We use cookies to distinguish you from other users and to provide you with a better experience on our websites. Close this message to accept cookies or find out how to manage your cookie settings.
Author's address:Universität Potsdam, Department Linguistik, Komplex II, Haus 35, Karl-Liebknechtstraße 24–25, D-14476 Golm, Germanyvicente@uni-potsdam.de
An abstract is not available for this content so a preview has been provided. Please use the Get access link above for information on how to access this content.
Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)
References
REFERENCES
Baković, Eric. 1998. Optimality and inversion in Spanish. In Barbosa, Pilar, Fox, Danny, Hagstrom, Paul, McGinnis, Martha & Pesetsky, David (eds.), Is the best good enough? Optimality and competition in syntax, 35–58. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bobaljik, Jonathan. 2002. Realizing Germanic inflection: Why morphology does not drive syntax. Journal of Germanic Linguistics6.2, 129–167.Google Scholar
Boeckx, Cedric. 2003. Islands and chains: Stranding as resumption. Amsterdam & Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 1970. Remarks on nominalization. In Jacobs, Roderick A. & Rosenbaum, Peter S. (eds.), Readings in English transformational grammar, 184–221. Waltham, MA: Ginn.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 2000. Minimalist inquiries: The framework. In Martin, Robert, Michaels, David & Uriagereka, Juan (eds.), Step by step: Essays in Minimalist syntax in honor of Howard Lasnik, 89–155. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 2001. Derivation by phase. In Kenstowicz, (ed.), 1–52.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 2004. Beyond explanatory adequacy. In Belletti, Adriana (ed.), The cartography of syntactic structures, vol. 3: Structures and beyond, 104–131. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 2005. Three factors in language design. Linguistic Inquiry36.1, 1–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 2006. On phases. In Freidin, Robert, Otero, Carlos & Zubizarreta, Maria Luisa (eds.), Foundational issues in linguistic theory: Essays in honor of Jean-Roger Vergnaud, 133–166. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 2007. Approaching UG from below. In Sauerland, Uli & Gärtner, Hans-Martin (eds.), Interfaces+recursion=language? Chomsky's Minimalism and the view from syntax-semantics, 1–29. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Gallego, Ángel J. 2007. Phase theory and parametric variation. Ph.D. dissertation, Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona.Google Scholar
Kenstowicz, Michael (ed.). 2001. Ken Hale: A life in language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Pesetsky, David & Torrego, Esther. 2001. T-to-C movement: Causes and consequences. In Kenstowicz, (ed.), 355–426.Google Scholar
Pesetsky, David & Torrego, Esther. 2004. Tense, Case, and the nature of syntactic categories. In Guéron, Jacqueline & Lecarme, Jacqueline (eds.), The syntax of time, 495–537. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Torrego, Esther. 1985. On empty categories in nominals. Ms., University of Massachusetts Boston.Google Scholar