Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-vdxz6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-21T19:59:33.729Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Preserving equipoise and performing randomised trials for COVID-19 social distancing interventions

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 October 2020

Ioana Alina Cristea
Affiliation:
Department of Brain and Behavioral Sciences, University of Pavia, Pavia, Italy
Florian Naudet
Affiliation:
University Rennes, CHU Rennes, Inserm, CIC 1414 (Centre d'Investigation Clinique de Rennes), F-35000, Rennes, France
John P. A. Ioannidis*
Affiliation:
Departments of Medicine, of Epidemiology and Population Health of Biomedical Data Science, and of Statistics, and Meta-Research Innovation Center at Stanford (METRICS), Stanford University, Stanford, California, USA
*
Author for correspondence: John P. A. Ioannidis, E-mail: jioannid@stanford.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

In the coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) pandemic, a large number of non-pharmaceutical measures that pertain to the wider group of social distancing interventions (e.g. public gathering bans, closures of schools, workplaces and all but essential business, mandatory stay-at-home policies, travel restrictions, border closures and others) have been deployed. Their urgent deployment was defended with modelling and observational data of spurious credibility. There is major debate on whether these measures are effective and there is also uncertainty about the magnitude of the harms that these measures might induce. Given that there is equipoise for how, when and if specific social distancing interventions for COVID-19 should be applied and removed/modified during reopening, we argue that informative randomised-controlled trials are needed. Only a few such randomised trials have already been conducted, but the ones done to-date demonstrate that a randomised trials agenda is feasible. We discuss here issues of study design choice, selection of comparators (intervention and controls), choice of outcomes and additional considerations for the conduct of such trials. We also discuss and refute common counter-arguments against the conduct of such trials.

Type
Special Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press

The conduct of randomised-controlled trials (RCTs) is linked to the principle of equipoise, which assumes there is a conflict or uncertainty within the scientific community about the ‘relative therapeutic, prophylactic, or diagnostic merits of a set of interventions’ (London, Reference London2017). Severe acute respiratory syndrome-coronavirus-2 (SARS-CoV-2) is a new and still poorly understood pathogen and in the absence of definitive pharmacologic treatments, non-pharmacological interventions (NPIs) have taken centre stage in the pandemic. These include measures collectively labelled social distancing, such as public gathering bans, closures of schools, workplaces and all but essential business, mandatory stay-at-home policies, travel restrictions, border closures and others. Here, we argue that RCTs are essential to perform in order to generate a reliable basis for major decisions pertaining to these NPIs. The existence of equipoise is an important condition for ethically acceptable research, because its presence indicates that studies are addressing a question of importance and in a way that does not knowingly make any participant worse off. RCTs of social distancing interventions are urgently needed. We offer examples of such trials and discuss issues of study design, choice of comparators, choice of outcomes, other considerations for the viable and informative conduct of these studies and address any residual counter-arguments against their conduct.

Current status of suboptimal alternatives to RCTs: modelling and observational data

When the coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) pandemic hit, no RCTs were available assessing any of these interventions even for other pathogens, e.g. influenza (Fong et al., Reference Fong, Gao, Wong, Xiao, Shiu, Ryu and Cowling2020). Assessment of social distancing measures, including the most stringent ones like horizontal lockdowns, has been exclusively derived from observational (Pan et al., Reference Pan, Liu, Wang, Guo, Hao, Wang, Huang, He, Yu, Lin, Wei and Wu2020) or modelling (Flaxman et al., Reference Flaxman, Mishra, Gandy, Unwin, Mellan, Coupland, Whittaker, Zhu, Berah, Eaton, Monod, Perez-Guzman, Schmit, Cilloni, Ainslie, Baguelin, Boonyasiri, Boyd, Cattarino, Cooper, Cucunubá, Cuomo-Dannenburg, Dighe, Djaafara, Dorigatti, van Elsland, FitzJohn, Gaythorpe, Geidelberg, Grassly, Green, Hallett, Hamlet, Hinsley, Jeffrey, Knock, Laydon, Nedjati-Gilani, Nouvellet, Parag, Siveroni, Thompson, Verity, Volz, Walters, Wang, Wang, Watson, Winskill, Xi, Walker, Ghani, Donnelly, Riley, Vollmer, Ferguson, Okell, Bhatt and Imperial College2020; Lai et al., Reference Lai, Ruktanonchai, Zhou, Prosper, Luo, Floyd, Wesolowski, Santillana, Zhang, Du, Yu and Tatem2020) studies on the current pandemic, and historical analysis of other epidemics (Markel et al., Reference Markel, Lipman, Navarro, Sloan, Michalsen, Stern and Cetron2007). These types of evidence are useful but leave much uncertainty (Fong et al., Reference Fong, Gao, Wong, Xiao, Shiu, Ryu and Cowling2020) and their inferences may be spurious.

Modelling studies are constrained to few alternatives and outcomes and hinge on the estimations of key parameters, which can be inaccurate or change over time. A modelling analysis (Flaxman et al., Reference Flaxman, Mishra, Gandy, Unwin, Mellan, Coupland, Whittaker, Zhu, Berah, Eaton, Monod, Perez-Guzman, Schmit, Cilloni, Ainslie, Baguelin, Boonyasiri, Boyd, Cattarino, Cooper, Cucunubá, Cuomo-Dannenburg, Dighe, Djaafara, Dorigatti, van Elsland, FitzJohn, Gaythorpe, Geidelberg, Grassly, Green, Hallett, Hamlet, Hinsley, Jeffrey, Knock, Laydon, Nedjati-Gilani, Nouvellet, Parag, Siveroni, Thompson, Verity, Volz, Walters, Wang, Wang, Watson, Winskill, Xi, Walker, Ghani, Donnelly, Riley, Vollmer, Ferguson, Okell, Bhatt and Imperial College2020) estimated 3.1 million deaths averted across 11 European countries due to NPIs and lockdown in particular. However, it is impossible to prove this huge benefit and disentangle how much is directly attributed to the implemented interventions and how much to other factors, like the accuracy of the prediction regarding number of deaths in the no intervention scenario. Other analyses of the same data have suggested that lockdown may have saved no lives (Chin et al., Reference Chin, Ioannidis, Tanner and Cripps2020). Moreover, even if the benefit was real, it is difficult to tell which among many measures in the bundle of ‘lockdown’ had the maximum benefits, and which might have had no benefits or even net harms.

Untoward consequences are rarely included, if at all, in such modelling calculations. For COVID-19, examples of harmful consequences of some social distancing interventions included increases in preventable deaths, as compulsory stay-at-home orders might make people wary of seeking medical help for other acute life-threatening problems (Solomon et al., Reference Solomon, McNulty, Rana, Leong, Lee, Sung, Ambrosy, Sidney and Go2020), or potential severe mental health consequences (Czeisler et al., Reference Czeisler, Lane, Petrosky, Wiley, Christensen, Njai, Weaver, Robbins, Facer-Childs, Barger, Czeisler, Howard and Rajaratnam2020) and corresponding attributable excess mortality, owing to increases in domestic violence, unemployment, poverty or social isolation (Moser et al., Reference Moser, Glaus, Frangou and Schechter2020). Mid- and long-term harms may be even more substantial (Ioannidis, Reference Ioannidis2020).

Observational studies, particularly when historical or retrospective, are subject to confounding, selection bias and other biases, and again depend on accuracy and completeness of reporting. Historical analyses have questionable applicability outside of the particular past context. For instance, it is unclear if the experience with a different pathogen (e.g. 1918 influenza) can be transplanted to COVID-19?

For previous pandemics, the dearth of RCTs on social distancing could be defended because such extreme measures were rarely deployed at a global scale for prolonged periods of time. Moreover, in the last 75 years (when RCTs have become available) previous pandemics ended or were controlled before the need of obtaining more certain information about the effectiveness of adopted measures became an issue. Conversely, some modelling studies suggest that COVID-19 may persist into 2024, with prolonged/intermittent social distancing required into 2022 (Kissler et al., Reference Kissler, Tedijanto, Goldstein, Grad and Lipsitch2020). There is vast uncertainty about this claim, because it is still unknown whether ‘herd immunity’ may be reached earlier than anticipated, e.g. because of pre-existing cellular immunity (Grifoni et al., Reference Grifoni, Weiskopf, Ramirez, Mateus, Dan, Moderbacher, Rawlings, Sutherland, Premkumar, Jadi, Marrama, de Silva, Frazier, Carlin, Greenbaum, Peters, Krammer, Smith, Crotty and Sette2020) or because of lack of homogeneity in the mixing of populations (Gomes et al., Reference Gomes, Corder, King, Langwig, Souto-Maior, Carneiro, Goncalves, Penha-Goncalves, Ferreira and Aguas2020). However, the vast timescale, along with the global outreach of the pandemic, creates an imperative to obtain more reliable evidence about benefits and harms of different measures.

RCTs of social distancing interventions conducted to-date in the COVID-19 era

Based on the ‘Living mapping and living systematic review of Covid-19 studies’ (https://covid-nma.com/the-project/), one RCT on social distancing measures in COVID-19 has been completed (Bretthauer, Reference Bretthauer2020), assessing whether gyms can be reopened. This study randomised 3764 individuals to two arms: allowing them to use training gym facilities (n = 1896) v. not using them (n = 1868). SARS-CoV-2 RT-PCR was done after two weeks and clinical assessment covered three weeks. None of the participants had outpatient visits or hospitalisations related to COVID-19 and only one tested positive for the virus, a participant randomised to the training arm but who had not used the facility at all.

Two additional randomised trials were brought to our attention through discussion with colleagues and their results have been released in the National Bureau of Economic Research and the SSRN preprint server, respectively (Banerjee et al., Reference Banerjee, Ferrara and Orozco-Olvera2019; Angrist et al., Reference Angrist, Bergman, Brewster and Matsheng2020). One trial (Banerjee et al., Reference Banerjee, Ferrara and Orozco-Olvera2019) conducted in India randomised people in West Bengal to a messaging campaign where a native Nobel laureate delivered messages v. messaging that referred to government information. Twenty-five million and three million individuals, respectively, were randomised to the intervention and control arms. The campaign was found to double the reporting of health symptoms to the community health workers, decrease travel beyond one's village, increase estimated handwashing, and probably the benefit spilled over to non-mentioned behaviours and also to non-recipients within the same community. The other trial (Angrist et al., Reference Angrist, Bergman, Brewster and Matsheng2020) randomised households in Botswana to three arms (SMS messaging, SMS messaging plus phone calls and control arm) and found that interventions improved the educational experience of students during lockdown.

These trials are imperfect, and they address focused questions, still leaving much to be desired. Moreover, one may wish to see trials addressing more fundamental questions, e.g. school opening rather than ways to improve education with closed schools. Nevertheless, their conduct suggests that the argument that one cannot do RCTs in an urgent situation, is refuted. These trials offer a proof-of-concept that RCTs are feasible.

Resistance against RCTs in COVID-19 and confusing multiplicity of measures

Even if feasible, the conduct of RCTs on social distancing must overcome some substantial resistance of the status quo. Decision makers may not be familiar with or even scared of such designs and may have a defensive view to do anything provided they are not blamed of negligence. Getting buy-in for an RCT is essential for its implementation. A cluster-RCT of school districts with an ingenious rapid-cycle randomisation was proposed for assessing the effects of school closures, but there are no concrete implementation plans (Kolata, Reference Kolata2020). Another similar trial in Norway (http://www.isrctn.com/ISRCTN44152751) suspended recruitment because it could not accrue support from decision makers.

As the pandemic progresses, there is more measured fatigue among both policy makers and the general public. Some social distancing measures may cause major harms, and policy makers need to become aware of these risks and the uncertainty surrounding them. The general public is also starting to see the potentially major repercussions of lockdown measures in their daily life, finances and health. Long-term prospects are uncertain but frightening. If properly explained and supported by scientists, RCTs can hopefully find support in the public argumentation. It requires mostly a sense of honesty: that there is much that we don't know. There is a lot at stake that can end badly unless we find out what the best action plan is.

There is a wide misconception that taking measures (any measures) is a good thing, and that, in principle, taking more measures is even better. Governments, ministries, counties, institutions, businesses, and other authorities have cumulatively issued zillions of measures, orders and practice recommendations during this pandemic, often changing them in rapid sequence and in ways that are confusing and sometimes even obviously self-contradicting. For example, until 29 June 2020 the state of Michigan had issued 132 executive orders and Colorado has issued 115, with each executive order including potentially tens and hundreds of measures and details thereof (https://ballotpedia.org/Executive_orders_issued_by_governors_and_state_agencies_in_response_to_the_coronavirus_(COVID-19)_pandemic,_2020).

Regardless, the lack of consideration of harms in the dominant narrative and messaging regarding social distancing measures is highly concerning. Scientists and experts have routinely communicated the necessity of implementing social distancing to deal with an extremely major threat. Communication overarchingly relied on exuding certainty and instilling fear, most probably under the assumption that by conveying uncertainty, individuals would not comply with the enforced measures. This approach might have been necessary in the critical point of the pandemic wave as hospitals in some regions risked of being overrun, so as to increase adherence to exceptional and costly restrictions. Predictions from mathematical models with highly uncertain early inputs claimed catastrophe (Ioannidis et al., Reference Ioannidis, Cripps and Tanner2020). On the long run, assertive communication (e.g. ‘staying home saves lives’, ‘flatten the curve’) stifled critical discussions and ostracised uncertainty and equipoise considerations. The fact that high-quality data on these measures is lacking and needs to be gathered with urgency has received little attention in communication with the general public. However, past the immediate crisis, it is difficult to hide the huge uncertainty about the best course of action regarding when and how to reopen. Obviously, multiple reasonable competing alternatives exist.

A highly visible example of multiplicity of measures and uncertainty (that should justify equipoise) is what to do with universities. The College Crisis Initiative (https://collegecrisis.shinyapps.io/dashboard/) lists and updates the re-opening plans of ~3000 colleges, community colleges and universities in the United States. As of 26 August 2020, for the fall 2020 out of 2958 institutions, 177 planned to operate fully online, 793 primarily online, 457 adopted hybrid approaches, 578 proposed to operate primarily in person, 73 fully in person, 714 had not yet decided and 166 had other recipes. Within each of these large bins, institutions in the same bin might still differ in essential aspects of how exactly they planned to operate. Clearly, this vast diversity proves that we just do not know what is best to do and there are thousands of opportunities for testing different approaches head-to-head under equipoise. The situation with plans for re-opening of schools is even more confusing than for universities.

Study designs for RCTs of social distancing interventions

RCTs of social distancing measures may randomise either individuals or groups and clusters. They may cover a wide spectrum: from the highly explanatory designs, more akin to clean, lab experiments done under ‘artificial’, ideal conditions among volunteering participants; to pragmatic, population-wide studies of complex interventions that are more messy, but come closer to real-life circumstances.

Designs potentially suitable for population-wide measures, such as cluster randomised trials of parallel clusters or stepped-wedge randomised trials (Hemming et al., Reference Hemming, Haines, Chilton, Girling and Lilford2015), have long existed and applied in other fields, including HIV and other infectious diseases, cancer, service delivery, criminal justice and other. The problem is not the lack of suitable study designs but the inappropriate erosion of equipoise and the potential resistance from politicians and other decision makers who may feel uneasy to say that they are withholding an intervention from some people. Some randomised designs can relieve the pressure from these stakeholders, since the intervention may still be applied to all groups. For example, stepped-wedge designs allow introducing an intervention to different clusters at different time points. Eventually all clusters are exposed to the intervention. However, one can still obtain a measure of the treatment effect from a randomised comparison.

The exact choice of best design depends on features which may vary according to what interventions need to be assessed and at what setting. For example, for cluster trials one has to consider the magnitude of the intra-cluster correlation, the potential for confounding by time (some interventions having different effectiveness when introduced earlier v. later), and the extent to which a cluster intervention may still require measurements in individual participants and, if so, whether these measurements may be affected by lack of blinding and of allocation concealment.

Questions asked

Table 1 shows a number of aspects that can be addressed by RCTs of social distancing. Most trials compare an intervention against an inactive control. Such trials may be feasible also for NPIs, although sham controls are often difficult or impossible. Doing nothing may also be difficult to select as control in charged circumstances. Conversely, questions of dosing and titration of the various interventions may be more acceptable. For each of the adopted measures, one can think of many grades and doses that range from least disruptive to highly disruptive deployments of interventions. The vast diversity that exists on how different measures have been implemented in different countries or locations shows that selection of dose has been highly haphazard and arbitrary. Moreover, there can be widely acceptable equipoise for comparisons of timing of implementation or weaning (e.g. early v. later) of combinations of measures; sequencing of measures; and how exactly different measures can be combined.

Table 1. Aspects of social distancing interventions that can be considered to choose randomised comparisons

Importantly, measures are theoretical constructs and what matters eventually is whether they are adhered to or not. A very aggressive measure may end up decreasing opportunities of infection to a lesser degree than a measure that is less aggressive, if the less aggressive measure may end up being easier to adopt and adhere to. For example, if students are deprived of direct participation in academic life (e.g. interaction with peers or mentors, use of libraries and study rooms, extra-curricular activities), and are constrained to move back home, they may unavoidably find other, perhaps even unsafe, opportunities for socialisation. How exactly adherence is enforced is also worth evaluating in RCTs. There is precedent of RCTs on interventions to improve adherence on other health problems (Kanters et al., Reference Kanters, Park, Chan, Socias, Ford, Forrest, Thorlund, Nachega and Mills2017; Wiecek et al., Reference Wiecek, Tonin, Torres-Robles, Benrimoj, Fernandez-Llimos and Garcia-Cardenas2019), for instance testing advice and messaging strategies (Thakkar et al., Reference Thakkar, Kurup, Laba, Santo, Thiagalingam, Rodgers, Woodward, Redfern and Chow2016). There is also literature on RCTs for using fines and penalties (Blæhr et al., Reference Blæhr, Væggemose and Søgaard2018). These RCT designs can be transferred to the COVID-19 paradigm.

Given that most locations around the world have already adopted various social distancing measures, most of the RCTs done currently could focus more on the weaning of measures rather than their original adoption, but they can also examine questions surrounding their potential re-introduction. For example, a recent study proposed three network-based social distancing strategies designed to mitigate negative effects of social isolation (Block et al., Reference Block, Hoffman, Raabe, Dowd, Rahal, Kashyap and Mills2020). These could be combined with health messaging strategies and tested in an RCT, if the need to increase social distancing in a region becomes stringent. Or the recently proposed ‘precautionary break’ lockdown combining very stringent NPI could be compared with the containment measures ‘as usual’ that are already applied. A modelling study (Keeling et al., Reference Keeling, Guyver-Fletcher, Holmes, Dyson, Tildesley, Hill and Medley2020) appears to show this intervention offers an important ‘brake’ when infection rates are on the rise, potentially allowing other measures (e.g. contact tracing) to be reinstated. Given the substantial economic, societal and psychological costs, it would be useful to evaluate the relative efficacy of such a drastic measure.

RCTs can also address whether any indicators need to be tracked to see if measures are adopted or not, and whether feedback indicators should modulate the packaging and delivery of measures. Such trials can learn from trials that use adaptive designs guided by information on molecular and other markers in cancer and other fields (Korn and Freidlin, Reference Korn and Freidlin2017).

Finally, RCTs may help identifying the optimal target group and setting where different social distancing measures should be implemented. We have substantial epidemiological knowledge already that the risk of serious outcomes and death with COVID-19 has a very steep age gradient (Davies et al., Reference Davies, Klepac, Liu, Prem, Jit, Pearson, Quilty, Kucharski, Gibbs, Clifford, Gimma, van Zandvoort, Munday, Diamond, Edmunds, Houben, Hellewell, Russell, Abbott, Funk, Bosse, Sun, Flasche, Rosello, Jarvis and Eggo2020; Levin et al., Reference Levin, Meyerowitz-Katz, Owusu-Boaitey, Cochran and Walsh2020), and there are strong risk factors reflected in predisposing diseases, and settings, such as specific facilities (e.g. nursing homes (Arons et al., Reference Arons, Hatfield, Reddy, Kimball, James, Jacobs, Taylor, Spicer, Bardossy, Oakley, Tanwar, Dyal, Harney, Chisty, Bell, Methner, Paul, Carlson, McLaughlin, Thornburg, Tong, Tamin, Tao, Uehara, Harcourt, Clark, Brostrom-Smith, Page, Kay, Lewis, Montgomery, Stone, Clark, Honein, Duchin and Jernigan2020)). Trials could compare precise implementation of interventions in specific populations and settings v. wider population-level adoption, or may use risk information to choose the populations and settings where interventions are to be applied.

Trial outcomes

Table 2 summarises considerations on choosing outcomes for social distancing interventions. Much like in other fields of medicine and public health, surrogate outcomes are easier to measure, but efforts should be made to measure outcomes that matter. A core outcome set for large-scale interventions COVID-19 trials is worthwhile developing, as has been done in multiple medical fields (e.g. the Core Outcome Measures in Effectiveness Trials initiative http://www.comet-initiative.org/). Documented COVID-19 infections are straightforward to capture, but even for this outcome, trialists should decide how it will be measured, e.g. with passive data collection or with aggressive testing. Issues of pragmatism also need to be considered (Loudon et al., Reference Loudon, Treweek, Sullivan, Donnan, Thorpe and Zwarenstein2015), because intense laboratory documentation of outcomes may not reflect the real world. Some trials with explanatory designs may nevertheless offer interesting hints about pathophysiology, e.g. transmission from asymptomatic patients or young children. Pragmatic trials, conversely, should focus on major clinical outcomes, e.g. hospitalisation, ICU admission and death.

Table 2. Key features to consider for outcomes for randomised trials of social distancing

It is important to avoid having RCTs focus unilaterally on COVID-19 transmission and health consequences alone. Many measures, especially the most aggressive ones, could be very disruptive to many aspects of life, health and other dimensions. Measuring these additional dimensions of impact would enhance understanding their risk−benefit ratio. These include quality of life indicators, educational attainment, impact on other diseases, impact on health utilisation and impact on social indicators (e.g. violence, unemployment). Capturing these outcomes may require linking different types of databases.

RCTs can also experiment with different timing of measurements and short-term or long-term time horizons for assessing the final endpoints. Carry-over effects from sequential treatments, and interactions can be studied using the knowledge we have accrued on cross-over, adaptive and factorial designs from RCT applications in other fields. Moreover, dealing with COVID-19 is more likely to be a marathon than a sprint and therefore, long-term impact is more important than short-term outcomes. Aggressive lockdown measures may have better outcomes on infection rate in the short term, but if they lead to intervention fatigue and adverse effects on other aspects of health, economy or society, they may end up having a more unfavourable outcome profile than less aggressive interventions. Capturing the big picture of diverse outcomes is most informative.

Rebuttal of additional counter-arguments

Many of the envisioned RCTs of social distancing may use cluster designs and they may not seek informed consent from individual participants. The lack of obtaining classic informed consent in cluster RCTs has already drawn attention and discussion on trials done for other topics in low-income countries. However, there is consensus that the conduct of such trials is entirely ethical, provided that equipoise exists (Weijer et al., Reference Weijer, Grimshaw, Eccles, McRae, White, Brehaut and Taljaard2012; MacKay and Chakrabarti, Reference MacKay and Chakrabarti2018; World Health Organization, 2019). Moreover, one should not forget that all the NPIs on COVID-19, including the most draconian ones, were imposed by governments and other policy makers without any consent of the targeted populations even though they may have placed high risk on the lives and livelihood of the participants.

Some scientists have argued that RCTs are not better, and are actually worse than observational data, for decision making (Deaton and Cartwright, Reference Deaton and Cartwright2018; Horwitz and Singer, Reference Horwitz and Singer2018). However, this position has received critical counterarguments in the past (Ioannidis, Reference Ioannidis2018). This viewpoint has taken an extreme form in the writings, op-eds and social media activity of some militant researchers/activists during the COVID-19 pandemic. For instance, Trisha Greenhalgh, though supposedly an early advocate of evidence-based medicine, has even prophesised that the COVID-19 pandemic will largely be the end of evidence-based medicine and of the need for RCTs (Greenhalgh, Reference Greenhalgh2020). We believe these trenchant positions are premised on a ‘strawman’ argument, which depicts advocating for gathering evidence with RCTs as the equivalent of implying that we do nothing until that evidence is collected and evaluated. To our knowledge, this argument has never been made. We agree that in many circumstances, particularly related to population-wide interventions and health, we cannot wait for evaluating the evidence before we take any action. We do not exclude the importance of natural experiments and a ‘practice-based evidence’ (Ogilvie et al., Reference Ogilvie, Adams, Bauman, Gregg, Panter, Siegel, Wareham and White2020). In truth, this has generally been the way in which NPI were evaluated in previous epidemics, when their deployment was more limited in time, scope and outreach.

Rather, acknowledging that COVID-19 is a protracted and global problem and that a multitude of policy actions to contain it are being deployed and modified in real time, we view RCTs are a way of reducing uncertainty about the chains of effects produced by these actions. Reducing uncertainty is particularly relevant when governments are scrambling with finding the ‘right’ combination and ‘dosing’ of containment strategies that would also minimise economic and societal harms. Moreover, RCTs provide intent-to-treat estimations, particularly relevant for interventions where adherence could be problematic or where the post-randomisation experiences of compared groups (e.g. one intervention could introduce several associated secondary changes) (Ioannidis, Reference Ioannidis2018) could be very different. This has significant pragmatic importance as many containment strategies target difficult to enforce changes in individual behaviours (e.g. government recommendations about the maximum number of people outside of the household that can be invited in private homes).

More broadly, the relative merits of different study designs need continuous evaluation and re-evaluation. RCTs and observational data can have different complementary contributions to generating meaningful evidence in different contexts (Ogilvie et al., Reference Ogilvie, Adams, Bauman, Gregg, Panter, Siegel, Wareham and White2020). Entirely eliminating a study design (actually the study design that inherently carries fewer problems with internal validity) seem to be an extreme position and may be particularly detrimental to science and public health.

A more cogent counter-argument is that although equipoise is a necessary condition for initiating a trial, it must also be the case that research has a strong chance of reducing that uncertainty and changing clinical practice. One may argue that RCTs would not be able to generate adequate evidence to satisfy this condition. We should acknowledge that most studies, both RCTs and others, indeed have limited usefulness (Ioannidis, Reference Ioannidis2016). However, this is not an issue limited to COVID-19 or to RCTs specifically. In fact, COVID-19 offers an opportunity to use what we have learned from research in other fields and to design trials that have a maximum chance of being useful and informative. This has been done multiple times in the past, and there is no reason why it cannot be done also in the COVID-19 setting. Nihilism should not prevail in thinking about an informative research agenda.

Conclusion

Widespread, prolonged implementation of social distancing can have considerable individual and societal harms (Melnick and Ioannidis, Reference Melnick and Ioannidis2020). If the benefit−harm ratio is uncertain, equipoise is upheld and should be communicated, and RCTs should be performed. Other scientists have also started examining the concept of performing RCTs for questions related to the effectiveness or implementation of social distancing policies (Haushofer and Metcalf, Reference Haushofer and Metcalf2020; Starr, Reference Starr2020). Further brainstorming should be encouraged.

Education of both scientists and the general public about the need of preserving and communicating equipoise is crucial. Particularly given the speed with which information is shared on social media, researchers need to understand the importance in communicating uncertainty. Advocacy can be extremely beneficial for societal causes, but detrimental when attached to interventions of uncertain harm−benefit ratio, as are some social distancing measures under some circumstances. Advocacy in this context can undermine equipoise and hamper the planning of RCTs.

Acknowledgements

None.

Author contributions

JPAI conceptualised the paper and wrote, together with IAC the 1st draft of the manuscript. FN substantially revised the subsequent drafts.

Financial support

METRICS is supported by a grant from the Laura and John Arnold Foundation. The work of JPAI is supported by an unrestricted gift from Sue and Bob O'Donnell. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish or preparation of the manuscript.

Conflict of interests

None.

References

Angrist, N, Bergman, P, Brewster, C and Matsheng, M (2020) Stemming learning loss during the pandemic: A rapid randomized trial of a low-tech intervention in Botswana. In SSRN. http://dx.doi.org/ao.2139/ssrn.3663098.Google Scholar
Arons, MM, Hatfield, KM, Reddy, SC, Kimball, A, James, A, Jacobs, JR, Taylor, J, Spicer, K, Bardossy, AC, Oakley, LP, Tanwar, S, Dyal, JW, Harney, J, Chisty, Z, Bell, JM, Methner, M, Paul, P, Carlson, CM, McLaughlin, HP, Thornburg, N, Tong, S, Tamin, A, Tao, Y, Uehara, A, Harcourt, J, Clark, S, Brostrom-Smith, C, Page, LC, Kay, M, Lewis, J, Montgomery, P, Stone, ND, Clark, TA, Honein, MA, Duchin, JS and Jernigan, JA (2020) Presymptomatic SARS-CoV-2 infections and transmission in a skilled nursing facility. New England Journal of Medicine 382, 20812090.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Banerjee, A, Ferrara, EL and Orozco-Olvera, VH (2019) The Entertaining Way to Behavioral Change: Fighting HIV with MTV. National Bureau of Economic Research Working Paper Series No. 26096.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blæhr, EE, Væggemose, U and Søgaard, R (2018) Effectiveness and cost-effectiveness of fining non-attendance at public hospitals: a randomised controlled trial from Danish outpatient clinics. BMJ Open 8, e019969.Google ScholarPubMed
Block, P, Hoffman, M, Raabe, IJ, Dowd, JB, Rahal, C, Kashyap, R and Mills, MC (2020) Social network-based distancing strategies to flatten the COVID-19 curve in a post-lockdown world. Nature Human Behaviour 4, 588596.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bretthauer, M (2020) Randomized re-opening of training facilities during the COVID-19 pandemic. medRxiv: 2020.2006.2024.20138768.Google Scholar
Chin, V, Ioannidis, J, Tanner, M and Cripps, S (2020) Effects of non-pharmaceutical interventions on COVID-19: A tale of three models. medRxiv: 2020.2007.2022.20160341.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Czeisler, , Lane, RR, Petrosky, E, Wiley, JF, Christensen, A, Njai, R, Weaver, MD, Robbins, R, Facer-Childs, ER, Barger, LK, Czeisler, CA, Howard, ME and Rajaratnam, SMW (2020) Mental health, substance use, and suicidal ideation during the COVID-19 pandemic – United States, June 24–30, 2020. MMWR and Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report 69, 10491057.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davies, NG, Klepac, P, Liu, Y, Prem, K, Jit, M, Pearson, CAB, Quilty, BJ, Kucharski, AJ, Gibbs, H, Clifford, S, Gimma, A, van Zandvoort, K, Munday, JD, Diamond, C, Edmunds, WJ, Houben, RMGJ, Hellewell, J, Russell, TW, Abbott, S, Funk, S, Bosse, NI, Sun, YF, Flasche, S, Rosello, A, Jarvis, CI, Eggo, RM and group CC-w (2020) Age-dependent effects in the transmission and control of COVID-19 epidemics. Nature Medicine 26, 12051211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deaton, A and Cartwright, N (2018) Understanding and misunderstanding randomized controlled trials. Social Science & Medicine 210, 221.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flaxman, S, Mishra, S, Gandy, A, Unwin, HJT, Mellan, TA, Coupland, H, Whittaker, C, Zhu, H, Berah, T, Eaton, JW, Monod, M, Perez-Guzman, PN, Schmit, N, Cilloni, L, Ainslie, KEC, Baguelin, M, Boonyasiri, A, Boyd, O, Cattarino, L, Cooper, LV, Cucunubá, Z, Cuomo-Dannenburg, G, Dighe, A, Djaafara, B, Dorigatti, I, van Elsland, SL, FitzJohn, RG, Gaythorpe, KAM, Geidelberg, L, Grassly, NC, Green, WD, Hallett, T, Hamlet, A, Hinsley, W, Jeffrey, B, Knock, E, Laydon, DJ, Nedjati-Gilani, G, Nouvellet, P, Parag, KV, Siveroni, I, Thompson, HA, Verity, R, Volz, E, Walters, CE, Wang, H, Wang, Y, Watson, OJ, Winskill, P, Xi, X, Walker, PGT, Ghani, AC, Donnelly, CA, Riley, S, Vollmer, MAC, Ferguson, NM, Okell, LC, Bhatt, S and Imperial College, C-RT (2020) Estimating the effects of non-pharmaceutical interventions on COVID-19 in Europe. Nature 584, 257261.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fong, MW, Gao, H, Wong, JY, Xiao, J, Shiu, EYC, Ryu, S and Cowling, BJ (2020) Nonpharmaceutical measures for pandemic influenza in nonhealthcare settings-social distancing measures. Emerging Infectious Diseases 26, 976984.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gomes, MGM, Corder, RM, King, JG, Langwig, KE, Souto-Maior, C, Carneiro, J, Goncalves, G, Penha-Goncalves, C, Ferreira, MU and Aguas, R (2020) Individual variation in susceptibility or exposure to SARS-CoV-2 lowers the herd immunity threshold. medRxiv: 2020.2004.2027.20081893.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenhalgh, T (2020) Will COVID-19 be evidence-based medicine's nemesis? PLoS Medicine 17, e1003266.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grifoni, A, Weiskopf, D, Ramirez, SI, Mateus, J, Dan, JM, Moderbacher, CR, Rawlings, SA, Sutherland, A, Premkumar, L, Jadi, RS, Marrama, D, de Silva, AM, Frazier, A, Carlin, AF, Greenbaum, JA, Peters, B, Krammer, F, Smith, DM, Crotty, S and Sette, A (2020) Targets of T cell responses to SARS-CoV-2 coronavirus in humans with COVID-19 disease and unexposed individuals. Cell 181, 14891501.e1415.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haushofer, J and Metcalf, CJE (2020) Which interventions work best in a pandemic? Science 368, 10631065.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hemming, K, Haines, TP, Chilton, PJ, Girling, AJ and Lilford, RJ (2015) The stepped wedge cluster randomised trial: rationale, design, analysis, and reporting. BMJ: British Medical Journal 350, h391.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horwitz, RI and Singer, B (2018) Introduction. What works? And for whom? Social Science & Medicine 210, 2225.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ioannidis, JPA (2016) Why most clinical research is not useful. PLoS Medicine 13, e1002049.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ioannidis, JPA (2018) Randomized controlled trials: often flawed, mostly useless, clearly indispensable: a commentary on Deaton and Cartwright. Social Science & Medicine (1982) 210, 5356.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ioannidis, JPA (2020) Global perspective of COVID-19 epidemiology for a full-cycle pandemic. European Journal of Clinical Investigation n/a, e13421. doi.org/10.1111/eci.13423Google ScholarPubMed
Ioannidis, JPA, Cripps, S and Tanner, MA (2020) Forecasting for COVID-19 has failed. International Journal of Forecasting. doi: 10.1016/j.ijforecast.2020.08.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kanters, S, Park, JJ, Chan, K, Socias, ME, Ford, N, Forrest, JI, Thorlund, K, Nachega, JB and Mills, EJ (2017) Interventions to improve adherence to antiretroviral therapy: a systematic review and network meta-analysis. The Lancet. Hiv 4, e31e40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keeling, MJ, Guyver-Fletcher, G, Holmes, A, Dyson, LJ, Tildesley, M, Hill, EM and Medley, GF (2020) Precautionary breaks: planned, limited duration circuit breaks to control the prevalence of COVID-19. medRxiv: 2020.2010.2013.20211813.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kissler, SM, Tedijanto, C, Goldstein, E, Grad, YH and Lipsitch, M (2020) Projecting the transmission dynamics of SARS-CoV-2 through the postpandemic period. Science 368, 860868.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kolata, G (2020) Did Closing Schools Actually Help? Available at https://www.nytimes.com/2020/05/02/sunday-review/coronavirus-school-closings.html (Accessed 17 June 2020).Google Scholar
Korn, EL and Freidlin, B (2017) Adaptive clinical trials: advantages and disadvantages of Various adaptive design elements. Journal of the National Cancer Institute 109, djx013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lai, S, Ruktanonchai, NW, Zhou, L, Prosper, O, Luo, W, Floyd, JR, Wesolowski, A, Santillana, M, Zhang, C, Du, X, Yu, H and Tatem, AJ (2020) Effect of non-pharmaceutical interventions to contain COVID-19 in China. Nature 585, 410413.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levin, AT, Meyerowitz-Katz, G, Owusu-Boaitey, N, Cochran, KB and Walsh, SP (2020) Assessing the age specificity of infection fatality rates for covid-19: systematic review, meta-analysis, and public policy implications. medRxiv: 2020.2007.2023.20160895.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
London, AJ (2017) Equipoise in research: integrating ethics and science in human research. JAMA 317, 525526.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loudon, K, Treweek, S, Sullivan, F, Donnan, P, Thorpe, KE and Zwarenstein, M (2015) The PRECIS-2 tool: designing trials that are fit for purpose. BMJ: British Medical Journal 350: h2147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacKay, D and Chakrabarti, A (2018) Government policy experiments and informed consent. Public Health Ethics 12, 188201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markel, H, Lipman, HB, Navarro, JA, Sloan, A, Michalsen, JR, Stern, AM and Cetron, MS (2007) Nonpharmaceutical interventions implemented by US cities during the 1918–1919 influenza pandemic. JAMA 298, 644654.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Melnick, ER and Ioannidis, JPA (2020) Should governments continue lockdown to slow the spread of covid-19? BMJ 369, m1924.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moser, DA, Glaus, J, Frangou, S and Schechter, DS (2020) Years of life lost due to the psychosocial consequences of COVID19 mitigation strategies based on Swiss data. European Psychiatry 63, 114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ogilvie, D, Adams, J, Bauman, A, Gregg, EW, Panter, J, Siegel, KR, Wareham, NJ and White, M (2020) Using natural experimental studies to guide public health action: turning the evidence-based medicine paradigm on its head. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health 74, 203208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pan, A, Liu, L, Wang, C, Guo, H, Hao, X, Wang, Q, Huang, J, He, N, Yu, H, Lin, X, Wei, S and Wu, T (2020) Association of public health interventions with the epidemiology of the COVID-19 outbreak in Wuhan, China. JAMA 323, 19151923.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Solomon, MD, McNulty, EJ, Rana, JS, Leong, TK, Lee, C, Sung, S-H, Ambrosy, AP, Sidney, S and Go, AS (2020) The covid-19 pandemic and the incidence of acute myocardial infarction. New England Journal of Medicine 383, 691693.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Starr, P (2020) Using controlled trials to resolve key unknowns about policy during the COVID-19 pandemic. JAMA 323, 23692370.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thakkar, J, Kurup, R, Laba, TL, Santo, K, Thiagalingam, A, Rodgers, A, Woodward, M, Redfern, J and Chow, CK (2016) Mobile telephone text messaging for medication adherence in chronic disease: a meta-analysis. JAMA Internal Medicine 176, 340349.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weijer, C, Grimshaw, JM, Eccles, MP, McRae, AD, White, A, Brehaut, JC and Taljaard, M (2012) The Ottawa statement on the ethical design and conduct of cluster randomized trials. PLoS Medicine 9, e1001346.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wiecek, E, Tonin, FS, Torres-Robles, A, Benrimoj, SI, Fernandez-Llimos, F and Garcia-Cardenas, V (2019) Temporal effectiveness of interventions to improve medication adherence: a network meta-analysis. PLoS ONE 14, e0213432.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
World Health Organization (2019) Ethical considerations for health policy and systems research. WHO.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Aspects of social distancing interventions that can be considered to choose randomised comparisons

Figure 1

Table 2. Key features to consider for outcomes for randomised trials of social distancing