Hostname: page-component-788cddb947-jbjwg Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-10-19T05:14:00.537Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Salience of infectious diseases did not increase xenophobia during the COVID-19 pandemic

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 October 2024

Lei Fan*
Affiliation:
Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Department of Experimental and Applied Psychology, Van der Boechorststraat 7, 1081 BT, Amsterdam, the Netherlands CEPDISC – Centre for the Experimental-Philosophical Study of Discrimination, Department of Political Science, Aarhus BSS, Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark Institute for Brain and Behavior Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
Joshua M. Tybur
Affiliation:
Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Department of Experimental and Applied Psychology, Van der Boechorststraat 7, 1081 BT, Amsterdam, the Netherlands Institute for Brain and Behavior Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
Paul A. M. Van Lange
Affiliation:
Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Department of Experimental and Applied Psychology, Van der Boechorststraat 7, 1081 BT, Amsterdam, the Netherlands Institute for Brain and Behavior Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
*
Corresponding author: Lei Fan; Email: l.fan@ps.au.dk

Abstract

Multiple proposals suggest that xenophobia increases when infectious disease threats are salient. The current longitudinal study tested this hypothesis by examining whether and how anti-immigrant sentiments varied in the Netherlands across four time points during the COVID-19 pandemic (May 2020, February 2021, October 2021 and June 2022 through Flycatcher.eu). The results revealed that (1) anti-immigrant sentiments were no higher in early assessments, when COVID-19 hospitalizations and deaths were high, than in later assessments, when COVID-19 hospitalizations were low, and (2) within-person changes in explicit disease concerns and disgust sensitivity did not relate to anti-immigrant sentiments, although stable individual differences in disgust sensitivity did. These findings suggest that anecdotal accounts of increased xenophobia during the pandemic did not generalize to the population sampled from here. They also suggest that not all increases in ecological pathogen threats and disease salience increase xenophobia.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - SA
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the same Creative Commons licence is used to distribute the re-used or adapted article and the original article is properly cited. The written permission of Cambridge University Press must be obtained prior to any commercial use.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Social media summary: No rise in anti-immigrant sentiments in the Netherlands during COVID-19 was seen, with no support for the disease–xenophobia link.

1. Introduction

Government policies during the COVID-19 pandemic drastically reduced the intermingling of individuals from different nations (e.g. Piccoli et al., Reference Piccoli, Dzankic and Ruedin2021). Anecdotal accounts suggest that such policies co-occurred with broad increases in prejudice towards members of ethnic groups associated with foreign nationalities (e.g. Bhandari et al., Reference Bhandari, Kotera, Ozaki, Abeysinghe, Kosaka and Tanimoto2021; Gover et al., Reference Gover, Harper and Langton2020). Such anecdotes seemingly support hypotheses that humans respond to infectious disease outbreaks with greater xenophobia (e.g. Van Bavel et al., Reference Van Bavel, Baicker, Boggio, Capraro, Cichocka, Cikara and Willer2020; Schaller et al., Reference Schaller, Murray and Hofer2022; Seitz et al., Reference Seitz, Aktipis, Buss, Alcock, Bloom, Gelfand and Haselton2020). The current paper puts such hypotheses to the test by examining the degree to which attitudes towards immigration varied across the COVID-19 pandemic.

1.1. Intergroup contact and infectious disease

Many infectious disease outbreaks are imported from foreign lands. COVID-19 ravaged Chile after emerging in China; the Spanish Flu ravaged India after emerging in the US; and the Black Death ravaged England after emerging in the Asian Steppe. The parasite stress literature proposes that these types of events have also occurred at smaller scales over the course of human evolution, with foraging groups on one side of a river or valley importing novel pathogens to neighbouring groups (Thornhill & Fincher, Reference Thornhill and Fincher2014). A key proposition within this literature suggests that xenophobia has evolved in humans partially to reduce the infection threats posed by intergroup contact. Much of the empirical support for this proposition comes from findings indicating that values encouraging within-group (relative to between-group) assortment are higher in parts of the world with especially high parasite threats (see Fincher and Thornhill, Reference Fincher and Thornhill2012, for an overview). Related studies have reported that implicit and explicit racial prejudice are higher in areas with higher infectious disease rates in the USA (O'Shea et al., Reference O'Shea, Watson, Brown and Fincher2019).

The behavioural immune system literature provides a complementary framework for understanding the proximate mechanisms that might lead individuals in higher parasite-stress ecologies to be more xenophobic (Fincher & Thornhill, Reference Fincher and Thornhill2012). It suggests that when disease-threats are salient, individuals feel more vulnerable to infection, and such perceptions of vulnerability feed into mechanisms that upregulate myriad anti-pathogen behaviours, including xenophobia and the tendency to feel disgust towards pathogen cues. Evidence for causal effects of disease salience on xenophobia comes from experimental studies reporting that negativity towards foreigners is higher among participants exposed to a disease-salience prime than among those exposed to a control prime (Faulkner et al., Reference Faulkner, Schaller, Park and Duncan2004; Huang et al., Reference Huang, Sedlovskaya, Ackerman and Bargh2011; Klavina et al., Reference Klavina, Buunk and Pollet2011; Navarrete & Fessler, Reference Navarrete and Fessler2006). Complementary evidence is provided by studies using individual differences measures: those who report greater explicit disease concerns or who report more disgust towards pathogen cues endorse more negative attitudes towards foreigners (e.g. Aarøe et al., Reference Aarøe, Petersen and Arceneaux2017; Brenner & Inbar, Reference Brenner and Inbar2015; Clifford et al., Reference Clifford, Erisen, Wendell and Cantú2022; Moran et al., Reference Moran, Goh, Kerry and Murray2021; Navarrete & Fessler, Reference Navarrete and Fessler2006).

Although designed to capture the same phenomenon, each of these methods has important limitations. Population-level studies have faced challenges in accounting for dependencies in units of analysis (Bromham et al., Reference Bromham, Hua, Cardillo, Schneemann and Greenhill2018; Currie & Mace, Reference Currie and Mace2012), controlling for confounds (e.g. the fact that Western European institutions were established earlier in areas with low parasite stress; Hruschka & Henrich, Reference Hruschka and Henrich2013) and making inferences about individual-level psychology using group-level data (Pollet et al., Reference Pollet, Tybur, Frankenhuis and Rickard2014). This latter point is especially important, since it restricts the possibility of testing whether individuals who perceive more vulnerability to infection have more xenophobic attitudes. Although experiments priming disease salience via verbal stimuli (e.g. Huang et al., Reference Huang, Sedlovskaya, Ackerman and Bargh2011) or visual stimuli (e.g. Faulkner et al., Reference Faulkner, Schaller, Park and Duncan2004; Klavina et al., Reference Klavina, Buunk and Pollet2011) are better equipped to make inferences about individual-level psychology, they also have limitations (Tybur et al., Reference Tybur, Frankenhuis and Pollet2014). The validity of such procedures is not always clear, and recent well-powered pre-registered studies have not observed effects of such manipulations on intergroup biases (e.g. Makhanova et al., Reference Makhanova, Plant, Ketterman and Maner2022) – an outcome that echoes the low replicability of social psychology studies using similar priming methods (e.g. Mac Giolla et al., Reference Mac Giolla, Karlsson, Neequaye and Magnus2022). Finally, all experimental studies in this area have used between-subjects designs (cf. Ko et al., Reference Ko, Neuberg, Pick, Varnum and Becker2024), which are unable to assess whether xenophobic attitudes vary within individuals as a function of disease salience.

1.2. Using the COVID-19 pandemic to test behavioural immune system hypotheses

The pandemic provided a unique opportunity for such a test concerning the effects of infectious disease on xenophobia. Hospitalizations and deaths from COVID-19 waxed and waned across time, and these changes reflected the likelihood of serious COVID-19 and, presumably, disease salience. These events allowed for research that overcomes limitations of existing cross-population and experimental priming studies. Specifically, they allowed for tests of whether average xenophobia within a population is higher when that population is experiencing a greater infectious disease threat, and they allowed for tests of whether individuals within that population report greater xenophobia when they feel more vulnerable to disease. Given the four-wave longitudinal nature of the current study, we were also able to address continuity and change in the primary variables (e.g. explicit disease concerns, pathogen disgust sensitivity, attitudes towards immigrants) across the pandemic.

In sum, this study provided a test of the hypothesis that attitudes towards immigrants become more negative as a function of pandemic severity. We further tested secondary hypotheses of why the relation between pathogen-avoidance psychology and anti-immigrant sentiments might be moderated by characteristics of the immigrant. One hypothesis suggests that pathogen-based xenophobia might be especially pronounced for immigrants who are more culturally dissimilar (e.g. Murray et al., Reference Murray, Trudeau and Schaller2011). Another hypothesis suggests that pathogen-based xenophobia might be especially pronounced for immigrants who offer fewer benefits as social partners (Tybur et al., Reference Tybur, Lieberman, Fan, Kupfer and de Vries2020; Van Leeuwen et al., Reference Van Leeuwen, Jaeger and Tybur2023). To test these secondary hypotheses, we described an immigrant that varied on his nationality, assimilation to the local culture, ability to provide benefits to locals and willingness to provide benefits to locals before asking participants to report their attitudes towards immigrants like that target.

2. Method

2.1. Participants

Participants were recruited from the Netherlands via the survey agency Flycatcher (https://www.flycatcher.eu/). Data were collected at four time points across the COVID-19 pandemic at approximately 8 month intervals – May 2020, February 2021, October 2021 and June 2022. We excluded six participants who reported inconsistent demographic characteristics (sex, age, education level) across waves. The final sample included 1011 valid responses at Wave 1 (512 female), 724 at Wave 2 (352 female), 558 at Wave 3 (260 female) and 537 at Wave 4 (255 female). We determined the sample size based on our resources rather than a power analysis. Only participants who had completed previous waves were invited to participate in Waves 2 and 3. All participants who had completed (at least) Wave 1 were invited to complete Wave 4.

The study's number of assessments was not pre-specified owing to the unpredictability of the pandemic. Consequently, an a priori power analysis was not employed, nor was a pre-registration. We conclude data collection after Wave 4, after most of the population had acquired partial immunity owing to vaccination and/or infection, and after the most stringent government rules for reducing transmission had expired. A subsequent sensitivity analysis suggested that the design provided 80% power to detect differences in immigration attitudes and pathogen disgust sensitivity of approximately d = 0.08 and d = 0.10, respectively between Wave 1 and Wave 4.

2.2. Procedures and materials

Participants followed the same procedure at each assessment. After providing consent, they were asked to complete a series of instruments, which took on average approximately 20 minutes. Those relevant to this paper concerned attitudes towards immigrants similar to the one described in a vignette (see below), explicit disease concerns, pathogen disgust sensitivity and demographic variables (sex, age and education level). The survey was conducted in Dutch. Materials were originally drafted in English and then they were translated from English to Dutch by a bilingual native Dutch speaker, back-translated into English by a bilingual native Dutch speakers, and checked by a native English speaker for consistency with the original items.

2.2.1. Immigrant description

Participants first read about an immigrant, who was described as coming from one of 25 different nations and as having a name typical of his origin nation. For example, the immigrant from the USA was named James, the immigrant from China was named Yong and the immigrant from South Korea was named Seojun. These nations were selected based on their cultural distances from the Netherlands (data were retrieved from Muthukrishna et al., Reference Muthukrishna, Bell, Henrich, Curtin, Gedranovich, McInerney and Thue2020). We selected 20 nations at approximately equidistant cultural distance points from the Netherlands, and another five nations that allowed for better representation of world regions. The description of the immigrant included three other manipulations: (1) his ability to confer benefits to others (coming either from a wealthy family or from a poor family); (2) his motivation to confer benefits to others (either motivated improve their neighbourhood or to amass possessions for himself); and (3) his assimilation to local norms (either rejecting Dutch social norms and maintaining those of their nation or origin or embracing Dutch social norms while rejecting those from their nation of origin). See the online supplementary information for the details.

For each wave, nation of origin was sampled from the pool of 25 nations without replacement, and values each for the other three manipulations were randomly assigned.

2.2.2. Immigration attitudes

After reading about the immigrant, participants first completed three manipulation check items assessing the immigrant's economic status (‘How wealthy is [TARGET] compared to the average person who grew up in the Netherlands?’), prosociality (‘How interested in helping others is [TARGET] compared to the average person who grew up in the Netherlands?’) and norm assimilation (‘To what degree has [TARGET] adopted Dutch norms, values, and traditions?’) on seven-point scales (for wealth, 1 = much less wealthy to 7 = much wealthier; for prosociality, 1 = not at all interested (in helping others) to 7 = very interested (in helping others); for norm assimilation, 1= not at all (adopting Dutch norms) to 7 = very much (adopting Dutch norms)). We also asked participants to rate the similarity between the immigrant's origin nation and the Netherlands in terms of general culture, religious practices, cuisine, attitudes towards sex and romance, and health and hygiene on seven-point scales (1 = very dissimilar to 7 = very similar).

Following the manipulation checks, participants completed five items assessing their attitudes towards immigrants similar to the one described in the vignette on seven-point scales (e.g. ‘I feel positively towards people like [TARGET] immigrating to the Netherlands’ and ‘I would support policies that allow people like [TARGET] to immigrate to the Netherlands’; 1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree, Cronbach's α = 0.93).

2.2.3. Explicit disease concerns and pathogen disgust sensitivity

Explicit disease concerns were assessed using six items of the disease avoidance subscale from the Fundamental Motives Inventory (Neel et al., Reference Neel, Kenrick, White and Neuberg2016), which were slightly modified to encourage participants to focus on current conditions (e.g. ‘I currently avoid places and people that might carry diseases’ and ‘I am not very worried about getting germs from others right now’; 1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree, Cronbach's α = 0.83). Participants then completed the Three-Domain Disgust Scale (Tybur et al., Reference Tybur, Lieberman and Griskevicius2009) in which they reported the degree to which they find each of 21 statements disgusting (1 = not at all disgusting to 7 = extremely disgusting). For the current study, we examined the seven-item pathogen subscale of the Three-Domain Disgust Scale, which assesses disgust towards infection risks (e.g. ‘Shaking hands with a stranger who has sweaty palms’ and ‘Stepping on dog poop’ (Cronbach's α = 0.77).

2.3. Analysis

Using linear-mixed models with maximal random-effects structures, we first tested whether explicit disease concerns, pathogen disgust sensitivity and immigration attitudes varied across the pandemic. We regressed each of these variables on data collection wave while also modelling a random intercept for participants. For immigration attitudes, we also modelled a random intercept for origin nation. As a robustness check, we also applied Bayesian approach for re-testing our core results. The conclusions were similar. More details are provided in the supporting information.

We also tested the effects of explicit disease concerns, pathogen disgust sensitivity, the three target manipulations (wealth, prosociality and norm assimilation), and the cultural distance of the immigrant's origin nation on immigration attitudes. We initially fitted the models with main effects of these variables, wave and participant sex. In further analysis, we added interactions between data collection wave, explicit disease concerns, disgust sensitivity and characteristics of the immigrant (see the detailed description in each section).

3. Results

3.1. Manipulation checks

Participants perceived countries that are more culturally distant from the Netherlands as less similar to the Netherlands in terms of general culture (β = –0.30, 95% CI [–0.46, –0.14], p < 0.001), religious practices (β = –0.36, 95% CI [–0.49, –0.23], p < 0.001), cuisine (β = –0.25, 95% CI [–0.40, –0.10], p = 0.001), attitudes towards sex and romance (β = –0.33, 95% CI [–0.44, –0.21], p < 0.001) and health and hygiene (β = –0.20, 95% CI [–0.32, –0.08], p = 0.001). Target manipulations also worked as anticipated (for wealth, M High = 4.75, 95% CI [4.64,4.85], M Low = 2.56, 95% CI [2.45, 2.66], t(2772) = 44.71, p < 0.01, Cohen's d = 1.70; for prosociality, M Prosocial = 5.45, 95% CI [5.37, 5.52], M Proself = 3.91, 95% CI [3.84, 3.98], t(2730) = 33.82, p < 0.01, Cohen's d = 1.29; for norm assimilation, M Adop = 5.21, 95% CI [5.13, 5.28], M Reject = 2.64, 95% CI [2.56, 2.72], t(2707) = 51.20, p < 0.01, Cohen's d = 1.97).

3.2. Explicit disease concerns and disgust sensitivity across waves

As expected, explicit disease concerns varied across waves, F(3, 2008) = 238.33, p < 0.001, ηp 2 = 0.26. They were much higher early in the pandemic, when COVID-19 mortality and hospitalizations remained high (M W1 = 5.00, 95% CI [4.93, 5.08]); M W2 = 5.03, 95% CI [4.95, 5.12]) than later in the pandemic, when immunity derived from vaccines and previous infection blunted hospitalizations and deaths (M W3 = 4.30, 95% CI [4.21, 4.40], M W4 = 3.91, 95% CI [3.82, 4.01]). In contrast, pathogen disgust sensitivity varied little across waves (M W1 = 4.77, 95% CI [4.72, 4.83], M W2 = 4.72, 95% CI [4.65, 4.78], M W3 = 4.68, 95% CI [4.62, 4.75], M W4 = 4.72, 95% CI [4.66, 4.79]; F(3, 1927) = 4.00, p = 0.01, ηp 2 = 0.01; see Figure 1).

Figure 1. Official COVID-19 deaths, explicit disease concerns, pathogen disgust sensitivity and immigration attitudes across timepoints during the pandemic. Note: * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01***, p < 0.001. In each plot except the upper left, the horizontal line indicates the mean, the box indicates ±1 standard deviation, the whisker indicates the range, and the shaded area indicates the density of the data with different transparencies indicating quartiles. In the upper-left panel, the y-axis of COVID-19 deaths in the Netherlands refers to the daily new confirmed COVID-19 deaths per million people in the Netherlands. Data were retrieved from JHU CSSE COVID-19 Data (Dong et al., Reference Dong, Du and Gardner2020). Colour bars indicate the approximate survey window of each wave in the current study.

3.3. Attitudes towards immigrants within and between waves

We did not detect differences in attitudes towards immigrants across the pandemic (F(3, 2066) = 0.99, p = 0.39, ηp 2 < 0.01, see Figure 1). Hence, although people felt more vulnerable to disease early in the pandemic relative to late in the pandemic, attitudes towards immigration remained similar across waves.

On average, attitudes towards immigrants were no different when disease concerns were, on average, relatively high (Waves 1 and 2) relative to when disease concerns were, on average, relatively low (Waves 3 and 4). However, this analysis does not address the association of within-person changes in pathogen-avoidance motivations with within-person changes in attitudes towards immigrants. To accomplish this task, we examined relations between attitudes towards immigrants and within-participant changes in explicit disease concerns and pathogen disgust sensitivity as well as the person-level average of these two variables. We averaged values within participants across waves to represent stable disease concerns and pathogen disgust sensitivity, and we subtracted these within-participant means from each observation to estimate within-person change across waves. We then regressed immigration attitudes on both mean- and wave-varying scores for both explicit disease concerns and pathogen disgust sensitivity. Bivariate correlations between these and all other variables are shown in Table 1.

Table 1. Bivariate correlations (N = 2827)

Note: Bold and italics, p < 0.001; bold, p < 0.01, italics = p < 0.05; EDC, explicit disease concerns; PDS, pathogen disgust sensitivity. Cronbach's alphas of multi-item measurements are on the diagonal. a Spearman rather than Pearson correlations are reported for survey wave given its nature. b For wealth manipulation, 0 = from a wealthy family, 1 = from a poor family. c For prosociality manipulation, 0 = to improve their neighbourhood, 1 = to amass possessions for himself. d For assimilation to local norm manipulation, 0 = assimilating Dutch social norms, 1 = rejecting Dutch social norms. e Culture distance refers to the distance between the origin nation of the immigrant target and the Netherlands, retrieved from Muthukrishna et al. (Reference Muthukrishna, Bell, Henrich, Curtin, Gedranovich, McInerney and Thue2020). f For participant sex, 0 = male, 1 = female.

We did not detect relations between attitudes towards immigrants and within-participant changes in either explicit disease concerns (β = 0.00, 95% CI [–0.03, 0.03], p = 0.91) or pathogen disgust sensitivity (β = –0.01, 95% CI [–0.03, 0.02], p = 0.53). Nor did we detect between-participant relations between explicit disease concerns and attitudes towards immigrants (β = 0.02, 95% CI [–0.03, 0.06], p = 0.49). We did, however, detect a relation between between-participant pathogen disgust sensitivity and immigration attitudes (β = –0.07, 95% CI [–0.11, –0.02], p < 0.01). These results suggest that changes in pathogen disgust sensitivity and explicit disease concerns across the pandemic did not relate to changes in immigration attitudes. However, stable individual differences in pathogen disgust sensitivity did relate to general negativity towards immigrants.

3.4. Moderating effects of immigrant characteristics

We detected the effects of all three individual-level immigrant manipulations. Participants held more negative attitudes towards immigrants who came from a wealthy background (M = 4.56, 95% CI [4.48, 4.65]) than a poor background (M = 4.65, 95% CI [4.57, 4.74], t(2440) = 1.97, p = 0.05); those with pro-self orientations (M = 4.31, 95% CI [4.22, 4.39]) than pro-social orientations (M = 4.91, 95% CI [4.83, 5.00], t(2481) = 13.30, p < 0.001); and those who reject Dutch social norms (M = 3.92, 95% CI [3.84, 4.01]) rather than embrace them (M = 5.29, 95% CI [5.21, 5.38], t(2438) = 30.21, p < 0.001). We also observed a weak negative relation between cultural distance of the immigrant's nation of origin and attitudes towards those immigrants (β = –0.03, 95% CI [–0.06, –0.00], p = 0.04).

To test the secondary hypotheses described earlier, we further added interactions between target manipulations and explicit disease concerns, pathogen disgust sensitivity and data collection wave. Only the interaction between person-average explicit disease concerns and the target norm assimilation manipulation was significant (β = –0.08, 95% CI [–0.13, –0.02], t(2393) = 2.62, p = 0.01); stable explicit disease concerns related to immigration attitudes when the immigrant was described as complying with the local norms (β = 0.06, 95% CI [0.00, 0.11]), but not when rejecting them (β = −0.03, 95% CI [−0.09, 0.02]). We did not observe other significant interactions (see Fig. 2). Thus, we did not detect evidence that the features of the immigrants we manipulated moderated the relation between immigration attitudes and data collection time, explicit disease concerns or disgust sensitivity.

Figure 2. Relations between culture distance to the Netherlands, participants’ perceived culture similarity appraisal, cross-wave immigration attitudes changes and regression coefficients between explicit disease concerns, pathogen disgust sensitivity and immigration attitude for each nation used in immigrant origin manipulation. Note: EDC, explicit disease concerns; PDS, pathogen disgust sensitivity. Cross-wave changes = (Wave 3 + Wave 4) – (Wave 1 + Wave 2). In each plot, the regression line indicates the zero-order regression of the variables on the axis. The shaded areas indicate their 95% confidence interval. Correlations reported on top right refer to zero-order correlations between the variables in the plot. The coefficients plotted in the bottom two panels were post-hoc simple slope analysis results of the interaction terms of immigrant origin nation by EDC (F(24, 2250) = 0.78, p = 0.77, ηp 2 = 0.01) and PDS (F(24, 2283) = 0.90, p = 0.61, ηp 2 = 0.01). The models were with control of the fixed effect of all target attribute manipulations, waves, main effect of origin nation and the other individual difference variables, as well as the random effect of participants.

4. Discussion

The current four-wave longitudinal study assessed whether xenophobia varied across the pandemic and whether it varied as a function of changes in explicit concerns about infectious disease and pathogen disgust sensitivity. Although explicit concerns about disease and deaths from COVID-19 were (much) higher earlier in the pandemic, negativity towards immigrants was not. Further, within-person changes in explicit disease concerns and disgust sensitivity did not correspond with changes in attitudes towards immigration. This finding appears incongruent with both anecdotal accounts of increased xenophobia during the pandemic and with findings in the behavioural immune system literature suggesting that disease salience and subjective vulnerability to infections increase xenophobia. We discuss the implications of these findings for our understanding of how the pandemic affected individual psychology below.

4.1. COVID-19 pandemic and anti-immigration attitudes

Although media reports and anecdotal accounts suggest that prejudice towards ethnic groups associated with foreign nationalities increased during the pandemic, the current study did not detect such changes. The stability in anti-immigrant sentiments we observed mirrors results from some other recent multi-wave surveys. One study conducted in the USA detected an increase in right-wing authoritarianism as COVID infections rose, but no increase in xenophobia (Pazhoohi & Kingstone, Reference Pazhoohi and Kingstone2021). Another conducted in Germany detected no increase in stigmatization of Chinese and Asian-looking people (Koller et al., Reference Koller, Villinger, Lages, Brünecke, Debbeler, Engel and Schupp2021). And another revealed that anti-immigrant prejudice in the Netherlands was no higher during in May 2020 than in 2017 (Muis & Reeskens, Reference Muis and Reeskens2022). These findings echo conclusions from a systematic review indicating that the pandemic caused changes in feelings of threat but very limited changes in attitudes (Brandt et al., Reference Brandt, Vallabha and Turner-Zwinkels2023). These (lack of) changes have especially important implications for how we understand the behavioural immune system.

4.2. The COVID-19 pandemic and the BIS

Disgust is often described as the motivational component of the behavioural immune system. Given predictions that the behavioural immune system should upregulate pathogen-avoidance motives when disease is salient, researchers have proposed that (pathogen) disgust sensitivity should increase during the pandemic. Some have interpreted data as supporting that proposal (e.g. Boggs et al., Reference Boggs, Ruisch and Fazio2022; Stevenson et al., Reference Stevenson, Saluja and Case2021), although others have reported small-to-no changes in disgust sensitivity (e.g. Carr et al., Reference Carr, Breese, Heath and McMullan2022; Miłkowska et al., Reference Miłkowska, Galbarczyk, Mijas and Jasienska2021; Stefanczyk et al., Reference Stefanczyk, Rokosz and Białek2024; Tybur et al., Reference Tybur, Fan, Jones, Holzleitner, Lee and DeBruine2022). The current study was more in line with the second set of findings and, although the current study did not include a pre-pandemic assessment, means in pathogen disgust sensitivity, which ranged from 4.68 to 4.77 across waves, were almost identical to (and, if anything, slightly lower than) the 4.82 reported in a similar sample recruited from the same survey company within the same country a decade earlier (Tybur & de Vries, Reference Tybur and de Vries2013).

The lack of an effect of COVID-19 on pathogen disgust sensitivity has two broad theoretical implications. First, in contrast to a widespread treatment of disgust sensitivity and explicit disease concerns as interchangeable constructs (e.g. Oosterhoff et al., Reference Oosterhoff, Shook and Iyer2018), the two pathogen-avoidance variables seem differentially responsive to threats posed by COVID-19 (cf. Ackerman et al., Reference Ackerman, Tybur and Blackwell2021). Second, and related, the present findings suggest that the presence of a novel respiratory pathogen – as well as ubiquitous socially transmitted information about that pathogen – probably does not act as input into whatever mechanisms output disgust responses.

Existing hypotheses have also suggested that the behavioural immune system might trigger or increase negativity towards only certain types of immigrants. We tested one such proposal by manipulating the origin nation of the immigrant and estimating the cultural distance between the origin nation and the host nation. Participants perceived more culturally distant nations as more culturally dissimilar. However, we did not detect interactions between cultural distance and time of assessment during the pandemic, explicit disease concerns or disgust sensitivity. Similarly, pathogen-relevant variables largely did not moderate attitudes towards immigrants who were more vs. less willing to provide resources, more vs. less able to provide resources and more vs. less assimilated. This result raises questions regarding the robustness of previous findings indicating that pathogen-avoidance psychology only motivates antipathy towards certain types of immigrants (e.g. Faulkner et al., Reference Faulkner, Schaller, Park and Duncan2004; Karinen et al., Reference Karinen, Molho, Kupfer and Tybur2019; Ji et al., Reference Ji, Tybur and van Vugt2019). In concert with findings that motivations to avoid interpersonal contact are much more strongly influenced by a target's symptoms of infection and their relationship closeness rather than their ethnic or national group membership (Fan et al., Reference Fan, Tybur and Jones2022; Tybur et al., Reference Tybur, Lieberman, Fan, Kupfer and de Vries2020; van Leeuwen & Peterson, Reference Van Leeuwen and Petersen2018; also see Bressan, Reference Bressan2021, Reference Bressan2023), the current findings suggest that the social effects of the behavioural immune system might be more limited than previously proposed (see also Makhanova et al., Reference Makhanova, Plant, Ketterman and Maner2022).

4.3. Implications for parasite stress theory

Parasite stress theory suggests that, over human evolutionary history, intergroup contact has posed greater infectious disease costs than intragroup contact, and that the relative risks of intergroup contact are greater in ecologies with greater parasite stress. Consequently, humans have evolved to respond to ecological parasite stress by adjusting attitudes, beliefs and values in a way that increases intragroup assortment and decreases intergroup assortment (Fincher & Thornhill, Reference Fincher and Thornhill2012). This proposal has been debated. For example, some have pointed out that the relation between group membership and pathogen threat is not straightforward, since pathogens sometimes adapt to infect their local hosts (de Barra and Curtis, Reference De Barra and Curtis2012). Further, even if outgroup pathogens are more dangerous, individual avoidance of outgroups might offer little protection against infection. If a single ingroup member interacts with outgroup members, the pathogen might spread to even those who avoided outgroups themselves. Hence, xenophobic individuals might miss out on all the benefits of intergroup interactions (e.g. trade, mating, knowledge transferring, access to territory) and still succumb to infections indirectly transmitted from outgroups (Fessler et al., Reference Fessler, Clark, Clint and Buss2015). Finally, parasite theory has been evaluated mostly by examining correlations between nation-level estimates of parasite stress and nation-level estimates of variables that putatively reflect preferences for intragroup assortment over intergroup assortment. This approach has been critiqued for multiple reasons, including its vulnerability to statistical artefacts (Bromham et al., Reference Bromham, Hua, Cardillo, Schneemann and Greenhill2018; Currie & Mace, Reference Currie and Mace2012), its (in)ability to evaluate causal relationships (Hruschka & Henrich, Reference Hruschka and Henrich2013; Pollet et al., Reference Pollet, Tybur, Frankenhuis and Rickard2014), and its (in)ability to distinguish between evoked and transmitted cultural mechanisms (Schaller & Murray, Reference Schaller and Murray2012). The issue of evoked vs. transmitted culture is especially relevant to the current study. Evoked culture would lead to cross-cultural variability because the features of the ecology taken as input by universal pathogen-avoidance adaptations vary across cultures; the latter would lead to the same variability because information and resulting norms are better transmitted, reproduced and retained in some ecologies than others (Gangestad et al., Reference Gangestad, Haselton and Buss2006; Tooby & Cosmides, Reference Tooby, Cosmides, Barkow, Cosmides and Tooby1992).

The current study speaks against one possible route through which xenophobia could reflect evoked culture as implied by parasite stress theory. The introduction of virulent novel pathogen and ubiquitous social information about that pathogen did not lead to more negativity towards individuals from foreign ecologies. Hence, if variation in parasites indeed leads to variation in xenophobia, it does so via adaptations that take other information as input (e.g. inflammation; Fincher & Thornhill, Reference Fincher and Thornhill2012) or via transmitted culture. The latter could take at least two forms. First, norms that limit intergroup interactions could be better transmitted and retained if they give greater benefits in high parasite stress ecologies. Second, pathogen-avoidance psychology could act as a cultural attractor for the development of ethnocentric beliefs (Fessler & Machery, Reference Fessler, Machery, Margolis, Samuels and Stich2012), potentially as a byproduct of the lower social affordances of outgroup members relative to ingroup members (cf. Tybur et al., Reference Tybur, Lieberman, Fan, Kupfer and de Vries2020). These beliefs could be advantageous for groups even if such advantages are unrelated to avoiding infection (Fessler et al., Reference Fessler, Clark, Clint and Buss2015). Naturally, more work is needed to test these ideas.

4.4. Limitations and future directions

We recruited our sample from the Netherlands – a nation that is relatively multicultural, liberal and developed (Spiecker & Steutel, Reference Spiecker and Steutel2001). Inferences based on the current study might be limited to the population surveyed (or similar populations). As one example, attitudes towards poor immigrants were more positive than attitudes towards wealthy immigrants – a pattern that might emerge only in populations with economic and sociopolitical characteristics similar to that sampled from here. We did not have pre-pandemic data on this panel. Thus, while we can assess change from early in the pandemic to a period after a widespread vaccination campaign, we cannot compare post-pandemic data with pre-pandemic data. The current study was not pre-registered, and we encourage secondary data analysis to verify the robustness of the conclusions reported here. Finally, the lack of an overall change in xenophobia does not preclude the possibility that the pandemic played a causal role in increasing intergroup violence perpetrated by a small number of individuals, even if attitudes towards immigrants did not change on average in the population sampled from. Naturally, the current findings do not speak to the impact or importance of the violence and harassment experienced by those targeted by such behaviours, regardless of such a causal relation.

While not suggesting that infectious disease is irrelevant to anti-immigrant sentiments, the current findings constrain the likely manner in which pathogens influence such prejudices. The presence of a novel pathogen (here, SARS-CoV-2), ubiquitous media attention to infectious disease, and shifts in subjective disease concerns do not appear to be relevant inputs into any specialized psychological mechanisms that would output xenophobia – at least not in the population sampled from here. Any systematic shifts in prejudice instead might have been deployed towards ecological ingroup members who flaunted anti-COVID rules and recommendations (e.g. Republicans in the USA; Ko et al., Reference Ko, Neuberg, Pick, Varnum and Becker2024).

Ultimately, the scientific and societal merit of the present findings derives from contributing novel insights into understanding how pathogen threat relates to xenophobia. While the belief in an association seems widely shared among scientists and the lay public, the present findings suggest that the association is not ubiquitous and certainly should not be considered a law of psychology. Future work in the parasite stress and behavioural immune system literatures can use these findings to triangulate upon the pathogen-relevant factors that might still contribute to xenophobia, even if a respiratory pandemic does not appear to be one of those factors. Regardless of the outcome of this research programme, the current results offer some reassurance that the inevitable respiratory pandemics of the future need not cause widespread increases in xenophobia.

Supplementary material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/ehs.2024.28

Acknowledgements

We thank the Department of Experimental and Applied Psychology, Vrije Universiteit for hosting the project. We also thank our colleagues, reviewers and editors for their help during the study production and publication.

Author contributions

All authors conceptualized the study. JT and LF curated the data. LF performed the formal analysis. JT and LF acquired the funding. LF conducted the investigation. JT and LF developed the methodology. All authors managed the project administration. JT and LF provided the resources. PL and JT supervised the work. LF created the visualizations. LF and JT wrote the original draft. All authors reviewed and edited the manuscript.

Financial support

This project is funded by China Scholarship Council (201806990045) and European Research Council (ERC-2015-StG HBIS 680002; ERC-2021-COG NONPHARM 101045225). Lei Fan is currently funded by the Danish National Research Foundation (DNRF144).

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Research transparency and reproducibility

Ethical approval of the current project was granted by the Scientific and Ethical Review Board (VCWE) of the Faculty of Behavior & Movement Sciences, VU University Amsterdam. Additional information is reported in the supporting information.

Data availability

Materials, data and analysis scripts are available on OSF (https://osf.io/ghbaf/).

References

Aarøe, L., Petersen, M. B., & Arceneaux, K. (2017). The behavioral immune system shapes political intuitions: Why and how individual differences in disgust sensitivity underlie opposition to immigration. The American Political Science Review, 111(2), 277. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0003055416000770CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ackerman, J. M., Tybur, J. M., & Blackwell, A. D. (2021). What role does pathogen-avoidance psychology play in pandemics? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 25(3), 177186. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2020.11.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bhandari, D., Kotera, Y., Ozaki, A., Abeysinghe, S., Kosaka, M., & Tanimoto, T. (2021). COVID-19: Challenges faced by Nepalese migrants living in Japan. BMC Public Health, 21(1), 752. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-021-10796-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boggs, S. T., Ruisch, B. C., & Fazio, R. H. (2022). Concern about salient pathogen threats increases sensitivity to disgust. Personality and Individual Differences, 186, 111348. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2021.111348CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brandt, M. J., Vallabha, S., & Turner-Zwinkels, F. M. (2023). The Onset of the COVID-19 pandemic made people feel threatened, but had a limited impact on political attitudes in the United States. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 01461672231190233. https://doi.org/10.1177/01461672231190233CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brenner, C. J., & Inbar, Y. (2015). Disgust sensitivity predicts political ideology and policy attitudes in the Netherlands. European Journal of Social Psychology, 45(1), 2738. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2072CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bressan, P. (2021). Strangers look sicker (with implications in times of COVID-19). BioEssays, 43(3), 2000158. https://doi.org/10.1002/bies.202000158CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bressan, P. (2023). Racism is not about ‘race’. Scientific Reports, 13(1), 22339. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-47653-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bromham, L., Hua, X., Cardillo, M., Schneemann, H., & Greenhill, S. J. (2018). Parasites and politics: Why cross-cultural studies must control for relatedness, proximity and covariation. Royal Society Open Science, 5(8), 181100. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.181100CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carr, P., Breese, E., Heath, C. J., & McMullan, R. (2022). The effect of the COVID-19 pandemic on disgust sensitivity in a sample of UK adults. Front Public Health, 10, 1020850. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpubh.2022.1020850CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clifford, S., Erisen, C., Wendell, D., & Cantú, F. (2022). Disgust sensitivity and support for immigration across five nations. Politics and the Life Sciences, 42(1) 6580. https://doi.org/10.1017/pls.2022.6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Currie, T. E., & Mace, R. (2012). Analyses do not support the parasite-stress theory of human sociality. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35(2), 8385. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X11000963CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Barra, M., & Curtis, V. (2012). Are the pathogens of out-groups really more dangerous? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35(2), 85. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X11000975CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dong, E. S., Du, H. R., & Gardner, L. (2020). An interactive web-based dashboard to track COVID-19 in real time. Lancet Infectious Diseases, 20(5), 533534. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1473-3099(20)30120-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fan, L., Tybur, J. M., & Jones, B. C. (2022). Are people more averse to microbe-sharing contact with ethnic outgroup members? A registered report. Evolution and Human Behavior, 43(6), 490500. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2022.08.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Faulkner, J., Schaller, M., Park, J. H., & Duncan, L. A. (2004). Evolved disease-avoidance mechanisms and contemporary xenophobic attitudes. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 7(4), 333353. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430204046142CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fessler, D. M., & Machery, E. (2012). Culture and Cognition. In Margolis, E., Samuels, R., & Stich, S. P. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of philosophy of cognitive science. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195309799.013.0021Google Scholar
Fessler, D. M., Clark, J. A., & Clint, E. K. (2015). Evolutionary psychology and evolutionary anthropology. In Buss, D. M. (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology: Integrations (pp. 10291046). John Wiley.Google Scholar
Fincher, C. L., & Thornhill, R. (2012). Parasite-stress promotes in-group assortative sociality: The cases of strong family ties and heightened religiosity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35(2), 6179. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X11000021CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gangestad, S. W., Haselton, M. G., & Buss, D. M. (2006). Evolutionary foundations of cultural variation: Evoked culture and mate preferences. Psychological Inquiry, 17(2), 7595. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327965pli1702_1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gover, A. R., Harper, S. B., & Langton, L. (2020). Anti-Asian hate crime during the COVID-19 pandemic: Exploring the reproduction of inequality. American Journal of Criminal Justice, 45(4), 647667. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12103-020-09545-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hruschka, D. J., & Henrich, J. (2013). Institutions, parasites and the persistence of in-group preferences. Plos One, 8(5), e63642. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0063642CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huang, J. Y., Sedlovskaya, A., Ackerman, J. M., & Bargh, J. A. (2011). Immunizing against prejudice: Effects of disease protection on attitudes toward out-groups. Psychological Science, 22(12), 15501556. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611417261CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ji, T., Tybur, J. M., & van Vugt, M. (2019). Generalized or origin-specific out-group prejudice?: The role of temporary and chronic pathogen-avoidance motivation in intergroup relations. Evolutionary Psychology, 17(1), 1474704919826851. https://doi.org/10.1177/1474704919826851CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karinen, A. K., Molho, C., Kupfer, T. R., & Tybur, J. M. (2019). Disgust sensitivity and opposition to immigration: Does contact avoidance or resistance to foreign norms explain the relationship? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 84. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2019.103817CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klavina, L., Buunk, A., & Pollet, T. (2011). Out-group mating threat and disease threat increase implicit negative attitudes toward the out-group among men. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 76. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00076CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ko, A., Neuberg, S. L., Pick, C. M., Varnum, M. E. W., & Becker, D. V. (2024, January 18). Responses to political partisans are shaped by a COVID-sensitive disease avoidance psychology: A longitudinal investigation of functional flexibility. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/zkxu7Google Scholar
Koller, J. E., Villinger, K., Lages, N. C., Brünecke, I., Debbeler, J. M., Engel, K. D., … & Schupp, H. T. (2021). Stigmatization of Chinese and Asian-looking people during the COVID-19 pandemic in Germany. BMC Public Health, 21(1), 17. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-021-11270-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mac Giolla, E., Karlsson, S., Neequaye, D. A., & Magnus, B. (2022). Evaluating the replicability of social priming studies. PsyArxiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/dwg9vGoogle Scholar
Makhanova, A., Plant, E. A., Ketterman, A. B., & Maner, J. K. (2022). Pathogen threat and intergroup prejudice using the minimal group paradigm: Evidence from a registered report. Evolution and Human Behavior, 43(5), 347357. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2022.05.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miłkowska, K., Galbarczyk, A., Mijas, M., & Jasienska, G. (2021). Disgust sensitivity among women during the COVID-19 outbreak. Front Psychol, 12, 622634. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.622634CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moran, J. B., Goh, J. X., Kerry, N., & Murray, D. R. (2021). Outbreaks and outgroups: Three tests of the relationship between disease avoidance motives and xenophobia during an emerging pandemic. Evolutionary Psychological Science, 7(4), 419429. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40806-021-00283-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muis, Q., & Reeskens, T. (2022). Are we in this together? Changes in anti-immigrant sentiments during the COVID-19 pandemic. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 86, 203216. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijintrel.2021.12.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, D. R., Trudeau, R., & Schaller, M. (2011). On the origins of cultural differences in conformity: Four tests of the pathogen prevalence hypothesis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37(3), 318329. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167210394451CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muthukrishna, M., Bell, A. V., Henrich, J., Curtin, C. M., Gedranovich, A., McInerney, J., & Thue, B. (2020). Beyond Western, Educated, Industrial, Rich, and Democratic (WEIRD) psychology: Measuring and mapping scales of cultural and psychological distance. Psychological Science, 31(6), 678701. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797620916782CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Navarrete, C. D., & Fessler, D. M. (2006). Disease avoidance and ethnocentrism: The effects of disease vulnerability and disgust sensitivity on intergroup attitudes. Evolution and Human Behavior, 27(4), 270282. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2005.12.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neel, R., Kenrick, D. T., White, A. E., & Neuberg, S. L. (2016). Individual differences in fundamental social motives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 110(6), 887907. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000068CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Shea, B. A., Watson, D. G., Brown, G. D. A., & Fincher, C. L. (2019). Infectious disease prevalence, not race exposure, predicts both implicit and explicit racial prejudice across the United States. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 11(3), 345355. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550619862319CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oosterhoff, B., Shook, N. J., & Iyer, R. (2018). Disease avoidance and personality: A meta-analysis. Journal of Research in Personality, 77, 4756. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2018.09.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pazhoohi, F., & Kingstone, A. (2021). Associations of political orientation, xenophobia, right-wing authoritarianism, and concern of COVID-19: Cognitive responses to an actual pathogen threat. Personality and Individual Differences, 182, 111081. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2021.111081CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piccoli, L., Dzankic, J., & Ruedin, D. (2021). Citizenship, Migration and Mobility in a Pandemic (CMMP): A global dataset of COVID-19 restrictions on human movement. Plos One, 16(3). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0248066CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pollet, T. V., Tybur, J. M., Frankenhuis, W. E., & Rickard, I. J. (2014). What can cross-cultural correlations teach us about human nature? Human Nature, 25(3), 410429. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-014-9206-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schaller, M., & Murray, D. R. (2012). Mechanisms by which parasites influence cultures, and why they matter. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35(2), 9192. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X11001038CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schaller, M., Murray, D. R., & Hofer, M. K. (2022). The behavioural immune system and pandemic psychology: The evolved psychology of disease-avoidance and its implications for attitudes, behaviour, and public health during epidemic outbreaks. European Review of Social Psychology, 33(2), 360396. https://doi.org/10.1080/10463283.2021.1988404CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seitz, B. M., Aktipis, A., Buss, D. M., Alcock, J., Bloom, P., Gelfand, M., …, Haselton, M. G. (2020). The pandemic exposes human nature: 10 evolutionary insights. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 117(45), 2776727776. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2009787117CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spiecker, B., & Steutel, J. (2001). Multiculturalism, pillarization and liberal civic education in the Netherlands. International Journal of Educational Research, 35(3), 293304. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0883-0355(01)00025-8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stefanczyk, M. M., Rokosz, M., & Białek, M. (2024). Changes in perceived vulnerability to disease, resilience, and disgust sensitivity during the pandemic: A longitudinal study. Current Psychology, 43, 2341223424. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-024-05825-9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stevenson, R. J., Saluja, S., & Case, T. I. (2021). The impact of the Covid-19 pandemic on disgust sensitivity. Frontiers in Psychology, 11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.600761CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thornhill, R., & Fincher, C. L. (2014). The parasite-stress theory of values and sociality: Infectious disease, history and human values worldwide (1st ed.). Springer International. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-08040-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1992). The psychological foundations of culture. In Barkow, J. H., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (Eds.), The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 19136). Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tybur, J. M., & de Vries, R. E. (2013). Disgust sensitivity and the HEXACO model of personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 55(6), 660665. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2013.05.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tybur, J. M., Lieberman, D., & Griskevicius, V. (2009). Microbes, mating, and morality: Individual differences in three functional domains of disgust. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(1), 103122. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015474CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tybur, J. M., Frankenhuis, W. E., & Pollet, T. V. (2014). Behavioral immune system methods: Surveying the present to shape the future. Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences, 8(4), 274283. https://doi.org/10.1037/ebs0000017CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tybur, J. M., Lieberman, D., Fan, L., Kupfer, T. R., & de Vries, R. E. (2020). Behavioral immune trade-offs: Interpersonal value relaxes social pathogen avoidance. Psychological Science, 31(10), 12111221.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tybur, J. M., Fan, L., Jones, B. C., Holzleitner, I. J., Lee, A. J., & DeBruine, L. M. (2022). Re-evaluating the relationship between pathogen avoidance and preferences for facial symmetry and sexual dimorphism: A registered report. Evolution and Human Behavior. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2022.01.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Bavel, J. J., Baicker, K., Boggio, P. S., Capraro, V., Cichocka, A., Cikara, M., …, Willer, R. (2020). Using social and behavioural science to support COVID-19 pandemic response. Nature Human Behaviour, 4(5), 460471. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-020-0884-zCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Leeuwen, F., & Petersen, M. B. (2018). The behavioral immune system is designed to avoid infected individuals, not outgroups. Evolution and Human Behavior, 39(2), 226234. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2017.12.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Leeuwen, F., Jaeger, B. & Tybur, J. M. (2023) A behavioural immune system perspective on disgust and social prejudice. Nature Reviews Psychology, 2, 676687. https://doi.org/10.1038/s44159-023-00226-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Official COVID-19 deaths, explicit disease concerns, pathogen disgust sensitivity and immigration attitudes across timepoints during the pandemic. Note: * p < 0.05, **p < 0.01***, p < 0.001. In each plot except the upper left, the horizontal line indicates the mean, the box indicates ±1 standard deviation, the whisker indicates the range, and the shaded area indicates the density of the data with different transparencies indicating quartiles. In the upper-left panel, the y-axis of COVID-19 deaths in the Netherlands refers to the daily new confirmed COVID-19 deaths per million people in the Netherlands. Data were retrieved from JHU CSSE COVID-19 Data (Dong et al., 2020). Colour bars indicate the approximate survey window of each wave in the current study.

Figure 1

Table 1. Bivariate correlations (N = 2827)

Figure 2

Figure 2. Relations between culture distance to the Netherlands, participants’ perceived culture similarity appraisal, cross-wave immigration attitudes changes and regression coefficients between explicit disease concerns, pathogen disgust sensitivity and immigration attitude for each nation used in immigrant origin manipulation. Note: EDC, explicit disease concerns; PDS, pathogen disgust sensitivity. Cross-wave changes = (Wave 3 + Wave 4) – (Wave 1 + Wave 2). In each plot, the regression line indicates the zero-order regression of the variables on the axis. The shaded areas indicate their 95% confidence interval. Correlations reported on top right refer to zero-order correlations between the variables in the plot. The coefficients plotted in the bottom two panels were post-hoc simple slope analysis results of the interaction terms of immigrant origin nation by EDC (F(24, 2250) = 0.78, p = 0.77, ηp2 = 0.01) and PDS (F(24, 2283) = 0.90, p = 0.61, ηp2 = 0.01). The models were with control of the fixed effect of all target attribute manipulations, waves, main effect of origin nation and the other individual difference variables, as well as the random effect of participants.

Supplementary material: File

Fan et al. supplementary material

Fan et al. supplementary material
Download Fan et al. supplementary material(File)
File 42 KB