Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t8hqh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T07:24:40.807Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Joint picture-book reading correlates of early oral language skill*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Barbara D. Debaryshe*
Affiliation:
University of North Carolina at Greensboro
*
Address for correspondence: Department of Human Development and Family Studies, University of North Carolina at Greensboro, Greensboro, NC 27412-5001, USA.

Abstract

The purpose of this study was to explore the relation between joint picture-book-reading experiences provided in the home and children's early oral language skills. Subjects were 41 two-year-old children and their mothers. Measures included maternal report of the age at which she began to read to the child, the frequency of home reading sessions, the number of stories read per week, and the frequency of visits by the child to the local library. Measures of language skill used were the child's receptive and expressive scores on the revised Reynell Developmental Language Scales. Multiple regression analyses indicated that picture-book reading exposure was more strongly related to receptive than to expressive language. Age of onset of home reading routines was the most important predictor of oral language skills. Directions of effect, the importance of parental beliefs as determinants of home reading practices, and the possible existence of a threshold level for reading frequency are discussed.

Type
Note
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1993

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

This study was conducted in partial fulfilment of the requirements for the author's doctoral degree and was supported by a Sigma Xi Grant-in-Aid of Research. I would like to extend my thanks to Jim Watson and a set of anonymous reviewers for their comments on an earlier version of this manuscript.

References

REFERENCES

Chaffee, A. C., Cunningham, C. E., Secord, G. & Elbard, H. (1990). Screening effectiveness of the Minnesota Child Development Inventory expressive and receptive language scales: sensitivity, specificity and predictive value. Psychological Assessment 2, 8085.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, M. M. (1975). Young fluent readers: what can they teach us? London: Heinemann.Google Scholar
Coplan, J. (1982). Parental estimate of child's developmental level in a high-risk population. American Journal of Diseases of Children 136, 101–4.Google ScholarPubMed
Dale, P. S., Bates, E., Reznick, J. S. & Morisset, C. (1989). The validity of a parent report instrument of child language at 20 months. Journal of Child Language 16, 239–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeBaryshe, B. D. (1987). Facilitating language development through picture-book reading: evaluation of a home read-aloud package for parents and preschoolers. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. State University of New York at Stony Brook.Google Scholar
DeBaryshe, B. D., Caulfield, M. B., Witty, J. P., Sidden, J., Holt, H. E. & Reich, C. E. (1991). The ecology of young children's home reading environments. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Seattle, WA.Google Scholar
Funk, J. B., Ruppert, E. S. & Jurs, S. (1982). Assessing the predictive validity of developmental milestones. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics 3, 143–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodnow, J. J. (1988). Parent's ideas, actions, and feelings: models and methods from developmental and social psychology. Child Development 59, 286320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hart, H., Box, M. & Jenkins, S. (1978). The value of developmental history. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 20, 442–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1991). Mother–child conversation in different social classes and communicative settings. Child Development 62, 782–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kenny, T. J., Hebel, J. R., Sexton, M. J. & Fox, N. L. (1987). Developmental screening using parent report. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics 8, 811.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ninio, A. (1980). Picture-book reading in mother–infant dyads belonging to two subgroups in Israel. Child Development 51, 587–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ninio, A. (1983). Joint book reading as a multiple vocabulary acquisition device. Developmental Psychology 19, 445–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pelligrini, A. D., Brody, G. H. & Sigel, I. E. (1985). Parents' book-reading habits with their children. Journal of Educational Psychology 77, 332–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reynell, J. K. (1985). Reynell Developmental Language Scales. Windsor, Berkshire: NFER-Nelson.Google Scholar
Share, D. L., Jorm, A. F., Maclean, R. & Waterman, B. (1983). Early reading achievement, oral language ability, and a child's home background. Australian Psychologist 18, 7587.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sigel, I. E. (1985). Parental belief systems: The psychological consequences for children. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Smith, R. D. (1978). The use of developmental screening tests by primary-care pediatricians. The Journal of Pediatrics 93, 524–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snow, C. E., Arlman-Rupp, A., Hassing, Y., Jobse, J., Joosten, J. & Vorster, J. (1976). Mother's speech in three social classes. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 5, 120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snow, C. E. & Goldfield, B. A. (1983). Turn the page please: situation-specific language acquisition. Journal of Child Language 10, 551–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tabachnick, B. G. & Fidell, L. S. (1983). Using multivariate statistics. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Wells, G. (1985). Preschool literacy-related activities and success in school. In Olson, D. R., Torrence, N., and Hildyard, A. (eds), Literacy, language and learning: the consequences of reading and writing. New York: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Wheeler, P. (1983). Context-related age characteristics in mother's speech: joint book reading. Journal of Child Language 10, 259–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar