between Rwandan and Kenyan students (Wilks's λ = 0.948; p < 0.001). Age-corrected z-scores are shown in Fig. 1b . Post hoc analyses showed significant differences in the items ‘ thought insertion/withdrawal ’ (p < 0.001), ‘ mind reading ’ (p < 0.001) and ‘ persecutory thoughts ’ (p < 0.001); there was trend towards significance for ‘ visual experiences ’ (p = 0.013). All items except for ‘ mind reading ’ were significantly higher in Rwandan students.
The χ 2 analysis showed no significant difference between countries for secondary schoolers meeting threshold for psychosis-risk state (5.22% – Rwanda, 4.6% – Kenya).
Symptom prevalence
Rwandan secondary school students had substantially higher ‘ grandiosity ’ prevalence than their Kenyan counterparts, at all occurrence frequencies (Fig. 2a ). This ranged from 49.1% (Rwanda) v. 21.9% (Kenya) experiencing grandiosity at least once (χ 2 = 350.3; p < 0.001), to 12.7% (Rwanda) v. 3.3% (Kenya) reporting grandiosity almost always (χ 2 = 145.4; p < 0.001). Other affective and psychotic symptoms showed lesser prevalence differences across countries. Pre-1994 and post-1994 birth Rwandans had differences in various symptom frequency prevalences; the most significant difference between groups was for ‘ sadness for at least one month ’. Interestingly, both birth cohorts in Rwanda showed virtually the same prevalence for the grandiosity item across all frequencies (Fig. 2b ).
Frequency of symptom occurrence
Analyses of affective symptom frequencies found multiple items different between countries (Fig. 3a ). Most prominently, mean ‘ grandiosity ’ frequency was substantially higher in Rwandans compared with Kenyans (p < 0.001). In comparing older and younger Rwandans (Fig. 3b ), many symptoms showed older Rwandans experiencing them more often.
Functional impact of symptoms
After Bonferroni correction (p < 0.005), Rwandan secondary school students who reported symptoms had significantly higher functional impairment compared with Kenyans on the items ‘ sadness ’, ‘ irritability ’, ‘ real v. imaginary ’ and ‘ persecutory thoughts ’ (Fig. 4a ). Older Rwandans showed higher impact on functioning than younger Rwandans in six items: ‘ sadness ’, ‘ irritability ’, ‘ real v. imaginary ’, ‘ thought insertion/withdrawal ’, ‘ persecutory thoughts ’ and ‘ auditory experiences ’ (Fig. 4b ).
Gender effects
Mean aWERCAP scores in Kenyan female secondary schoolers were higher than in males (f: 8.99[8.15]; m: 7.92[7.69]; p < 0.001). Mean pWERCAP scores did not show gender differences (f: 8.23[10.3]; m: 8.17[10.1]; p = 0.874). Female students endorsed higher average frequencies of several affective symptoms, although only ‘ irritability ’ showed a statistically significant difference after correction for multiple comparisons (p < 0.003). There was no gender difference functional impairment from symptoms among Kenyans.
Among Rwandan secondary school students, females had higher aWERCAP scores than males (f: 11.78[9.13]; m: 9.50[8.61]; p < 0.001). Females had higher pWERCAP scores as well (f: 9.75[11.5]; m: 7.82[10.4]; p = 0.001). Females endorsed higher symptom frequencies across both affective and psychosis domains compared to males. After Bonferroni correction (p < 0.003), significant differences were observed for ‘ sadness ’, ‘ hypersomnia + sadness ’, ‘ irritability ’, ‘ real v. imaginary ’, and ‘ thought insertion/withdrawal ’ – all of which were more frequent in Rwandan females than males. There were no gender differences in functional impairment from symptoms.
Other analyses
We compared youth born after 1994 in both countries to investigate potential effects of country or post-conflict environment on a population without the direct exposure to the genocide or events of the war. For the aWERCAP, young secondary school students in Kenya and Rwanda did not differ in scores; pWERCAP scores, however, were lower in the younger Rwandan population (Kenya: 8.26[10.2]; Rwanda: 6.88[9.55]; p < 0.001). For secondary schoolers born before 1994, Rwandans scored higher on the aWERCAP (Rwanda: 12.2[9.26]; Kenya: 10.0[8.23]). When comparing across gender, young Rwandan males continued to demonstrate lower pWERCAP scores compared with Kenyans (Kenya: 8.20[10.1]; Rwanda: 5.66[7.83]; p < 0.001) while young females in both countries showed no difference in the pWERCAP (Kenya: 8.28[10.3]; Rwanda: 8.19[11.0]). While young Kenyans showed no difference between genders in the pWERCAP, young Rwandan females had higher scores compared with Rwandan males (p < 0.001). For young Rwandans, significantly more females met the high-risk threshold on pWERCAP compared with males (4.96% v. 1.57%; χ 2 = 6.99; p = 0.008); no such gender difference was seen in young Kenyans.
Discussion
The current study is the first to our knowledge that investigates psychosis-risk or bipolar symptoms in Rwanda. The large sample size (over 5000) provides rich information about the extent of psychotic and affective symptoms. Our findings expand knowledge of war-related psychopathology by investigating a region lacking in research on the subject and by examining youth born after conflict in a country as opposed to only those who lived through conflict. Our study design allowed for both the comparison of Rwandan secondary school students to a similarly aged population in a nearby country as well as an intra-country comparison of Rwandan students born both before and after the genocide and end of civil war. There were several notable findings in our investigation.
Older Rwandans have higher symptom risk and functional impairment than younger Rwandans
Across both symptom domains, Rwandans born in and before 1994 had higher scores than Rwandans born afterwards. Functional impairment was also higher across multiple items and there were significantly more older Rwandans meeting criteria for psychosis-risk state compared with the younger cohort. These differences persisted after controlling for age. As additional evidence of the high likelihood of time of birth affecting symptoms, when comparing Kenyan students born in and before 1994 v. Kenyans born after, there is no difference in psychosis, affective, or functional symptoms after controlling for age. Different symptoms among the Rwandan age cohorts are likely secondary to effects of being alive during the genocide and war as opposed to a simple effect of age. Even though the older cohort was on average less than 2 years old by the war's end and lived through the same post-conflict environment as their younger counterpart, birth before the war's end appears a critical factor in risk. As there are relatively few studies globally looking at the influence of conflict and war on psychotic risk in youth, our findings help to elucidate these putative effects. However, as psychosis-risk determinations have not yet been shown to consistently predict later development of full psychotic diagnoses to satisfactory degree (Yung et al. Reference Yung, Phillips, Yuen and McGorry2004; Cannon et al. Reference Cannon, Cadenhead, Cornblatt, Woods, Addington, Walker, Seidman, Perkins, Tsuang, McGlashan and Heinssen2008; Ruhrmann et al. Reference Ruhrmann, Schultze-Lutter, Salokangas, Heinimaa, Linszen, Dingemans, Birchwood, Patterson, Juckel, Heinz, Morrison, Lewis, von Reventlow and Klosterkötter2010), the full impact of our findings would be further supported with longitudinal follow-up of individuals. Nevertheless, the increased psychotic symptoms and functional impairment – especially in older Rwandan students – are important signs of distress from a public health perspective whether or not there is future development of a diagnosed disorder.
Rwandan students have higher symptom risk scores than Kenyans
Age-corrected multivariate analyses showed multiple affective and psychotic symptoms with higher scores among Rwandans compared with Kenyans, a finding consistent with past studies in conflict areas (Catani et al. Reference Catani, Schauer, Elbert, Missmahl, Bette and Neuner2009; Priebe et al. Reference Priebe, Bogic, Ajdukovic, Franciskovic, Galeazzi, Kucukalic, Lecic-Tosevski, Morina, Popovski, Wang and Schützwohl2010a ; Amone-P'Olak et al. Reference Amone-P'Olak, Ovuga, Croudace, Jones and Abbott2014), though lack of between-country difference on initial summary WERCAP scores suggests caution in over-interpretation of result. However, a very salient symptom with significant group differences in prevalence and frequency was grandiosity – endorsed substantially more in Rwanda. The marked difference between countries may speak to something particular about this symptom in populations living after the cessation of significant conflict. Past research has suggested a development of relative optimism living through the trauma of ongoing conflict in Israel (Bleich et al. Reference Bleich, Gelkopf, Melamed and Solomon2005). An additional factor may be a difference in the ability to appreciate one's existing life situation in the context of a recent ending of societal violence. A study of former American prisoners of war during the Vietnam War showed that the ability to experience post-traumatic growth and obtain a positive outlook after traumatic experiences strongly correlated with appreciation of life (Feder et al. Reference Feder, Southwick, Goetz, Wang, Alonso, Smith, Buchholz, Waldeck, Ameli, Moore, Hain, Charney and Vythilingam2008). This would suggest that grandiosity in these individuals is not necessarily related to psychopathology or the bipolar-risk state, which the WERCAP Screen was designed to measure. That being said, the grandiosity item on the WERCAP probes for the feeling of having ‘great abilities or supernatural powers’ not shared by anyone else in the world. Longitudinal studies of individuals with high grandiosity scores would be needed to determine eventual clinical outcomes.
Of note, when summary WERCAP scores of secondary school students born post-conflict across countries were compared, Rwandans showed no difference in affective scores and had lower pWERCAP scores compared with post-1994 birth Kenyans. This suggests that while Rwandan secondary schoolers show more risk symptomatology than Kenyans, much of this may be explained by the older Rwandans born before the end of the war and genocide.
Symptoms cause more dysfunction in Rwandan than Kenyan adolescents
Among individuals who reported symptoms, Rwandan secondary school students were more functionally impaired from symptoms than their Kenyan counterparts. The reason for this difference is unclear, and may be related to some adverse impacts of a post-war climate in Rwanda. Research on individuals with early psychosis syndromes found greater functional impairment in those who experienced abuse early in life compared with those who experienced none (Alameda et al. Reference Alameda, Ferrari, Baumann, Gholam-Rezaee, Do and Conus2015). Similar results were seen in a 2010 study of correlates to functional outcome among first-episode psychotic patients (Conus et al. Reference Conus, Cotton, Schimmelmann, McGorry and Lambert2010); other studies have shown significant associations between early-life trauma and symptom intensity in bipolar disorder, depression and anxiety (Martins et al. Reference Martins, Von Werne Baes, Tofoli and Juruena2014; Duhig et al. Reference Duhig, Patterson, Connell, Foley, Capra, Dark, Gordon, Singh, Hides, McGrath and Scott2015). Indeed, the extreme stress caused by the aftermath of war and genocide – particularly in children – can help explain much of the risk for psychosis in this vulnerable population (Veling et al. Reference Veling, Counotte, Pot-Kolder, van Os and van der Gaag2016). Neuroendocrine findings on the relationship between environmental stress, cortisol and psychosis risk also provide some clues on potential mechanisms for dysfunction development (Carol & Mittal, Reference Carol and Mittal2015). These findings support the possibility that growing up in a country laden with the aftermath of war would increase the magnitude of dysfunction from psychiatric symptoms.
Rwandan female students report greater psychosis than males
Both Rwandan and Kenyan female secondary schoolers had higher aWERCAP scores than males. However, only the Rwandan secondary school females had increased psychosis severity compared with their male counterparts. Younger (post-1994 birth) Rwandan females also showed more psychosis-risk symptoms than young males (a finding that was not seen with young Kenyans). Additionally, Rwandan females did not show diminution in psychotic symptoms from the older to younger population (as was seen with Rwandan males). Furthermore, significantly more young Rwandan females than males met threshold for psychosis high-risk, while no such gender differences were seen in the young Kenyan sample. Importantly, these gender differences in Rwanda held true for youth as a whole as well as for those born after 1994. While males are known to generally experience more severe psychotic symptoms with average onset several years before females (Loranger, Reference Loranger1984), our findings show that Rwandan students reveal the opposite pattern. This suggests the possibility of an effect of country or post-conflict environment that has differential gender effects. Recent research has shown associations between early experiences of trauma and core features of psychotic illness that are stronger in females than males (Haug et al. Reference Haug, Øie, Andreassen, Bratlien, Nelson, Aas, Møller and Melle2015) as well as the role of stress sensitivity in mediating the relationship between early life traumas and later psychotic symptoms – a phenomenon that has been seen to occur in females as opposed to males (Gibson et al. Reference Gibson, Anglin, Klugman, Reeves, Fineberg, Maxwell, Kerns and Ellman2014). Future longitudinal outcome studies would be helpful to better contextualise the meaning of findings in our investigation.
Limitations
Due to the nature of our study as an examination of the general youth population, we did not have exclusions other than age and school attendance. Our study's setting and logistics precluded us from collecting additional demographic variables such as previous refugee status amongst Kenyans (although the location in Kenya makes such status unlikely), any previous psychiatric history, or drug use. While Kenya is without a recent history of war and genocide, the countries may have other unaccounted for cultural differences that may explain some of the differences seen in symptom report. Additionally, the language used in the administered instrument was different between countries, which could have led to slightly different interpretations of questions despite our diligent efforts during translation to maintain the integrity of the original questionnaire. The grandiosity item in particular may have had different interpretations depending on language and phrasing (Mamah et al. Reference Mamah, Mbwayo, Mutiso, Barch, Constantino, Nsofor, Khasakhala and Ndetei2012; Ndetei et al. Reference Ndetei, Muriungi, Owoso, Mutiso, Mbwayo, Khasakhala, Barch and Mamah2012). However, in a separate analysis grandiosity was also more prevalent and had higher scores in Rwandan secondary schoolers compared with Rwandan college students. This suggests that language and cultural differences cannot account for all disparities observed between countries.
While the use of a self-report screen has some advantages over a structured interview in terms of administration time and in obtaining potentially sensitive information (Bowling, Reference Bowling2005), the ability to accurately interpret a participant's response or to ensure full understanding of an item is less. Additionally, long-term follow-up of individuals would be needed to determine the rate of progression to diagnosable psychopathology, something that cannot be completed with a cross-sectional study. Despite these limitations, this study represents important first steps into wider investigations of psychosis and affective risk symptoms in a part of the world where such research is lacking. An investigative strategy that addresses potential risk burden in a general population exposed to serious conflict has important policy and health provision implications in a non-resource-rich country. Broader and more longitudinal studies along with further work into connections between serious trauma and subsequent development of mental illness – particularly psychosis – will provide important contributions to our knowledge on psychiatric risk in young and vulnerable populations around the world.
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank our research assistants and staff for their hard work and all of our study participants for their time, energy and cooperation.
Financial Support
This work was supported by the National Institute of Health (grant no. MH095645).
Conflict of Interest
None.
Ethical Standards
The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.
Availability of data and materials
The data from our study are not publicly available as data sharing of collected information was not part of the approved IRB documentation across the three review bodies nor was it part of the consent materials provided to participants.