We use cookies to distinguish you from other users and to provide you with a better experience on our websites. Close this message to accept cookies or find out how to manage your cookie settings.
An abstract is not available for this content so a preview has been provided. Please use the Get access link above for information on how to access this content.
Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)
References
REFERENCES
Akhtar, N. & Tomasello, M. (2000). The social nature of words and word learning. In Golinkoff, R. M. & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (eds) Becoming a word learner: A debate on lexical acquisition, 115–135. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCune, L. (1995). A normative study of representational play at the transition to language. Developmental Psychology31, 198–206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCune-Nicolich, L. (1981). Toward symbolic functioning: Structure of early pretend games and potential parallels with language. Child Development52, 785–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1962). Play, dreams and imitation. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Quine, W. V. O. (1960). Word and object. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Thelen, E. & Smith, L. B. (1994). A dynamic systems approach to the development of cognition and action. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Werner, H. & Kaplan, B. (1948/1963). Symbol formation. New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar