Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-gb8f7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-24T00:38:53.174Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Young children's understanding of ‘in front of’ and ‘behind’ in the placement of objects*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

M. V. Cox
Affiliation:
University of York

Abstract

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Notes and Discussion
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1979

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Clark, E. V. (1971). On the acquisition of before and after. JVLVB 10. 266–75.Google Scholar
Clark, H. H. (1973). Space, time, semantics, and the child. In Moore, T. E. (ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Donaldson, M. & Balfour, G. (1968). Less is more: a study of language comprehension in children. BJPsychol 59, 461–72.Google Scholar
Friedenberg, L. & Olson, G. M. (1977). Children's comprehension of simple descriptions of vertical arrays. ChDev 48. 265–9.Google Scholar
Kuczaj, S. A. & Maratsos, M. P. (1975). On the acquisition of front, back, and side. ChDev 46. 202–10.Google Scholar
Museyibova, T. A. (1961). The development of an understanding of spatial relations and their reflection in the language of children of pre-school age. In Anan'yev, B. G. & Lovov, B. F. (eds), Problemy vospriyatiya prostranstva i prostranstvennykh predskavleniy, Moscow. (Translation: Problems of spatial preception and spatial concepts. NASA, June, 1964.)Google Scholar
Sutcliffe, J. P. (1957). A general method of analysis of frequency data for multiple classification designs. PsychBull 54, 134–7.Google ScholarPubMed