Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-4rdpn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-03T08:29:01.205Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The role of maternal input in the development of wh-question comprehension in autism and typical development*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  24 January 2014

ANTHONY GOODWIN*
Affiliation:
University of Connecticut
DEBORAH FEIN
Affiliation:
University of Connecticut
LETITIA NAIGLES
Affiliation:
University of Connecticut
*
Address for correspondence: Anthony Goodwin, University of Connecticut – Department of Psychology, 406 Babbidge Road, Unit 1020, Storrs, Connecticut 06269-1020, United States. e-mail: anthony.goodwin@uconn.edu

Abstract

Social deficits have been implicated in the language delays and deficits of children with autism (ASD); thus, the extent to which these children use language input in social contexts similarly to typically developing (TD) children is unknown. The current study investigated how caregiver input influenced the development of wh-question comprehension in TD children and language-matched preschoolers with ASD. Children were visited at four-month intervals over 1.5 years; mother–child play sessions at visits 1–2 were coded for maternal wh-question use. At visits 3–5 children watched videos in the Intermodal Preferential Looking paradigm, to assess their comprehension of subject and object wh-questions. Mothers' use of wh-questions with verbs and complex wh-questions positively predicted wh-question comprehension in the TD group; in contrast, mothers' use of wh-questions with ‘be’ as the main verb negatively predicted wh-question comprehension in the ASD group. Thus, TD children and children with ASD appear to use their linguistic input differently.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

This research was funded by a grant from the National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders (Grant number: R01 DC007428). We are grateful to Rose Jaffery and Janina Piotroski for assistance in stimulus creation and data collection, and to the undergraduates of the UConn Child Language Lab for coding and transcribing. We thank George Hollich for sharing the wh-question IPL video with us, and Wendy Stone for making the STAT available to us. We appreciate the helpful commentary received from Inge-Marie Eigsti, Alice Carter, William Snyder, and attendants at IMFAR, BUCLD, and the SRCD biennial meetings. Finally, many thanks are due to the children and families who participated in our study.

References

REFERENCES

Ambridge, B., Rowland, C. F., Theakston, A. L. & Tomasello, M. (2006). Comparing different accounts of inversion errors in children's non-subject wh-questions: ‘What experimental data can tell us?’ Journal of Child Language 33, 519–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bloom, L., Merkin, S. & Wooten, J. (1982). Wh-questions: Linguistic factors that contribute to the sequence of acquisition. Child Development 53, 1084–92.Google Scholar
Casenhiser, D. & Goldberg, A. E. (2005). Fast mapping between a phrasal form and meaning. Developmental Science 8(6), 500–8.Google Scholar
Charman, T. (1998). Specifying the nature and course of the joint attention impairment in autism in the preschool years: Implications for diagnosis and intervention. Autism: The International Journal of Research and Practice 2, 6179.Google Scholar
Charman, T., Drew, A., Baird, C. & Baird, G. (2003). Measuring early language development in preschool children with autism spectrum disorder using the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory (Infant Form). Journal of Child Language 30, 213–36.Google Scholar
Chlebowski, C., Green, J. A., Barton, M. L. & Fein, D. (2010). Using the Childhood Autism Rating Scale to diagnose autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 40(7), 787–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Villiers, J., Roeper, T. & Vainikka, A. (1990). The acquisition of long-distance rules. In Frazier, L. & de Villiers, J. (eds), Language processing and language acquisition, 257–97. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.Google Scholar
Eigsti, I.-M., Bennetto, L. & Dadlani, M. B. (2007). Beyond pragmatics: Morphosyntactic development in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 37, 1007–23.Google Scholar
Eigsti, I.-M. & Ciccheti, D. (2004). The impact of child maltreatment on expressive syntax at 60 months. Developmental Science 7(1), 88102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fenson, L., Dale, P., Reznick, J., Bates, E., Thal, D. & Pethick, S. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 59, 1424.Google Scholar
Fernald, A., Perfors, A. & Marchman, V. A. (2006). Picking up speed in understanding: Speech processing efficiency and vocabulary growth across the 2nd year. Developmental Psychology 41(1), 98116.Google Scholar
Gertner, Y., Fisher, C. & Eisengart, J. (2006). Learning words and rules: Abstract knowledge of word order in early sentence comprehension. Psychological Science 17, 684–91.Google Scholar
Gleitman, L., Newport, E. & Gleitman, H. (1984). The current status of the motherese hypothesis. Journal of Child Language 11, 4379.Google Scholar
Golinkoff, R., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Cauley, K. & Gordon, L. (1987). The eyes have it: Lexical and syntactic comprehension in a new paradigm. Journal of Child Language 14, 2345.Google Scholar
Goodwin, A., Fein, D. & Naigles, L. R. (2012). Comprehension of wh-questions precedes their production in typical development and autism spectrum disorders. Autism Research 5, 109–23.Google Scholar
Hoff, E. & Naigles, L. (2002). How children use input to acquire a lexicon. Child Development 73(2), 418–33.Google Scholar
Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1985). Some contributions of mothers' speech to their children's syntactic growth. Journal of Child Language 12, 367–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huttenlocher, J., Waterfall, H., Vasilyeva, M., Vevea, J. & Hedges, L. V. (2010). Sources of variability in children's language growth. Cognitive Psychology 61, 343–65.Google Scholar
Klin, A. (1991). Young autistic children's listening preferences in regard to speech: A possible characterization of the symptom of social withdrawal. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 21, 2942.Google Scholar
Kuhl, P. K., Conboy, B. T., Padden, D., Nelson, T. & Pruitt, J. (2005). Early speech perception and later language development: Implications for the “critical period”. Language Learning and Development 1(3/4), 237–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kukona, A. & Tabor, W. (2011). Impulse processing: A dynamical systems model of incremental eye movements in the visual world paradigm. Cognitive Science 35, 1009–51.Google Scholar
Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. & Risi, S. (1999). The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule–Generic. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Service.Google Scholar
Lovaas, O. I. (1981). Teaching developmentally disabled children: The me book. Baltimore: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Lovaas, O. I. (1987). Behavioral treatment and normal educational and intellectual functioning in young autistic children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 55, 39.Google Scholar
Maratsos, M. (1998). The acquisition of grammar. In Kuhn, D. & Siegler, R. S. (eds), Handbook of child psychology, Vol. 2: Cognition, perception, and language, 421–66. New York: John Wiley and Sons.Google Scholar
Matthews, D. & Bannard, C. (2010). Children's production of unfamiliar word sequences is predicted by positional variability and latent classes in a large sample of child-directed speech. Cognitive Science 34, 465–88.Google Scholar
McDuffie, A. & Yoder, P. (2010) Types of parent verbal responsiveness that predict language in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 53, 1026–39.Google Scholar
Mullen, E. M. (1995). Mullen Scales of Early Learning (AGS Edition). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Mundy, P., Sigman, M. & Kasari, C. (1994). Joint attention, developmental level, and symptom presentation in autism. Developmental Psychology 31, 851–65.Google Scholar
Naigles, L. R. & Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1998). Why are some verbs learned before other verbs? Effects of input frequency and structure on children's early verb use. Journal of Child Language 25, 95120.Google Scholar
Naigles, L. R., Kelty, E., Jaffery, R. & Fein, D. (2011). Abstractness and continuity in the syntactic development of young children with autism. Autism Research 5, 422–37.Google Scholar
Newport, E. L., Gleitman, H. & Gleitman, L. R. (1977). Mother, I'd rather do it myself: Some effects and non-effects of maternal speech style. In Snow, C. E. & Ferguson, C. A. (eds), Talking to children: Language input and acquisition, 109149. London: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Rowland, C. F., Pine, J. M., Lieven, E. V. M. & Theakston, A. L. (2003). Determinants of acquisition order in wh questions: Re-evaluating the role of caregiver speech. Journal of Child Language 30(3), 609–35.Google Scholar
Rowland, C. F., Pine, J. M., Lieven, E. V. M. & Theakston, A. L. (2005). The incidence of error in young children's wh-questions. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 48, 384404.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schopler, E., Reichler, R. J. & Renner, B. R. (1988). The Childhood Autism Rating Scale. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
Searle, J. R. (1969). Speech acts: An essay in the philosophy of language. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seidl, A., Hollich, G. & Jusczyk, P. W. (2003). Early understanding of subject and object wh-questions. Infancy 4(3), 423–36.Google Scholar
Shatz, M., Hoff-Ginsberg, E. & MacIver, D. (1989). Induction and the acquisition of English auxiliaries: The effects of differentially enriched input. Journal of Child Language 16, 121–40.Google Scholar
Snyder, W. (2007). Child language: The parametric approach. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sparrow, S. S., Cicchetti, D. V. & Balla, D. A., (2005). Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales: Second Edition (Vineland II), Survey Interview Form/Caregiver Rating Form. Livonia, MN: Pearson Assessments.Google Scholar
Stone, W. L., Coonrod, E. E. & Ousley, O. Y. (2000). Screening Tool for Autism in Two-Year-Olds (STAT): Development and preliminary data. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 30, 607–12.Google Scholar
Stone, W. L. & Yoder, P. J. (2001). Predicting spoken language level in children with autism spectrum disorders. Autism 5, 341–61.Google Scholar
Stromswold, K. (1995). The acquisition of subject and non-subject wh-questions. Language Acquisition 4(1), 548.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sussman, R. S. & Sedivy, J. C. (2003). The time-course of processing syntactic dependencies: Evidence from eye movements. Language and Cognitive Processes 18(2), 143–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swensen, L. D., Kelley, E., Fein, D. & Naigles, L. (2007). Processes of language acquisition in children with autism: Evidence from preferential looking. Child Development 78, 542–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swensen, L. D., Naigles, L. R. & Fein, D. (2007). Does maternal input affect the language of children with autism?. In Caunt-Nulton, H., Kulatilake, S. & Woo, I. (eds), BUCLD 31: Proceedings of the 31st annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, 609–19. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H. (1994). Dissociations in form and function in the acquisition of language by autistic children. In Tager-Flusberg, H. (ed.) Constraints on language acquisition: Studies of atypical children, 175–94. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Publishers.Google Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H. (2006). Defining language phenotypes in autism. Clinical Neuroscience Research 6, 219–24.Google Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H., Calkins, S., Nolin, T., Baumberger, T., Anderson, M. & Chadwick-Dias, A. (1990). A longitudinal study of language acquisition in autistic and Down syndrome children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 20, 121.Google Scholar
Tek, S., Jaffery, G., Fein, D. & Naigles, L. R. (2008). Do children with autism spectrum disorders show a shape bias in word learning? Autism Research 1, 208–22.Google Scholar
Tovar, A., Fein, D. & Naigles, L. (2012). General and specific predictors of understanding Tense/Aspect in young children with ASD. Poster presented at the International Meeting for Autism Research, Toronto, Canada, May.Google Scholar
Tyack, D. & Ingram, D. (1977). Children's production and comprehension of questions. Journal of Child Language 4, 211–24.Google Scholar
Valian, V. (1999). Input and language acquisition. In Ritchie, W. C. & Bhatia, T. K. (eds), Handbook of child language acquisition, 497530. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Valian, V. & Casey, L. (2003). Young children's acquisition of wh-questions: The role of structured input. Journal of Child Language 30, 117–43.Google Scholar
Vasilyeva, M., Huttenlocher, J. & Waterfall, H. (2006). Effects of language intervention on syntactic skill levels of preschoolers. Developmental Psychology 42, 164–74.Google Scholar
Ventola, P. E., Kleinman, J., Pandey, J., Barton, M., Allen, S., … Fein, D. (2006). Agreement among four diagnostic instruments for autism spectrum disorders in toddlers. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 36, 839–47.Google Scholar
Wagner, L., Swensen, L. & Naigles, L. R. (2009). Children's early productivity with verbal morphology. Cognitive Development 24, 223–39.Google Scholar
Warren, S., Gilkerson, J., Richards, J., Oller, D. K., Xu, D., Yapanel, U. & Gray, S. (2009). What automatic vocal analysis reveals about the vocal production and language learning environment of young children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 40, 555569.Google Scholar
Yoder, P. J., Davies, B., Bishop, K. & Munson, L. (1994). The effect of adult topic continuing wh-questions on conversational participation in children with developmental disabilities. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 37(1), 193204.Google Scholar
Yoder, P. J. & Warren, S. F. (2004). Early predictors of language in children with and without Down syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation 109, 285300.Google Scholar