Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-dh8gc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-10-31T03:33:37.895Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Aerobic fitness and multidomain cognitive function in advanced age

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 June 2010

Yael Netz*
Affiliation:
The Zinman College of Physical Education and Sport Sciences, Wingate Institute, Israel
Tzvi Dwolatzky
Affiliation:
Department of Geriatrics and Memory Clinic, Mental Health Center, Beer Sheva, Israel Faculty of Health Sciences, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beer Sheva, Israel
Yael Zinker
Affiliation:
Faculty of Health Sciences, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beer Sheva, Israel
Esther Argov
Affiliation:
The Zinman College of Physical Education and Sport Sciences, Wingate Institute, Israel
Ruth Agmon
Affiliation:
The Zinman College of Physical Education and Sport Sciences, Wingate Institute, Israel
*
Correspondence should be addressed to: Yael Netz, PhD, School of Physical Education, The Zinman College of Physical Education and Sport Sciences, Wingate Institute 42902, Israel. Phone: +972-9-8639-362; Fax: +972-9-8650-960. Email: neyael@wincol.ac.il.

Abstract

Background: Studies generally describe the relationship between physical fitness and cognitive function by measuring only one or two specific cognitive tasks. In addition, in spite of the significant increase in life expectancy, the age of participants in these studies does not extend beyond a mean age of 70 years. This study was thus designed to examine the relationship between physical fitness and function in multiple cognitive domains in subjects older than those previously reported.

Methods: Thirty-eight individuals, aged 65.3 to 85.3 years, performed a graded, progressive, maximal exercise test. Based on a median score of peak VO2, participants were divided into low-fitness and moderately-fit groups. Cognitive function was assessed by means of a computerized neuropsychological battery.

Results: The moderately-fit group achieved significantly better scores on the global cognitive score (U = 97, p = 0.04), and a significant correlation was found between peak VO2 and attention, executive function, and global cognitive score (rs = .37, .39, .38 respectively). The trend for superior cognitive scores in the moderate-fitness compared to the low-fitness groups was unequivocal, both in terms of accuracy and reaction time.

Conclusion: Maintenance of higher levels of cardiovascular fitness may help protect against cognitive deterioration, even at an advanced age. An adequately powered randomized controlled trial should be performed to further evaluate this hypothesis.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © International Psychogeriatric Association 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

American College of Sports Medicine (ACSM) (2006a). American College of Sports Medicine's Guidelines for Exercise Testing and Prescription. Baltimore: Lippincott Williams and Wilkins.Google Scholar
American College of Sports Medicine (2006b). American College of Sports Medicine's Resource Manual for Guidelines for Exercise Testing and Prescription. Baltimore: Lippincott Williams and Wilkins.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edn.Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Angevaren, M., Aufdemkampe, G., Verhaar, H. J. J., Aleman, A. and Vanhees, I. (2008). Physical activity and enhanced fitness to improve cognitive function in older people without known cognitive impairment (Review). The Cochrane Library, 3, 196.Google Scholar
Baecke, J. A. H., Burema, J. and Frijters, J. E. R. (1982). A short questionnaire for the measurement of a habitual physical activity in epidemiological studies. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 36, 936942.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barnes, D. E., Yaffe, K., Satariano, W. A. and Tager, I. B. (2003). A longitudinal study of cardiorespiratory fitness and cognitive function in healthy older adults. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 51, 459465.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borg, G. (1986). Psychophysical studies of effort and exertion: some historical theoretical and empirical aspects. In Borg, G. and Ottoson, D. (eds.), The Perception of Exertion in Physical Work (pp. 312). London: Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brink, T. L., Yesavage, J. A., Lum, O., Heersema, P., Adey, M. B. and Rose, T. L. (1982). Screening tests for geriatric depression. Clinical Gerontologist, 1, 3744.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cauraugh, J. H. (1990). Speed-accuracy tradeoff during response preparation. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 61, 331337.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colcombe, S. J. and Kramer, A. F. (2003). Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults: a meta-analytic study. Psychological Science, 14, 125130.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colcombe, S. J. et al. (2003). Aerobic fitness reduces brain tissue loss in aging humans. Journals of Gerontology: Medical Sciences, 58A, 176180.Google Scholar
Colcombe, S. J. et al. (2004). Cardiovascular fitness, cortical plasticity, and aging. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA, 101, 33163321.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Der, G., and Deary, I. J. (2006). Age and sex differences in reaction time in adulthood: results from the United Kingdom Health and Lifestyle Survey. Psychology and Aging, 21, 6273.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dwolatzky, T. et al. (2003). Validity of a novel computerized cognitive battery for mild cognitive impairment. BMC Geriatrics, 3, 416.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Etnier, J. L. and Landers, D. M. (1997). The influence of age and fitness on performance and learning. Journal of Aging and Physical Activity, 5, 175189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Etnier, J. L., Sibley, B. A., Pomeroy, J. and Kao, J. C. (2003). Components of response time as a function of age, physical activity, and aerobic fitness. Journal of Aging and Physical Activity, 11, 319332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Folstein, M. E., Folstein, S. E. and McHugh, P. R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Folstein, M. E., Anthony, J. C., Parhad, I., Duffy, B. and Gruenberg, E. M. (1985). The meaning of cognitive impairment in the elderly. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 33, 228235.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hillman, C. H. et al. (2006). Physical activity and cognitive function in a cross-section of younger and older community-dwelling individuals. Health Psychology, 25, 678687.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lawton, M. P. and Brody, E. M. (1969). Assessment of older people: self-maintaining and instrumental activities of daily living. Gerontologist, 9, 179–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
National Center for Health Statistics (2004). Health, United States, 2004, with Chartbook on Trends in the Health of Americans. Hyattville, MD: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention.Google Scholar
Neeper, S., Gomez-Pinilla, F., Choi, J. and Cottman, C. (1995). Exercise and brain neurotrophins. Nature, 373, 109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Netz, Y., Argov, E. and Inbar, O. (2009). Fitness's moderation of the facilitative effect of acute exercise on cognitive flexibility in older women. Journal of Aging and Physical Activity, 17, 154166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pols, M. A., Peeters, P. H., Kemper, H. C. and Collette, H. J. (1996). Repeatability and relative validity of two physical activity questionnaires in elderly women. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 28, 10201025.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogers, R. L., Meyer, J. S. and Mortel, K. F. (1990). After reaching retirement age physical activity sustains cerebral perfusion and cognition. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 38, 123128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schaie, K. W. (2004). Cognitive aging. In Pew, R. W. and Can Hemel, S. B. (eds.), Technology for Adaptive Aging (pp. 4363). Washington, DC: National Research Council.Google Scholar
Schweiger, A., Doniger, G. M., Dwolatzky, T., Jaffe, D. and Simon, E.S. (2003). Reliability of a novel computerized neuropsychological battery for mild cognitive impairment. Acta Neuropsychologica, 1, 407413.Google Scholar
Spreen, O. and Strauss, E. (1998). A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration, Norms, and Commentary. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Themanson, J. R., Hillman, C. H. and Curtin, J. J. (2006). Age and physical activity influence on action monitoring during task switching. Neurobiology of Aging, 27, 13351345.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weuve, J., Kang, J. H., Manson, J. E., Breteler, M. M., Ware, J. H. and Grodstein, F. (2004). Physical activity, including walking, and cognitive function in older women. JAMA, 292, 14541461.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yaffe, K., Barnes, D., Nevitt, M., Lui, L. Y. and Covinsky, K. (2001). A prospective study of physical activity and cognitive decline in elderly women. Archives of Internal Medicine, 161, 17031708.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed