Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-lnqnp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T05:08:38.501Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Emotional state talk and emotion understanding: a training study with preschool children*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 March 2011

ILARIA GRAZZANI*
Affiliation:
Università degli Studi di Milano-Bicocca, Faculty of Educational Sciences, Department of Human Sciences ‘R. Massa’, Milan, Italy
VERONICA ORNAGHI
Affiliation:
Università degli Studi di Milano-Bicocca, Faculty of Educational Sciences, Department of Human Sciences ‘R. Massa’, Milan, Italy
*
Address for correspondence: Prof Ilaria Grazzani, Università degli Studi di Milano Bicocca, Piazza Ateneo Nuovo 1, 20126, Building U6, 4th floor, Milan, Italy. e-mail: ilaria.grazzani@unimib.it

Abstract

The present study investigates whether training preschool children in the active use of emotional state talk plays a significant role in bringing about greater understanding of emotion terms and improved emotion comprehension. Participants were 100 preschool children (M=52 months; SD=9·9; range: 35–70 months), randomly assigned to experimental or control conditions. They were pre- and post-tested to assess their language comprehension, metacognitive language comprehension and emotion understanding. Analyses of pre-test data did not show any significant differences between experimental and control groups. During the intervention phase, the children were read stories enriched with emotional lexicon. After listening to the stories, children in the experimental group took part in conversational language games designed to stimulate use of the selected emotional terms. In contrast, the control group children did not take part in any special linguistic activities after the story readings. Analyses revealed that the experimental group outperformed the control group in the understanding of inner state language and in the comprehension of emotion.

Type
Brief Research Reports
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

This study was part-funded by a doctoral research grant from Università degli Studi di Milano Bicocca, Milan, Italy. We wish to thank all children who participated in the study, Dr Carlo di Chiacchio for his help with the statistical analysis, and Dr Clare O'Sullivan for the linguistic revision of the article. Our particular thanks also go to Professor Keith Oatley, who read the first draft of the article and gave us valuable feedback. Finally, we are also most grateful to the anonymous referees for encouraging us to interpret our findings more carefully and critically.

References

REFERENCES

Adriàn, J. E., Clemente, R. A., Villanueva, L. & Rieffe, C. (2005). Parent–child picture-book reading, mothers' mental state language, and children's theory of mind. Journal of Child Language 32, 673–.86CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Albanese, O. & Molina, P. (eds) (2008). Lo sviluppo della comprensione delle emozioni e la sua valutazione. La standardizzazione italiana del Test di Comprensione delle Emozioni (TEC) [The development of emotion comprehension. The Italian standardization of TEC]. Milano: Unicopli.Google Scholar
Astington, J.W. & Baird, J. A. (eds) (2005). Why language matters for theory of mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Astington, J. W. & Jenkins, J. M. (1999). A longitudinal study of the relation between language and theory of mind development. Developmental Psychology 35, 1311–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Astington, J. W. & Olson, D. R. (1990). Metacognitive and metalinguistic language: Learning to talk about thoughts. Applied Psychology 39, 7787.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Astington, J. W. & Pelletier, J. (2005). Theory of mind, language and learning in the early years: Development origins of school readiness. In Homer, B. D. & Tamis-Le Monda, C. S. (eds), The development of social cognition and communication, 205230. Mahawah, NJ: LEA.Google Scholar
Bartsch, K. & Wellman, H. M. (1995). Children talk about the mind. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bretherton, I. & Beegley, M. (1982). Talking about internal states: The acquisition of an explicit theory of mind. Developmental Psychology 18(6), 906921.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brockmeier, J. & Grazzani Gavazzi, I. (1999). Words, names, and intentional worlds. Redefining Literacy. The Language, Literacy, and the Mind Research Group of the Centre for Applied Cognitive Science, University of Toronoto/OISE, Fall <http//:lsn.oise.utoronto.ca/RedefiningLiteracy>..>Google Scholar
Cervantes, C. A. & Callanan, M. A. (1998). Labels and explanations in mother–child emotion talk: Age and gender differentiation. Developmental Psychology 34, 8898.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cianchetti, C. & Sannio Fancello, G. (1997). TVL, Test di Valutazione del Linguaggio. Livello Prescolare [Test of Language Assessment. Preschool Level]. Trento: Erickson.Google Scholar
Ciceri, M. R. (2001). Comunicare il pensiero. Trento: Edizioni Omega.Google Scholar
Denham, S. A., Blair, K. A., DeMulder, E., Levitas, J., Sawyer, K., Auerbach-Major, S. & Queenan, P. (2003). Preschool emotional competence: Pathway to social competence? Child Development 74(1), 238–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Rosnay, M. & Hughes, C. (2006). Conversation and theory of mind: Do children talk their way to social-cognitive understanding? British Journal of Developmental Psychology 24, 7–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Rosnay, M., Pons, F. & Harris, P. L. (2008). Prolegomenon to the Italian standardization of the TEC. In Albanese, O. & Molina, P. (eds), The development of emotion comprehension. The Italian standardization of TEC, 917. Milano: Unicopli.Google Scholar
de Villiers, J. & de Villiers, P. (2000). Linguistic determinism and the understanding of false beliefs. In Mitchell, P. & Riggs, K. J. (eds), Children's reasoning and the mind, 191228. Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Dunn, J., Brown, J. & Beardsale, L. (1991). Family talk about feeling states and children's later understanding of others' emotions. Developmental Psychology 27, 448–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garfield, J. L., Peterson, C. C. & Perry, T. (2001). Social cognition, language acquisition and the development of the theory of mind. Mind and Language 16, 494–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garner, P. W., Jones, D. C., Gaddy, G. & Rennie, K. M. (1997). Low-income mothers' conversations about emotions and their children's emotional competence. Social Development 6, 3752.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grazzani, I. & Ornaghi, V. (2002). Children's conceptions of name: A study on metalinguistic awareness in Italian children. In Brockmeier, J., Wang, M. & Olson, D. R. (eds), Literacy, narrative and culture, 199214. Richmond: Curzon Press.Google Scholar
Grazzani, I. & Ornaghi, V. (2008). Psychological talk and theory of mind: The role of language games. In Arcuri, A., Boscolo, P. & Peressotti, F. (eds), Cognition and language, 278–83. Padova: Cleup.Google Scholar
Hale, C. M. & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2003). The influence of language on theory of mind: A training study. Developmental Science 6, 346–59.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, P. L. (1989). Children and emotion: The development of psychological understanding. Oxford: Balckwell.Google Scholar
Harris, P. L. (2008). Children's understanding of emotion. In Lewis, M., Haviland-Jones, J. M. & Feldman Barrett, L. (eds), Handbook of emotion, 3rd edn.320–31. New York: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Hughes, C., Lecce, S. & Wilson, A. (2007). Do you know what I want? Preschoolers' talk about desires, thoughts and feelings in their conversations with sibs and friends. Cognition & Emotion 21, 330–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hutto, D. D. (2007). The narrative practice hypothesis. In Hutto, D. D. (ed.), Narrative and understanding persons, 4368. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iannello, P. & Antonietti, A. (eds) (2006). Rappresentazioni della mente [Representations of the mind]. Milano: ISU Publications.Google Scholar
Jenkins, J. M., Turrel, S. L., Kogushi, Y., Lollis, S. & Ross, H. S. (2003). A longitudinal investigation of the dynamics of mental state talk in families. Child Development 74, 905920.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuebli, J. & Fivush, R. (1992). Gender differences in parent–child-conversations about past emotions. Sex Roles 27, 683–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lohmann, H. & Tomasello, M. (2003). The role of language in the development of false belief understanding: A training study. Child Development 74, 1130–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milligan, K., Astington, J. W. & Dack, L. A. (2007). Language and theory of mind: Meta-analysis of the relation between language ability and false-belief understanding. Child Development 78(2), 622–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Montgomery, D. E. (2005). The developmental origins of meaning for mental states. In Astington, J. W. & Baird, J. A. (eds), Why language matters for theory of mind, 106122. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K. (1996). Language in cognitive development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K. (2007). Young minds in social worlds. Experience, meaning, and memory. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ornaghi, V., Brockmeier, J. & Grazzani Gavazzi, I. (2011). The role of language games in children's understanding of mental states: A training study. Journal of Cognition and Development.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pelletier, J. & Astington, J. W. (1998). Metacognitive vocabulary test. Unpublished ms., Institute for Child Studies, University of Toronto.Google Scholar
Peskin, J. & Astington, J. W. (2004). The effects of adding metacognive language to story texts. Cognitive Development 19, 253–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pons, F. & Harris, P. L. (2000). TEC, Test of Emotion Comprehension. Oxford: Oxford University.Google Scholar
Pons, F. & Harris, P. L. (2005). Longitudinal change and longitudinal stability of individual differences in children's emotion understanding. Cognition and Emotion 19(8), 1158–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pons, F., Harris, P. L. & de Rosnay, M. (2004). Emotion comprehension between 3 and 11 years: Developmental periods and hierarchical organization. European Journal of Developmental Psychology 1(2), 127–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rumelhart, D. E. (1975). Notes on a schema for stories. In Borrow, D. G. & Collins, A. (eds), Representation and understanding. Studies in cognitive science, 211–36. New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saarni, C. (1999). The development of emotional competence. New York: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Saarni, C. & Harris, P. L. (eds) (1989). Children's understanding of emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Siegal, M. (1999). Language and thought: The fundamental significance of conversational awareness for cognitive development. Developmental Science 2, 114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siegal, M. (2008). Marvellous minds. The discovery of what children know. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Slade, L. & Ruffman, T. (2005). How language does (and does not) relate to theory-of-mind: A longitudinal study of syntax, semantics, working memory and false belief. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 23, 117–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tenenbaum, H. R., Alfieri, L., Brooks, P. J. & Dunne, G. (2008). The effects of explanatory conversations on children's emotion understanding. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 26, 249–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tenenbaum, H. R., Visscher, P., Pons, F. & Harris, P. L. (2004). Emotional understanding in Quechua children from an agro-pastoralist village. International Journal of Behavioral Development 28(5), 471–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wittgenstein, L. (1953). Philosophical investigations. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar