Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rdxmf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-29T06:44:41.968Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Children choose their own stories: the impact of choice on children's learning of new narrative skills*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 June 2013

KIREN KHAN
Affiliation:
The Pennsylvania State University, USA
KEITH NELSON
Affiliation:
The Pennsylvania State University, USA
ELISABETH WHYTE
Affiliation:
The Pennsylvania State University, USA

Abstract

Despite evidence that early narrative abilities are predictive of literacy skills and academic achievement, only limited progress has been made in understanding how the development of these narrative skills can be facilitated. The current study measured the effectiveness of a new narrative intervention conducted with 26 preschoolers. Children were assigned to one of two intervention conditions: an active-choice condition (able to choose story components) or a no-choice condition (story components were preselected). Both groups otherwise received the same explicit and engaging teaching of story grammar. As predicted, greater narrative skill gains were evident for the active-choice intervention; including larger gains on both central story grammar components and story information overall. Future implications for how stories might be presented to young children in order to more richly facilitate narrative skill acquisition are discussed.

Type
Brief Research Reports
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

Address for correspondence: Kiren Khan, The Pennsylvania State University – Psychology, Graduate Student Mailroom, Moore Building, University Park, PA 16802, United States.

References

REFERENCES

Bowyer-Crane, C., Snowling, M. J., Duff, F. J., Fieldsend, E., Carroll, J. M., Miles, J. N. V. & Hulme, C. (2008). Improving early language and literacy skills: differential effects of an oral language versus a phonology with reading intervention. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 49, 422–32.Google Scholar
Cordova, D. I. & Lepper, M. R. (1996). Intrinsic motivation and the process of learning: beneficial effects of contextualization, personalization, and choice. Journal of Educational Psychology 88, 715–30.Google Scholar
Fazio, B. B., Naremore, R. C. & Connell, P. J. (1996). Tracking children from poverty at risk for specific language impairment: a 3-year longitudinal study. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 39(3), 611–24.Google Scholar
Feagans, L. V. (1982). The development and importance of narratives for school adaptation. In Feagans, L. & Farran, D. C. (eds.), The language of children reared in poverty, 95116. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Feagans, L. V. & Appelbaum, M. J. (1986). Validation of language subtypes in learning disabled children. Journal of Educational Psychology 78(5), 358–64.Google Scholar
Feagans, L. V. & Farran, D. C. (1994). The effect of daycare intervention in the preschool years on the narrative skills of poverty children in kindergarten. International Journal of Behavioral Development 17(3), 503–23.Google Scholar
Fiorentino, L. & Howe, N. (2004). Language competence, narrative ability, and school readiness in low-income preschool children. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science 36(4), 280–95.Google Scholar
Gardner, M. F. (1990). Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised. Novato, CA: Academic Therapy Publications.Google Scholar
Gillam, R., McFadden, T. U. & Kleeck, A. V. (1995). Improving narrative abilities: whole language and language skills approaches. In Fay, M. E., Windsor, J. & Warren, S. F. (eds.), Language intervention: preschool through the elementary years, vol. 5, 145–82. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Glasgow, G. & Cowley, J. (1994). The Renfrew bus story-American edition. Unpublished manuscript, Centreville School, Centreville, DE.Google Scholar
Hayley, K., Camarata, S. & Nelson, K. (1994). Social valence in children with specific language impairment during imitation-based and conversation-based language intervention. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 37, 378–88.Google Scholar
Hayward, D. & Schneider, P. (2000). Effectiveness of teaching story grammar knowledge to pre-school children with language impairment: an exploratory study. Child Language Teaching and Therapy 16(3), 255–84.Google Scholar
Hudson, J. & Nelson, K. (1983). Effects of script structure on children's story recall. Developmental Psychology 19(4), 625–35.Google Scholar
Johnson, N. S. & Mandler, J. M. (1980). A tale of two structures: underlying and surface forms in stories. Poetics 9, 5186.Google Scholar
Mandler, J. M. (1978). A code in the node: the use of a story schema in retrieval. Discourse Processes 1, 1435.Google Scholar
Mandler, J. M. & Johnson, N. S. (1977). Remembrance of things parsed: story structure and recall. Cognitive Psychology 9(1), 111–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayer, M. (1967). A boy, a dog, and a frog. New York: Dial Press.Google Scholar
Mayer, M. (1975). One frog too many. New York: Dial Press.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. E. & Arkenberg, M. E. (2008). Language and reading development reflect dynamic mixes of learning conditions. In Moody, M. & Silliman, E. R. (eds.), Brain, behavior, and learning in language and reading disorders, 315–48. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. E., Craven, P. L., Xuan, Y. & Arkenberg, M. E. (2004). Acquiring art, spoken language, sign language, and text: developmental and evolutionary observations from a Dynamic Tricky Mix theoretical perspective. In Hudson, J., Fivush, R., Bauer, P. & Lucariello, M. (eds.), The development of the mediated mind: sociocultural context and cognitive development, 175222. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. E., Heimann, M. & Tjus, T. (1997). Theoretical and applied insights from multimedia facilitation of communication skills in autistic, deaf, motorically disabled, and children with other disabilities. In Adamson, L. & Romski, M. A. (eds.), Communication and language acquisition: discoveries from atypical development, 295325. Baltimore: Brookes.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. E., Prinz, P. M., Prinz, E. A. & Dalke, D. (1991). Processes for text and language acquisition in the context of microcomputer videodisc instruction for deaf and multi handicapped children. In Martin, D. S. (ed.), Cognition, education and deafness, vol. 3, 162–69. Washington, DC: Gallaudet College Press.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. E., Welsh, J., Camarata, S., Heimann, M. & Tjus, T. (2001). A rare event transactional dynamic model of tricky mix conditions contributing to language acquisition and varied communicative delays. In Nelson, K. E., Aksu-Koc, A. & Johnson, C. E. (eds.), Children's language, vol. 11, 165–95. Mahwah, NJ: Laurence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Paris, A. H. & Paris, S. G. (2001). Children's comprehension of narrative picture books: CIERA report. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan, Center for the Improvement of Early Reading Achievement.Google Scholar
Paris, A. H. & Paris, S. G. (2003). Assessing narrative comprehension in young children. Reading Research Quarterly 38, 3676.Google Scholar
Patall, E. A., Cooper, H. & Robinson, J. C. (2008). The effects of choice on intrinsic motivation and related outcomes: a meta-analysis of research findings. Psychological Bulletin 134, 270300.Google Scholar
Petersen, D. B. (2011). A systematic review of narrative-based language intervention with children who have language impairment. Communication Disorders Quarterly 32, 207–20.Google Scholar
Peterson, C., Jesson, B. & McCabe, A. (1999). Encouraging narratives in preschoolers: an intervention study. Journal of Child Language 26(1), 4969.Google Scholar
Peterson, C. & McCabe, A. (1983). Developmental psycholinguistics: three ways of looking at a child's narrative. New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peterson, C. & McCabe, A. (1994). A social interactionist account of developing decontextualized narrative skill. Developmental Psychology 30(2), 937–48.Google Scholar
Snow, C. E. (1983). Literacy and language: relationships during the preschool years. Harvard Education Review 53(2), 165–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stein, N. L. (1988). The development of storytelling skill. In Franklin, M. B. & Barten, S. (eds.), Child language: a book of readings, 282–97. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Stein, N. L. & Albro, E. R. (1997). Building complexity and coherence: children's use of goal-structured knowledge in telling stories. In Bamberg, M. (ed.), Narrative development: six approaches, 544. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Stein, N. L. & Glenn, C. (1979). An analysis of story comprehension in elementary school children. In Freedle, R. (ed.), New directions in discourse processing, 53120. Hilldale, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Tabors, P. O., Snow, C. E. & Dickinson, D. K. (2001). Homes and schools together: supporting language and literacy development. In Tabors, P. O. & Dickinson, D. K. (ed.), Beginning literacy with language: young children learning at home and school, 313–34. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing.Google Scholar
Thelen, E. & Smith, L. B. (1994). A dynamics systems approach to the development of cognition and action. Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books/MIT Press.Google Scholar
Trabasso, T. & Nickels, M. (1992). The development of goal plans of action in the narration of a picture story. Discourse Processes 15, 249–75.Google Scholar
van den Broek, P., Lorch, E. P. & Thurlow, R. (1996). Children's and adult's memory for television stories: the role of causal factors, story-grammar categories, and hierarchical level. Child Development 67, 3010–28.Google Scholar
Wiig, E. H., Secord, W. & Semel, E. M. (1992). Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals – Preschool. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Zevenbergen, A. A. & Whitehurst, G. J. (2003). Dialogic reading: a shared picture book reading intervention for preschoolers. In van Kleek, A., Stahl, S. A. and Bauer, E. B. (eds.), On reading books to children: parents and teachers, 177200. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar