Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-s2hrs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-04T06:11:47.607Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Quality of adult vocalizations affects the quality of infant vocalizations*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 February 2009

Kathleen Bloom*
Affiliation:
University of Waterloo
*
Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario, CanadaN2L 3G1

Abstract

The purpose of the present study was to determine whether the VERBAL component of babytalk affected the vocal quality of infants. Two groups of 20 0;3 infants experienced either conversational turn taking or random responsiveness of an adult. Adult responsiveness consisted of a smile, light touch to the abdomen, and a NONVERBAL ‘tsk, tsk, tsk’ sound (instead of speech). All infant vocalizations were counted and then coded as ‘syllabic’ or ‘vocalic’ sounds. The results of the study demonstrated that TURN TAKING facilitated a speak-listen pattern of vocalizing by the infant, but in the absence of the VERBAL component of the adult's response, turn taking had no special facilitative effect on syllabic sounds. It was concluded that what adults say to 3-month-old infants influences what infants ‘say’ in return. Possible mechanisms underlying the infant's vocal response to verbal input were reviewed. It was suggested that the more mature sounds (syllabic) may proportionally increase as a result of the combined effects of turn-taking and verbal stimulation on the motor control of respiration and phonation.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1988

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

Appreciation is expressed to Catherine Snow for suggesting the study, to Laura D'Odorico and Gabriele di Stefano for their comments concerning the manuscript, and particularly to Lois Bloom for her generous and invaluable conceptual contributions to all phases of the research programme. Susan Davis, Elsa Lo, and Ann Russell provided technical assistance. Special thanks are extended to the parents and infants who participated in this study and to the staff of the Grace Maternity Hospital, Halifax, Nova Scotia, who helped with the recruitment of participants. This research was made possible by a research grant (410-85-0227) and a Leave Fellowship from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada, and with the kind sabbatical support of the Dipartimento di Psicologia dello Sviluppo e della Socializzazione, Universita degli Studi di Padova, where the manuscript was written.

References

REFERENCES

Abravanel, E. & Sigafoos, A. D. (1984). Exploring the presence of imitation during early infancy. Child Development 55. 381–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beebe, B., Jaffe, J., Feldstein, S., Mays, K. & Alson, D. (1985). Interpersonal timing: the application of an adult dialogue model to mother-infant vocal and kinesic interactions. In Field, T. M. & Fox, N. A. (eds), Social perceptions in infants. Norwood NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Bloom, K. (1974). Eye contact as a setting event for infant learning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 17. 250–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bloom, K. (1975) Social elicitation of infant vocal behaviour. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 20. 51–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloom, K. (1977 a). Patterning of infant vocal behaviour. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 23. 367–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloom, K. (1977 b). Operant baseline procedures suppress infant social behavior. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 23. 128–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bloom, K. (1979). Evaluation of infant vocal conditioning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 27. 6070.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bloom, K. (1984). Distinguishing between social reinforcement and social elicitation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 38. 93102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloom, K. & Esposito, A. (1975). Social conditioning and its proper control procedures. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 19. 209–22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bloom, K., Russell, A. & Wassenberg, K. (1987). Turn taking affects the quality of infant vocalizations. Journal of Child Language 14. 211–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crystal, D. (1973). Non-segmental phonology in language acquisition: a review of the issues. Lingua 32. 145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
D'Odorico, L. (1984). Non-segmental features in prelinguistic communications: an analysis of some types of infant cry and non-cry vocalizations. Journal of Child Language 11. 1727.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferguson, C. A. (1964). Baby talk in six languages. American Anthropologist 66. 103–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fernald, C. A. (1984). The perceptual and affective salience of mother's speech to infants. In Feagans, L., Garvey, C., Golinkoff, R., Greenberg, M. T., Harding, C. & Bohannon, J. N. (eds), The origins and growth of communication. Norwood NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Fernald, C. A. (1985). Four-month-old infants prefer to listen to motherese. Infant Behaviour and Development 8. 181–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garnica, O. K. (1977). Some prosodic and paralinguistic features of speech to young children. In Snow, C. E. & Ferguson, C. A. (eds), Talking to children: language input and acquisition. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Jasnow, M. & Feldstein, F. (1986). Adult-like temporal characteristics of mother-infant vocal interaction. Child Development 57. 754–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kent, R. D. (1981). Articulatory-acoustic perspectives on speech development. In Stark, R. E. (ed.), Language behaviour in infancy and early childhood. Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Kent, R. D. & Murray, A. D. (1982). Acoustic features of infant vocalic utterances at 3, 6, and 9 months. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 72 (2). 353–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kühl, P. K. (1985). Categorization of speech by infants. In Mehler, J. & Fox, R. (eds), Neonate cognition: beyond the blooming buzzing confusion. Hillsdale N.J: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Laufer, M. Z. & Horii, H. (1977). Fundamental frequency characteristics of non-distress vocalizations during the first twenty-four weeks. Journal of Child Language 4. 171–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McKenzie, B. & Over, R. (1983). Young infants fail to imitate facial and manual gestures. Infant Behaviour and Development 6. 8595.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meltzoff, A. (1985). The roots of social and cognitive development: models of man's original nature. In Field, T. & Fox, N. (eds), Social perception in infants. Norwood NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Netsell, R. (1981). The acquisition of speech motor control: a perspective with directions for research. In Stark, R. E. (ed.), Language behaviour in infancy and early childhood. Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Oller, D. K. (1980). The emergence of the sounds of speech in early infancy. In Yeni-Komshian, G. H., Kavanagh, J. F. & Ferguson, C. A. (eds), Child phonology. Vol. 1. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Oller, D. K. (1981). Infant vocalizations: exploration and reflexivity. In Stark, R. E. (ed.), Language behaviour in infancy and early childhood. Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1951). Play, dreams and imitation in childhood. New York: W. W. Norton.Google Scholar
Proctor, A. (in press). Noncry vocal development in normal and at-risk infants: a review of the first 12 months. In Marfo, K. (ed.), Parent—child interactions and developmental disability. New York: Praeger Publishers.Google Scholar
Rheingold, H. L., Gewirtz, J. L. & Ross, B. G. (1959). Social conditioning of infant vocalizations in the infant. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 52. 6873.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snow, C. E. (1986). Conversations with children. In Fletcher, P. & Garman, M. (eds), Language acquisition. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Snow, C. E. & Ferguson, C. A. (eds) (1977). Talking to children: language input and acquisition. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Stark, R. E. (1978). Features of infant sounds: the emergence of cooing. Journal of Child Language 5. 379–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stark, R. E. (ed.) (1981). Language behaviour in infancy and early childhood. Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Stark, R. E. (1986). Prespeech segmental feature development. In Fletcher, P. & Garman, M. (eds), Language acquisition. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Sullivan, J. W. & Horowitz, F. D. (1983). The effects of intonation on infant attention: the role of the rising intonation contour. Journal of Child Language 10. 521–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomkins, S. (1962). Affect, imagery, consciousness. Vol. 1. The positive affect. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Tonkova-Yampol'skaya, R. V. (1973). Development of speech intonation in infants during the first two years of life. In Ferguson, C. A. & Slobin, D. I. (eds), Studies of child language development. Toronto: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Valentine, C. W. (1930). The psychology of imitation with special reference to early childhood. Journal of Psychology 21. 105–32.Google Scholar
Vinter, A. (1986). The role of movement in eliciting early imitation. Child Development 57. 6671.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weisberg, P. (1963). Social and nonsocial conditioning of infant vocalizations. Child Development 34. 377–88.Google ScholarPubMed