Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-g8jcs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-20T23:46:46.713Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Talking to children: the effects of rate, intonation, and length on children's sentence imitation*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

John D. Bonvillian
Affiliation:
Vassar College
Vicki P. Raeburn
Affiliation:
Vassar College
Elizabeth A. Horan
Affiliation:
Vassar College

Abstract

Twelve nursery school children (mean age = 3; 9) attempted to imitate sentences which varied systematically in rate of presentation, intonation and length. The children more successfully imitated shorter sentences than longer ones, and sentences spoken at a rate nearer their own than sentences spoken at slower or faster rates. The imitation of long sentences read with normal intonation was superior to the imitation of long sentences read with flat intonation. Since adults frequently address children in short sentences, spoken slowly and with exaggerated intonation, these results indicate that these speech modifications may enhance the children's language comprehension.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1979

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

The authors wish to thank Bella M. DePaulo and Keith E. Nelson for their comments on an earlier draft of this paper. Order of the first two authors is arbitrary. Address for correspondence: John D. Bonvillian, Department of Psychology, Gilmer Hall, University of Virginia, Charlottesville, Virginia 22901, U.S.A.

References

REFERENCES

Gleason, J. Berko (1973). Code switching in children's language. In Moore, T. E. (ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Blount, B. G. (1972). Parental speech and language: some Luo and Samoan examples. AnthLing 14. 119–30.Google Scholar
Bohannon, J. N. III & Marquis, A. L. (1977). Children's control of adult speech. ChDev 48. 1002–8.Google Scholar
Broen, P. (1972). The verbal environment of the language learning child. ASHA Monogr. 17.Google Scholar
Brown, R. & Bellugi, U. (1964). Three processes in the child's acquisition of syntax. In Lenneberg, E. (ed.), New directions in the study of language. Cambridge, Mass.: M.I.T. Press.Google Scholar
Cazden, C. (1965). Environmental assistance to the child's acquisition of grammar. Un published doctoral dissertation, Harvard University.Google Scholar
Cross, T. B. (1977). Mothers' speech adjustments: the contributions of selected child listener variables. In Snow, C. E. & Ferguson, C. A. (eds), Talking to children: language input and acquisition. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
DePaulo, B. M. & Bonvillian, J. D. (1978). The effects on language development of the special characteristics of speech addressed to children. JPsycholingRes 7. 189211.Google Scholar
Ferguson, C. A. (1964). Baby talk in six languages. AmAnth 66. 103–14.Google Scholar
Garnica, O. K. (1977). Some prosodic and paralinguistic features of speech to young children. In Snow, C. E. & Ferguson, C. A. (eds), Talking to children: language input and acquisition. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Grishaver, J. R. (1976). Adults' speech to children: the influence of intonation and rate of presentation during imitation. Unpublished senior thesis, Vassar College.Google Scholar
McNeill, D. (1970). The acquisition of language: the study of developmental psycholinguistics. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
McNeill, D. (1974). The two-fold way for speech. In Problènes actuels en psycholinguistique. Paris: Editions du Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique.Google Scholar
Phillips, J. R. (1973). Syntax and vocabulary of mothers' speech to young children: age and sex comparisons. ChDev 44. 183–5.Google Scholar
Sachs, J. S., Brown, R. & Salerno, R. A. (1976). Adults' speech to children. In von Raffler Engel, W. & Lebrun, Y. (eds), Baby talk and infant speech. Amsterdam: Swets & Zeitlinger.Google Scholar
Scholes, R. J. (1970). On functors and contentives in children's imitations of word strings. JVLVB 9. 167–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shatz, M. & Gelman, R. (1973). The development of communication skills: modifications in the speech of young children as a function of listener. Monogr. Soc. Res. Ch. Devel. 38. No. 5.Google ScholarPubMed
Snow, C. E. (1972). Mothers' speech to children learning language. ChDev 43. 549–65.Google Scholar
Snow, C. E. & Ferguson, C. A. (eds). (1977). Talking to children: language input and acquisition. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Weeks, T. (1971). Speech registers in children. ChDev 42. 1119–31.Google Scholar