Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-q99xh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T23:05:08.508Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Voice mismatch and contrast in French Right-Node Raising

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 February 2023

ANNE ABEILLÉ
Affiliation:
Université Paris Cité, CNRS, anne.abeille@u-paris.fr
AOI SHIRAISHI
Affiliation:
University of Tokyo, Japan, ioaaoi430@gmail.com
BARBARA HEMFORTH
Affiliation:
Université Paris Cité, CNRS, barbara-edith.hemforth@cnrs.fr
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Right-Node Raising is generally considered to impose stricter identity conditions than other kinds of ellipsis, such as VP ellipsis, according to Hankamer & Sag 1976 and Hardt 1993. In this paper, we investigate voice mismatch in French Right-Node Raising (RNR) through a corpus study and two experiments. We show that RNR with voice mismatch can be found in a written corpus (frTenTen 2012) and that many examples involve coordination of a reflexive active and a short passive form. We suggest this is because semantic contrast (here, between self and external agent) plays a role according to Hartmann (2000) and Abeillé and Mouret (2010). We ran two acceptability judgement experiments to test voice mismatch and semantic contrast. We did not find any penalty for voice mismatch with VP ellipsis but an interaction with semantic contrast. We also found an effect of semantic contrast when coordinating an active and a passive VP without participle ellipsis. We conclude that voice mismatch is acceptable with RNR and propose a Head-driven Phrase-Structure Grammar (HPSG) analysis, following Chaves (2014) and Shiraïshi et al. (2019).

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

1. Introduction

The possibility of voice mismatch has been a hot topic in the study of VP ellipsis since it undermines the deletion under identity approach, or structural approach to ellipsis (Merchant Reference Merchant2013). On the other hand, it is expected under semantics-based approaches to elliptical constructions (Ginzburg & Sag Reference Ginzburg and Sag2000 a.o.). Other kinds of ellipsis (gapping, sluicing) have been shown to resist voice mismatch, but Right-Node Raising (RNR) has not been considered in this respect. Different analyses have been proposed for RNR (Ross Reference Ross1967, Kayne Reference Kayne1994, Chaves Reference Chaves2014) but none of them predict the existence of such mismatch.

1.1. Voice mismatch and ellipsis

Voice mismatch has been studied for VP ellipsis and is attested in English (Hardt Reference Hardt1993, Kehler Reference Kehler2000, Kertz Reference Kertz2013). Both active elided VPs with a passive antecedent (Examples (1a, c)) and the reverse (Example (1b)) (a passive elided VP with an active antecedent) are observed.

Merchant (Reference Merchant2013) tries to reconcile the possibility of voice mismatch with a deletion under identity approach: he posits a VoiceP node and assumes that VP ellipsis takes place below VoiceP: so in this approach, there is no mismatch. (Figure 1, Figure 2).

Figure 1 Passive structure from Merchant (Reference Merchant2013)

Figure 2 Active structure from Merchant (Reference Merchant2013)

This analysis makes two predictions: (i) VP ellipsis with voice mismatch should be as acceptable as with match and (ii) voice mismatch is only possible with small (VP) ellipsis but not with large (clausal) ellipsis.

Most of the time, there is a penalty associated with mismatch (Kim & Runner Reference Kim and Runner2018). Different authors have proposed factors that favor or disfavor voice mismatch with VP ellipsis. According to Kehler (Reference Kehler2000), asymmetric discourse relations between the source and the target such as Cause-Effect (Example (3a)) make voice mismatches more acceptable than symmetric discourse relations such as Resemblance (Example (3b)).

Kertz (Reference Kertz2013) suggests that topic continuity plays an important role in VP ellipsis. In a series of experimental studies, she shows there is a penalty with voice mismatch when the subjects are contrastive topics as in Example (4a). In Example (4b), on the other hand, the subject of the sentence with voice mismatch is not a contrastive topic (the agent is the same as in the source sentence) and the mismatch penalty is reduced.

She also suggests that the penalty for lack of topic continuity is independent of ellipsis.

From a different perspective, Arregui et al. (Reference Arregui, Clifton, Frazier and Moulton2006) assume that the grammar of ellipsis requires a syntactically matching antecedent for the elided constituent. Ellipsis is ungrammatical when no syntactically matching antecedent is available, despite the actual occurrence of such examples in naturally occurring speech. In case of ungrammaticality, the parser may however “recycle” a parsed constituent and thus give the illusion of grammaticality. In a series of experiments, they found a greater penalty for a passive elided VP with an active antecedent (Examples (1b), (5b)) than for an active elided VP with a passive antecedent (Examples (1c), (5a)).

The Recycling Hypothesis relies on earlier findings showing that a passive, as a non canonical construction, is more easily misrecalled as an active (Mehler Reference Mehler1963, Thuilier et al. Reference Thuilier, Abeillé, Crabbé and Grant2020) than the other way around (an active being misrecalled as a passive). A passive-active mismatch may thus create an illusion of grammaticality because the speaker misrecalls the antecedent as active.

Poppels & Kehler (Reference Poppels and Kehler2019) tested the Recycling Hypothesis in a series of experiments where they found a general passive penalty: VP ellipsis with a passive antecedent is judged worse than with an active antecedent, even with voice match.

They also challenge the Recycling Hypothesis by testing cataphoric VP ellipsis: if the ellipsis site precedes the source, there is no antecedent to be misrecalled.

Poppels & Kehler (Reference Poppels and Kehler2019) found a mismatch penalty (Examples (7c), (7d)) and a passive first penalty (Examples (7b), (7d)) but no interaction between the two. This contradicts the Recycling Hypothesis that would predict that the mismatch in the passive first condition would be better since the passive could be misrecalled as active. They conclude that their results suggest a more general Passive Ellipsis Clause Penalty (PECP), accounting for the contrast between Examples (5-a) and (5-b), between Examples (6a) and (6b), and between Examples (7c) and (7d). To see whether PECP applies to other ellipsis, they tested sluicing and gapping in a pilot study: only sluicing led to a small non-significant passive penalty so that they concluded that PECP may be unique to VPE.

If Merchant Reference Merchant2013 is right, “larger” ellipsis should not allow for voice mismatch. Indeed, sluicing Example (8b) and gapping Example (8d) do not exhibit such acceptable mismatches.

But contrary to Merchant (Reference Merchant2013)’s predictions, in a corpus study, Miller (Reference Miller and Pinon2014), as well as Kim & Runner (Reference Kim and Runner2022), found attested examples of voice mismatch with pseudogapping (Example (9a)), that should not be possible under a raising remnant analysis (Kuno Reference Kuno1981) (NP, Noun Phrase, PP, Prepositional Phrase; VP, Verb Phrase):

We can conclude that structural identity-based approaches may either accept (Merchant Reference Merchant2013) or reject (Arregui et al. Reference Arregui, Clifton, Frazier and Moulton2006) voice mismatch, and have a hard time predicting which cases of voice mismatch are acceptable and which are not. It also remains to be tested whether voice mismatch is acceptable for other types of ellipsis than VP ellipsis and pseudogapping.

In this paper, we focus on voice mismatch in RNR (Ross Reference Ross1967, Chaves Reference Chaves2014) for French, which has been shown to accept some syntactic mismatches (Shiraishi et al. Reference Shiraishi, Abeillé, Hemforth and Miller2019).

1.2. RNR and syntactic mismatch

Right-Node Raising is often set apart from other elliptical constructions since the elided clause precedes the full clause.

Several syntactic analyses have been proposed for RNR. It has been analyzed in terms of rightward movement (Ross Reference Ross1967, Hankamer Reference Hankamer1971, Postal Reference Postal1974, Gazdar Reference Gazdar1981, Steedman Reference Steedman1996, Sabbagh Reference Sabbagh2007, Sabbagh Reference Sabbagh2014) (Example (10a)), multidominance (McCawley Reference McCawley1988, Goodall Reference Goodall1987, Moltmann Reference Moltmann1992, Wilder Reference Wilder, Bird, Carnie, Haugen and Norquest1999, Gracanin-Yuksek Reference Gracanin-Yuksek2007, Bachrach & Katzir Reference Bachrach, Katzir and Grohmann2009) (Example (10b)) (Figure 3), and deletion under identity (Wexler & Culicover Reference Wexler and Culicover1980, Kayne Reference Kayne1994, Hartmann Reference Hartmann2000, Yatabe Reference Yatabe, Flickinger and Kathol2001, Beavers & Sag Reference Beavers, Sag and Müller2004, Yatabe Reference Yatabe and Müller2012, Chaves Reference Chaves2014, Shiraishi et al. Reference Shiraishi, Abeillé, Hemforth and Miller2019) (Example (10c)).

Figure 3 Simplified RNR analysis with multidominance

Syntactic mismatches between elided and peripheral material cast doubt on raising and multidominance theories. Under their most natural implementations, all of these analyses predict that voice mismatch should not be possible.

Chaves (Reference Chaves2014) proposes that the missing and peripheral strings must have the same morphophonology and that the latter must be prosodically independent. Shiraishi et al. (Reference Shiraishi, Abeillé, Hemforth and Miller2019) propose a revised deletion analysis based on lexeme identity instead of morphosyntactic identity, in order to capture polarity, preposition, and verb form mismatch. Verb form mismatch has been observed by Shiraishi et al. (Reference Shiraishi, Abeillé, Hemforth and Miller2019) (Example (11a)) as well as polarity (Example (11b)) (Kayne Reference Kayne1994) or preposition mismatches (Example (11c)) (Abeillé et al. Reference Abeillé, Crysmann, Shiraïshi and Piñón2016):

Some examples of voice mismatches can also be found in English, but it is not easy to tell RNR apart from cataphoric VP ellipsis.

Other languages such as German may have voice mismatch in RNR too (without having VP ellipsis) (examples from B. Crysmann, pc).

Some examples of voice mismatches can be found for French as well (Abeillé et al. Reference Abeillé, Crysmann, Shiraïshi and Piñón2016).

In Examples (14) and (15), the missing past participle in the first conjunct corresponds to the passive participle in the second conjunct. In French, as in English, past participle and passive participle are syncretic forms. But unlike English, French does not have VP ellipsis with auxiliaries (Emonds Reference Emonds1987; Abeillé & Godard Reference Abeillé and Godard2002), be they followed by a past or a passive participle (avoir, être).Footnote 2

1.3. RNR and semantic contrast

According to Hartmann (Reference Hartmann2000) and Ha (Reference Ha2008), RNR needs to have a semantic contrast (contrasting values inside a set of alternatives) between the predicates in the sequences (typically conjuncts) sharing the peripheral sequence (e.g., detests and likes in Example (17a) and has and have not in Example (17b)) (see Rooth Reference Rooth1992 for a formal definition of semantic contrast).

As pointed out by Chaves, following Kentner et al. (2008), this semantic contrast does not always lead to typical contrastive focus intonation, even if it may, as noted by the capital letters in Example (17a). In an English corpus study (Penn Treebank), Bîlbîie (Reference Bîlbîie2013) found that most RNR examples involved polarity, modality, aspect, or tense contrast (Examples (18), see also, Example (11a) above with tense contrast).

We call this the “contrast condition on RNR”, and, to the best of our knowledge, it has not been tested experimentally. In the absence of such contrast, syntactically well-formed RNR are less acceptable, as shown by the intuitive difference between Examples (19a) and (19b):Footnote 3

The unacceptability of Example (19b) shows that subject contrast is not enough, and in that case, VPE (or stripping) is preferred, as in Example (19c).

For French, Abeillé & Mouret (Reference Abeillé and Mouret2010) conducted a study on written (newspaper) and spoken (radio) corpora. They found that most RNR examples displayed polarity, tense, or modality contrast between the two verbal conjuncts, as in the following examples (see also Example (15) above which displays past/present tense contrast.

They also observe a tendency towards topic continuity, with VP coordination (with the same subject) rather than clausal coordination, in naturally occurring examples of RNR, although it is not always observed in the linguistic literature, Example (19a).

Given the lack of empirical data on voice mismatch and semantic contrast in RNR, we investigate French RNR in comparison with previous studies on voice mismatch in VP ellipsis. Among other differences, RNR differs from VP ellipsis since: (i) almost anything can be elided (not just a VP or a predicate phrase), (ii) the ellipsis site precedes the full clause, and (iii) RNR must be intrasentential, unlike VPE. In what follows, we limit ourselves to (VP) RNR in coordinated clauses.

In Section 2 of this paper, we present the results of a corpus study on voice mismatch in French RNR. In Section 3, we report on the results of an acceptability judgement experiment on VP RNR, showing no mismatch penalty but a possible effect of semantic contrast. In Section 4, we present a second experiment which confirms a semantic contrast effect in coordinated clauses independently of VP RNR. In the last section, we sketch a Head-driven Phrase-Structure Grammar (HPSG) analysis based on lexeme identity, following Shiraishi et al. Reference Shiraishi, Abeillé, Hemforth and Miller2019.

2. A corpus study on voice mismatch in French RNR

We first conducted an informal search on the Internet, with conjoined active and passive auxiliaries. Short passives are easier to find than long passives (with a “par” (by) phrase). RNR involving an active verb with an NP complement and a long passive verb as in Example (21) are not very natural (Abeillé et al. Reference Abeillé, Crysmann, Shiraïshi and Piñón2016).

We mostly found examples of voice mismatch with reflexive active verbs, as in Example (22) from a newspaper article:

We then did a more systematic search on the web corpus frTenTen 2012 (about 10 billion words, Baroni et al. Reference Baroni, Bernardini, Ferraresi and Zanchetta2009) for sequences of coordinated relative clauses with different voice auxiliaries and for sequences with coordination of different voice auxiliaries. We searched for the following sequences :

qui se sont et/ou qui ont été (“who se(reflexive) are and/or who have been’), qui ont été et/ou qui se sont (“who have been and/or who se(reflexive) are”), qui sest et/ou qui a été (“who se(reflexive) is and/or who has been”), qui a été et/ou qui sest (“who has been and/or who se(reflexive) is”), quon a et/ou qui a/ont été (”that one has and/or that has/have been”), se sont et/ou ont été (“se(reflexive) are and/or have been”), ont été et/ou se sont (“have been and/or se(reflexive) are”), sest et/ou a été (“se(reflexive) is and/or has been”), a été et/ou sest (“has been and/or se(reflexive) is”).

French reflexive verbs come in different varieties (Melis Reference Melis1990, Barque & Candito Reference Barque and Candito2019). We only kept those which display an argumental reflexive. We thus left aside cases without a non reflexive counterpart, be they intrinsic reflexives (s’évanouir “faint”), mediopassives, or anticausatives, as in Example (23).

The results are provided in Table 1. We do not report the strings which returned no hits.

Table 1 RNR with voice mismatch in frTenTen12 corpus.

We found 27 examples of RNR with voice mismatch in frTenTen 2012. Both active-passive (Example (24a)) and passive-active (Example (24b)) orders are attested but the latter are more frequent (21 out of 27).Footnote 4 This is difficult to compare with previous findings on voice mismatch with VP ellipsis (Section 1.1): Arregui et al. (Reference Arregui, Clifton, Frazier and Moulton2006) found that passive-active mismatch was easier than active-passive mismatch, and proposed a processing explanation, but, in the case of VPE, the full antecedent precedes the elliptical VP, and can be misrecalled, while in RNR, the ellipsis site precedes the peripheral material. As mentioned before, Poppels & Kehler (Reference Poppels and Kehler2019) proposed a more general passive penalty for VP ellipsis, but in our data, the first elided predicate is actually more often the passive one.

All examples that we found involve a reflexive active and a passive. We did not find any examples with a non reflexive active and a passive. Reflexives have been argued to bear some similarities with passives in French. They use the same être (be) auxiliary as passives, and the participle obeys the same (subject) agreement pattern (non reflexive transitive verbs have the avoir (have) auxiliary and the participle does not agree with the subject) (Abeillé et al. Reference Abeillé, Godard, Sag, Hinrichs, Kathol and Nakazawa1998, Abeillé & Godard Reference Abeillé and Godard2002):

From a derivational perspective, some authors treat French se verbs as intransitive unaccusatives, like passives (Wehrli Reference Wehrli and Borer1986): se absorbs accusative case or se prevents the verb from assigning case to its object, which raises to subject position (see also Grimshaw Reference Grimshaw and Bresnan1982 for an analysis of Romance reflexives as intransitive verbs, inside a non derivational framework). In this approach, there is less syntactic mismatch between a reflexive active and a passive, since both have an empty object position, than between a transitive active and a passive.

However, this unaccusative approach of passives is controversial. Sells et al. (Reference Sells, Zaenen, Zec, Zec, Iida and Wechsler1987) show that reflexive predicates vary cross-linguistically in the mapping between the lexicon, syntax, and semantics, in particular, as being closed or open predicates. Labelle (Reference Labelle2008) argues that the French se verb is an open predicate, since it can be transitive and have another reflexive marker (Example ((26)): she proposes that se does not reduce the argument structure of the verb and that the DP that surfaces in subject position is the external argument. We follow this proposal. For more on our syntactic analysis, which does not rely on syntactic movement, see Section 5.2 below.

From a semantic perspective, in our corpus examples (as in Example (24)), the reflexive and the (agentless) passive verbs obey the semantic contrast condition on RNR seen above (Section 1.3), since one involves a reflexive predicate (self agent) and the other the same predicate with an external (unknown or implicit) agent. In an example of a non reflexive active followed by a passive (Example (27a)), on the other hand, the two conjuncts do not exhibit a simple semantic contrast, since one has an unknown patient and the other an unknown agent.

Furthermore, from a discourse perspective, reflexives may also be argued to share some properties with passives, since in our examples, they also keep topic continuity (same subject) better than non reflexive actives, a condition independently observed by (Kertz Reference Kertz2013) for English VPE. To conclude, we suggest that the tendency found in the corpus for examples with a (intransitive) reflexive active and a passive (and not with a transitive active and a passive) may not come from syntax (assuming a reflexive verb has an empty object position like a passive) but rather from semantics, since reflexives/short passives allow a contrast between self and external agent, and also from discourse since they assure topic continuity.

3. Experiment 1: Testing RNR with voice mismatch

Our corpus data lead us to suppose that RNR with voice mismatch can be acceptable. We conducted two acceptability judgement experiments in order to test both the effect of voice mismatch and the effect of semantic contrast on French RNR. We tested whether voice match and semantic contrast play a role and whether there is an interaction between the two factors. To the best of our knowledge, semantic contrast has not been tested experimentally so far (see Section 1.3 above).

3.1. Materials

The materials for Experiment 1 were inspired by the examples from the corpus study, with two conjoined relative clauses and a shared participle. We constructed 20 target items with four conditions: $ \pm $ match, $ \pm $ contrast (Example (28)).Footnote 5 All conditions have topic continuity with the same subject. In the mismatch conditions, we always have active-passive order, in order to avoid a penalty predicted by Arregui et al. (Reference Arregui, Clifton, Frazier and Moulton2006)’s Recycling Hypothesis and Poppels & Kehler (Reference Poppels and Kehler2019)’s Passive Ellipsis Penalty. In the conditions with semantic contrast, the thematic roles of the antecedent of the relative clauses are contrasted with an agent in the first clause and a patient in the second clause. In the mismatch condition, the agent of the predicate is thus the subject of the (reflexive) active, while it is unspecified in the passive (Example (28a)). In the match condition, the agent is the subject of the reflexive active and the indefinite “on” of the non reflexive active (Example (28c)). In the conditions without contrast, the antecedent is always a patient: the agent is indefinite (on) or unspecified in the mismatch condition (Example (28b)), and is unspecified in the match condition (Example (28d)). Notice that in this last condition (no mismatch, no agent contrast), the auxiliary past tenses are as similar as possible (imperfect and perfect).

20 target items were created, each of them in the four conditions described above. They were distributed across four lists using a Latin Square design, such that each participant saw five items in each condition but never the same item in more than one condition. Target items and 40 unrelated fillers were presented in a random order which was different for each participant. The full set of items can be found in the OSF-project: RNR-Materials (https://osf.io/2pdnj/). Fillers were French garden-path sentences based on materials from (Paape et al. Reference Paape, Hemforth and Vasishth2018). These fillers had the advantage of spanning a wide range of acceptability (from difficult garden-paths with a mean acceptability of 4.9 to easy non-garden-paths with a mean acceptability of 7.7, on a 0–10 scale).

3.2. Procedure

We conducted the online experiment on IbexFarm (Drummond Reference Drummond2010), using a server at the University Paris Cité for data storage. Before starting the experiment, subjects provided information about their age, native language, and gender and gave their written consent to participate in the experiment. For each experimental trial, participants read a sentence and judged its acceptability on a scale from 0 (not at all acceptable) to 10 (fully acceptable).Footnote 6 The duration of the experiment was 15 minutes on average. Each experimental session started with three practice items.

48 participants (22 female, mean age: 30 years), recruited on Prolific, with the constraints that they currently live in France and only spoke French when growing up, judged the acceptability of the sentences on a scale of 0 to 10 and answered simple comprehension questions. Two participants were excluded from the analysis for answering less than 80% of the questions correctly. All participants received £2.20 via the Prolific platform.Footnote 7

3.3. Results

Given that we obtained acceptability judgements from our participants, which do not fulfill requirements for linear mixed models, we analyzed the results with cumulative link mixed models using the clmm function in the ordinal package in R (Christensen Reference Christensen2019). Figures in this paper show means and confidence intervals for convenience since most readers are familiar with this kind of data presentation, although models for data analysis are based on the raw ordinal data resulting from the acceptability judgements. Independent variables were voice mismatch and semantic contrast. They were centered around zero for statistical analyses. Participants and items were included as random variables. Only intercept models could be run due to convergence failure of more complex models (model equation: clmm(judgment = contrast $ \ast $ match $ + $ (1 $ \mid $ participant)+(1 $ \mid $ item))). The results are given in Figure 4.Footnote 8

Figure 4 (Color online) Results of Experiment 1 on VP RNR

While acceptability judgements across conditions are all very high (7–8 out of 10), we did not find any significant effect of voice mismatch. We did, however, find a significant interaction of Match and Contrast, with Contrast leading to higher acceptability ratings in the mismatch condition. No other effects were significant (interaction effect: $ \beta $ = $ -.75, st. error $ = $ .24,z $ = $ -3.114,p $ < $ .002 $ ).

In the match condition, the contrast condition (Example (28c)) involves two active auxiliaries (qui se sont ou quon a), whereas the no contrast condition (Example (28d)) involves two passive auxiliaries (qui étaient ou qui ont été), but there was no significant difference there. In the mismatch condition, the contrast condition (Example (28a)) involves an active reflexive and a passive (qui se sont ou qui ont été), while the no contrast condition (Example (28b)) involves a non reflexive active and a passive (qu’on a ou qui ont été), and we find a significant difference in favor of the former ( $ \beta $ = $ .50, st. error $ = $ .25,z $ = $ 2.034,p $ < $ .05 $ ).

Unlike the voice mismatch penalty observed in previous experiments on English, VP ellipsis (Arregui et al. Reference Arregui, Clifton, Frazier and Moulton2006, Kertz Reference Kertz2013, Poppels & Kehler Reference Poppels and Kehler2019 a.o.), we did not find a mismatch penalty: the target items with voice mismatch are as acceptable as the items with voice match. As predicted by Hartmann (Reference Hartmann2000), Ha (Reference Ha2008), we found a penalty for items without semantic contrast but only in the case of voice mismatch. This does, however, not necessarily mean that the contrast effects depend on mismatch (see Experiment 2 below). The lack of an effect in the match condition may also be due to the minimal difference between the two past tense forms in the +match $ - $ contrast condition (Example (28d)), which can be interpreted as aspect contrast between imperfect and perfect.

One may attribute the high acceptability of RNR with voice mismatch to the syncretism between active and passive participles. However, Shiraishi et al. (Reference Shiraishi, Abeillé, Hemforth and Miller2019) found that RNR with verb form mismatch without syncretism is highly acceptable in French.

One may also consider the VoiceP hypothesis to play a role for French. If we follow Merchant (Reference Merchant2013)’s proposal, there is no syntactic mismatch between active and passive. The structure for active-passive would involve the same VP as passive-passive:

Figure 5 Active structure with VoiceP

Figure 6 Passive structure with VoiceP

What about reflexives? If they are similar to passives (syntactically intransitives, with an object raised to subject position (Wehrli Reference Wehrli and Borer1986)), our (Example (28a)) condition (reflexive and passive) would be a case of match and our (Example (28c)) condition (reflexive and active) would be a case of mismatch. However, we did not find a significant difference between the two (p $ > $ .40). On the other hand, Labelle (Reference Labelle2008) has argued that reflexive is a specific voice in French, with its own VoiceP, different from (non reflexive) active and passive:

Figure 7 Reflexive structure with VoiceP

Under Labelle’s view, there would be voice mismatch also in the (Example (28c)) (reflexive-active) condition. But the reflexive-active condition (with contrast) was rated higher than the match (passive-passive) (Example (28d)) condition (without contrast).

We conclude that the effect of structural identity (with voice mismatch under some analyses, without mismatch under a VoiceP analysis) is not sufficient to explain our data, and that semantic contrast plays a role.

As for semantic contrast (Hartmann Reference Hartmann2000, Ha Reference Ha2008), we found an interaction with voice mismatch in Experiment 1, so that the effect of contrast was significant in the mismatch condition. Footnote 9 Since we expect this effect to be independent from voice mismatch, we ran a second experiment without voice mismatch.

4. Experiment 2: Contrast effects without voice mismatch

Experiment 2 was designed to test whether semantic contrast plays a role independently of voice mismatch. We therefore used materials similar to Experiment 1 but with the participle repeated and not elided. Most of the time, a final phrase was shared between the two conjuncts (and, hence, could be argued to be elided in the first conjunct). In Example (31), it is the PP à Rome (”to Rome”). We applied the same 2x2 design ( $ \pm $ match, $ \pm $ contrast) as in Experiment 1.

20 target items were created, each in the four conditions described above. Target items and 16 unrelated fillers were distributed across four lists using a Latin Square design and presented in a random order which was different for each participant.

The filler items came from an unrelated experiment, on the attachment of relative clauses to coordinated noun phrases as in Example (32) (Hedier et al. Reference Hedier, Su and Hemforth2021).

4.1. Procedure

The procedure in this experiment was nearly identical to Experiment 1. The only difference was that participants judged sentence acceptability on a scale from 1 (not at all acceptable) to 5 (fully acceptable), since this scale was used by the unrelated experiment which provided the filler items.Footnote 10 The duration of the experiment was 10 minutes on average.

Data from 54 French native speakers (32 female), recruited on the RISC web site (http://www.risc.cnrs.fr/) who volunteered to participate in the online experiment on Ibex (Drummond Reference Drummond2010) were analyzed in this experiment. The results from one participant who was not a native speaker of French were excluded from the analyses. The age of the participants ranged from 18 to 69, with a mean age of 31 years.

4.2. Results

We analyzed the results with cumulative link mixed models mixed effects regression models using the clmm function in the ordinal package in R (Christensen Reference Christensen2019). As before, independent variables were voice mismatch and semantic contrast. They were centered around zero for statistical analyses. Participants and items were included as random variables. Convergence failures made it impossible to include random slopes for the fixed factors. The results are given in Figure 8.

Figure 8 (Color online) Results of Experiment 2 on PP RNR

Even without VP RNR, we could establish a significant effect of semantic contrast with an advantage for the +contrast conditions (contrast: $ \beta $ = $ -0.4744, st. error $ = $ .1367,z $ = $ -3.472,p $ < $ .001 $ ). We also found an advantage for the mismatch condition (match: $ \beta $ = $ 0.3546, st. error $ = $ 0.1312,z $ = $ 2.701,p $ < $ .01 $ ) but no interaction between the two factors.

4.3. Discussion

The +contrast conditions received higher acceptability ratings than the $ - $ contrast conditions even without participle ellipsis. This shows that the contrast effect shown in Experiment 1 on VP RNR is more general. This confirms Hartmann (Reference Hartmann2000), Ha (Reference Ha2008), and Abeillé & Mouret (Reference Abeillé and Mouret2010)’s contrast condition on RNR proposed for English, German, and French. It is also possible that a semantic contrast between two coordinate predicates is an independent requirement of verbal/clausal coordination.

The advantage for the mismatch condition may be the consequence of the fact that the items were based on naturalistic items with mismatch from the corpus study, while match items were adapted from them. It may be possible that factors not included in this study make the mismatch condition more natural. This would be the topic of future studies.

5. An HPSG analysis

Following Chaves (Reference Chaves2014), Shiraishi et al. (Reference Shiraishi, Abeillé, Hemforth and Miller2019), we show how an HPSG analysis can account for our data.

5.1. Previous analyses of RNR

As noted earlier (see Section 1.2), several analyses have been proposed for RNR. Even though a movement analysis may be reconciled with mismatch, various arguments have been proposed against movement analyses (for overall discussion, see Beavers & Sag Reference Beavers, Sag and Müller2004, Bachrach & Katzir Reference Bachrach, Katzir and Grohmann2009 a.o.), namely, RNR can target more than one constituent (Abbott Reference Abbott1976), as shown in Example (33a); it can target non-constituents, as shown in Example (33b) below; and it does not obey locality constraints otherwise observed for wh-movement or extraposition (Levine Reference Levine1985), as shown in Example (33c).

Syntactic mismatches cast doubt on multidominance approaches, pace (Citko Reference Citko2018). Such approaches are at pains to explain why in case of conflict, it is enough for the shared element to meet the requirements of the final conjunct and to ignore those of the first conjunct. For all these reasons, we do not adopt a movement-based analysis nor a multidominance analysis. This means that we consider Right-Node Raising a misnomer and that Peripheral Ellipsis would be more appropriate. However, we keep the traditional name and acronym here.

5.2. An analysis for French passives and reflexives

In what follows, we rely on previous work on French using HPSG (Pollard & Sag Reference Pollard and Sag1994, Müller et al. Reference Müller, Abeillé, Borsley and Koenig2021). Following Bresnan (Reference Bresnan and Bresnan1982), passives are analyzed in HPSG via a valence changing lexical rule (Flickinger Reference Flickinger1987, Müller Reference Müller, Kathol and Flickinger2001). Assuming a verb has its syntactic arguments in an argument structure list (ARG-ST), the first one corresponding to the subject, the passive lexical rule gives rise to a passive lexeme with a different argument structure, while keeping the semantics (and the thematic roles) the same. In French, the by-phrase can be introduced by par or de, depending on the verb agentivity (Koenig Reference Koenig1999, Zribi-Hertz & Abeillé Reference Zribi-Hertz, Abeillé, Abeillé and Godard2021), and it is analyzed as a complement (see also DaCunha & Abeillé Reference DaCunha and Abeillé2021, Angelopoulos et al. Reference Angelopoulos, Collins and Terzi2021).

Transitive verbs select the perfect auxiliary avoir, via an aux feature (Abeillé & Godard Reference Abeillé and Godard2002). Reflexive verbs are derived through another lexical rule that changes the auxiliary selection to être and types their complement as a non-canonical anaphoric affix (Abeillé et al. Reference Abeillé, Godard, Sag, Hinrichs, Kathol and Nakazawa1998), following the affixal analysis of French pronominal clitics (Miller Reference Miller1992, Miller & A. Sag. Reference Miller and Sag1997).

In the perfect tense, the reflexive affix is realized on the auxiliary as any other clitic.

In order to account for clitic climbing (Example (37)), Abeillé & Godard (Reference Abeillé, Godard, Aranovich, Byrne, Preuss and Senturia1994, Reference Abeillé and Godard2002) analyze French auxiliaries as complex predicates: they analyze the perfect auxiliaries as taking the past participle as a complement, and inheriting its arguments as subject and complements.Footnote 11 They also analyze the passive construction as a complex predicate, with the copula taking the passive participle as its complement, since it allows for clitic climbing as well (Example (38)).

5.3. An HPSG analysis for RNR

Following Shiraishi et al. (Reference Shiraishi, Abeillé, Hemforth and Miller2019), we assume that RNR does not require phonological identity (Example (11a)) but lexeme identity (unless pun or zeugma). The following example is unfelicitous because it involves two different homophonous lexemes: the baseball bat (elided) and the animal bat (peripheral):

To account for Example (11b), they consider that positive and negative polarity sensitive determiner (any/some) share the same lexeme, and introduce the Lexical Identifier (LID) feature to capture lexeme identity.

We assume the same LID value for passive and past participles which are derived from the same lexeme (Sadler & Spencer Reference Sadler, Spencer, Booij and Van Maarle2001, Bonami & Webelhuth Reference Bonami, Webelhuth, Chumakina and Corbett2013). The feature structure in Example (40) illustrates a simplified lexical entry for the past participle mobilisé. The feature structure in Example (41) gives a simplified lexical entry for the past participle with reflexive mobilisé. The feature structure in Example (42) shows a simplified lexical entry for the passive participle mobilisé.

Following Chaves (Reference Chaves2014), we formalize a RNR rule (43) which targets Morphophonology (MP) and does not add further syntactic constraints. It basically says that in a construction with two sublists with identical lexemes, and usually identical phonology, the first sublist can be deleted, leaving the meaning unchanged. We leave for future work the addition of a Contrast relation in the semantics (inside synsem), since we conjecture it could be more general and associated with clausal coordination.Footnote 12

The MP feature of the Daughter phrase is divided into five sublists, which must obey prosodic constraints, which we ignore here. The left-hand sublists, $ {\mathrm{L}}_1 $ and $ {\mathrm{L}}_2 $ (respectively, in black and in grey in (43)), correspond to any non final conjunct, and the right-hand sublists, $ {\mathrm{R}}_1 $ (in blue), $ {\mathrm{R}}_2 $ (in red), and $ {\mathrm{R}}_3 $ (in black), to the final conjunct. The first sublist $ {\mathrm{L}}_1 $ is kept. The deleted sublist $ {\mathrm{L}}_2 $ (in grey) must comprise elements with the same LID as $ {\mathrm{R}}_2 $ (the shared peripheral element(s), in red), and by default (noted /) the same phonology. Note that the elements in $ {\mathrm{L}}_2 $ are not preserved in the MP feature of the mother phrase (resulting in their not being pronounced). Thus, the form of the peripheral elements $ {\mathrm{R}}_2 $ (in red) is always that required by the final conjunct. The $ {\mathrm{R}}_1 $ (in blue) is the sublist before the shared elements and may comprise a coordinating conjunction. The extra $ {\mathrm{R}}_3 $ sublist accounts for Right-Node Wrapping Footnote 13 and can be empty.

Figure 9 provides the analysis for RNR with voice mismatch.

Figure 9 Analysis of French RNR with voice mismatch

6. Conclusions

In this paper, we have investigated voice mismatch in French Right-Node Raising through a corpus study and two acceptability judgement experiments, and provided an HPSG formalization. RNR is often thought to be special because it is backwards and less syntactically constrained than other kinds of ellipsis (Chaves Reference Chaves2014). We show that RNR allows for voice mismatch like VP ellipsis and pseudogapping. We found that RNR with voice mismatch is attested in written French. In our corpus study, most RNR with voice mismatch involve coordination of reflexive active and passive forms. This shows the importance of semantic contrast (here, between self and non self agent) in RNR, and also of topic continuity as in VP ellipsis (Kertz Reference Kertz2013). The first acceptability judgement experiment revealed that RNR with voice mismatch is as acceptable as without mismatch, and that (agent) contrast may play a role, in particular, for voice mismatch. The importance of semantic contrast is confirmed by the second experiment, without VP RNR.

This may be difficult to account for by a raising or multidominance analysis of RNR. We show that it is compatible with a lexeme deletion-based approach. Following Chaves (Reference Chaves2014) and Shiraishi et al. (Reference Shiraishi, Abeillé, Hemforth and Miller2019), we present an HPSG deletion-based analysis of RNR based on lexeme identity.

Footnotes

This work has been supported by the LabEx Empirical Foundations of Linguistics (ANR 10-LABX-0083). Part of this work was presented at the 2015 CSSP Conference in Paris, AMLaP 2016 Conference in Bilbao, the 2019 ECBAE Conference at UC Davis, and at the Linguistics Department of UC San Diego in 2019. We thank the participants at those events for their comments, as well as the JofL anonymous reviewers and the special issue editors.

[2] It has been argued that French allows VPE after some modal verbs (see, e.g., Busquets & Denis Reference Busquets and Denis2001, Dagnac Reference Dagnac, Arregi, Fagyal, Montrul and Tremblay2010), but there is a general consensus that it is impossible after tense and passive auxiliaries.

[3] As noted by a reviewer, a degree contrast on the verbs is possible too: Bill LIKES, and Mary absolutely ADORES the TV show (see also Example (11c) above with degree contrast.

[4] We also found more cases of “ou” (or) disjunctions than “et” (and) conjunctions. Due to the lack of syntactic annotations in the corpus, we cannot compare the mismatch cases with the number of match VP RNR. We therefore compared them to the general case of coordinations of the type “qui v et/ou qui v” (who v and/or who v), for which we found 30,795 for “et” and 7,241 for “ou”, so we find an overrepresentation of “ou” (or) in our RNR examples.

[5] Abeillé et al. (Reference Abeillé, Crysmann, Shiraïshi and Piñón2016) ran a previous version of Experiment 1 with only 12 target items, which did not show a mismatch effect either but a main effect of semantic contrast (p.18).

[6] The 0–10 scale has been used successfully in a number of recent experiments on French (Shiraishi et al. Reference Shiraishi, Abeillé, Hemforth and Miller2019, Abeillé et al. Reference Abeillé, Hemforth, Winckel and Gibson2020, An & Abeillé Reference An and Abeillé2021), and it is close to the rating scale used in the French school system.

[7] We actually ran this experiment twice, once with 19 distractors and once with 40 distractors. Since the patterns of results from both experiments are nearly identical, we will only present results from the latter experiment.

[8] No violations of the proportional odds assumption were found in any analysis in this paper when applying the nominal option in clmm2 separately for each factor (clmm2 analyses did not include random factors).

[9] As noted by a reviewer, Kim & Runner (Reference Kim and Runner2018) also found that acceptability of voice mismatch in VPE can be modulated by more general (not ellipsis specific) semantic or discourse constraints. However, they always found a residual VPE-specific voice mismatch penalty, that we do not observe in French RNR with voice mismatch.

[10] To our experience, switching between a 0 to 10 scale (Experiment 1) and a 1 to 5 scale (Experiment 2) has never resulted in relevant changes in results.

[11] Bonami & Webelhuth (Reference Bonami, Webelhuth, Chumakina and Corbett2013) reconcile this syntactic analysis with morphological periphrasis, using Paradigm Function Morphology: even though the auxiliary and the participle are different words, which are syntactically combined, their combination belongs to the inflectional paradigm of the verbal lexeme. Aguila-Multner & Crysmann (Reference Aguila-Multner and Crysmann2021) propose an alternate syntactic analysis with a VP complement and no argument composition. Nothing in the present paper depends on the syntactic analysis of the auxiliaries.

[12] As noted by Chaves (Reference Chaves2014), RNR is not limited to coordination, as shown by the attested example: she learns how to relax them to accept instead of contracting them to repel [the entering object]. (Brown Corpus, Bîlbîie Reference Bîlbîie2013). This is why the rule in Example (43) is not limited to coordination.

[13] See Whitman (Reference Whitman, Hinrichs and Nerbonne2009), who provides numerous attested examples, among which (i), in which the “wrapped” segment ”away” corresponds to $ {\mathrm{R}}_3 $ :(i) I’ve got friends in low places, where the whiskey drowns my blues and the beer chases my blues away. (“Friends in Low Places”, Earl Bud Lee and DeWayne Blackwell (Whitman Reference Whitman, Hinrichs and Nerbonne2009: 235, Shiraishi et al. Reference Shiraishi, Abeillé, Hemforth and Miller2019 fn16).

References

REFERENCES

Abbott, Barbara. 1976. Right Node Raising as a test for constituenthood. Linguistic Inquiry 7, 639642.Google Scholar
Abeillé, Anne & Godard, Danièle. 1994. The complementation of French auxiliaries. In Aranovich, Raul, Byrne, William, Preuss, Susanne & Senturia, Martha (eds.), Proceedings of the Thirteenth West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics, vol. 13, 157172. Stanford University.Google Scholar
Abeillé, Anne, Godard, Danièle & Sag, Ivan A.. 1998. Two kinds of composition in French complex predicates. In Hinrichs, Erhard, Kathol, Andreas & Nakazawa, Tsuneko (eds.), Complex predicates in non-derivational syntax, 141. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Abeillé, Anne & Godard, Danièle. 2002. The syntactic structure of French auxiliaries. Language 78.3, 404452.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abeillé, Anne & Mouret, François. 2010. Quelques contraintes sur les coordinations elliptiques en français. Revue de Sémantique et de Pragmatique 24, 177207.Google Scholar
Abeillé, Anne, Crysmann, Berthold & Shiraïshi, Aoi. 2016. Syntactic mismatch in French peripheral ellipsis. In Piñón, Christopher (ed.), Empirical issues in syntax and semantics, vol. 11, 130. doi:http://www.cssp.cnrs.fr/eiss11/eiss11_abeille-and-crysmann-and-shiraishi.pdf. (last accessed 2022-12-06).Google Scholar
Abeillé, Anne, Hemforth, Barbara, Winckel, Elodie & Gibson, Edward. 2020. Extraction from subject: Differences in acceptability depend on the discourse function of the construction. Cognition 104. doi:http://10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104293. (last accessed 2022-12-06).Google Scholar
Aguila-Multner, Gabrielle & Crysmann, Berthold. 2021. French clitic climbing as periphrasis. Lingvisticæ Investigationes 43.1, 2361.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
An, Aixiu & Abeillé, Anne. 2021. Closest conjunct agreement in the grammar: The case of French attributive adjectives. Journal of French Language Studies 22, 128.Google Scholar
Angelopoulos, Nikos, Collins, Chris & Terzi, Arhonto. 2021. Greek and English passives, and the role of by-phrases. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics 5.1, 90. doi:https://doi.org/10.5334/gjgl.1185. (last accessed 2022-12-06).Google Scholar
Arregui, Ana, Clifton, Charles, Frazier, Lyn & Moulton, Keir. 2006. Processing elided verb phrases with flawed antecedents: The recycling hypothesis. Journal of Memory and Language 55, 232246.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bachrach, Asaf & Katzir, Roni. 2009. Right-node raising and delayed spell-out. In Grohmann, Kleanthes K. (ed.), Interphases: Phase-theoretic investigations of linguistic interfaces, 283316. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Baroni, Marco, Bernardini, Silvia, Ferraresi, Adriano & Zanchetta, Eros. 2009. The WaCky wide web: A collection of very large linguistically processed web-crawled corpora. Language Resources and Evaluation 43.3, 209226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barque, Lucie & Candito, Marie. 2019. La classification des verbes réfléchis à l’épreuve d’une annotation en corpus. Langages 216.4, 121137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beavers, John & Sag, Ivan A.. 2004. Coordinate ellipsis and apparent non-constituent coordination. In Müller, Stefan (ed.), Proceedings of the HPSG-2004 Conference, Center for Computational Linguistics, Katholieke Universiteit Leuven, 4869. Stanford: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
Bîlbîie, Gabriela. 2013. A quantitative study on right node raising in the Penn Treebank. Presentation at the International Congress of Linguists, Geneva.Google Scholar
Bonami, Olivier & Webelhuth, Gert. 2013. The phrase-structural diversity of periphrasis: A lexicalist account. In Chumakina, Marina & Corbett, Greville (eds.), Periphrasis: The role of syntax and morphology in paradigms, Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bresnan, Joan. 1982. The passive in lexical theory. In Bresnan, Joan (ed.), The mental representation of grammatical relations, 386. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Busquets, Joan & Denis, Pascal. 2001. L’ellipse modale en français: Le cas de devoir et pouvoir. Cahiers de Grammaire 26, 5574.Google Scholar
Chaves, Rui Pedro. 2014. On the disunity of right-node raising phenomena: Extraposition, ellipsis, and deletion. Language 90.4, 834886.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christensen, R. H. B. 2019. ordinal—regression models for ordinal data. R package version 2019.12-10. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=ordinal (last accessed 2022-10-10).Google Scholar
Citko, Barbara. 2018. On the relationship between forward and backward gapping. Syntax Journal of Theoretical, Experimental and Interdisciplinary Research 21.1, 136.Google Scholar
DaCunha, Yanis & Abeillé, Anne. 2021. L’alternance actif/passif en français: Une étude statistique sur corpus écrit. Discours 27. doi:https://journals.openedition.org/discours/10956. (last accessed 2022-12-06).Google Scholar
Dagnac, Anne. 2010. Modal ellipsis in French, Spanish and Italian. Evidence for a TP-deletion analysis. In Arregi, Karlos, Fagyal, Zsuzsanna, Montrul, Silvina A. & Tremblay, Annie (eds.), Romance linguistics 2008: Interactions in Romance, 157170. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Drummond, Alex. 2010. Internet Based EXperiments (IBEX)(version 0.3).Google Scholar
Emonds, Joseph. 1987. The verbal complex V’ - V in French. Linguistic Inquiry 9.2, 151175.Google Scholar
Flickinger, Daniel Paul. 1987. Lexical rules in the hierarchical lexicon. Stanford University dissertation.Google Scholar
Gazdar, Gerald. 1981. Unbounded dependencies and coordinate structure. Linguistic Inquiry 12.2, 155184.Google Scholar
Ginzburg, Jonathan & Sag, Ivan A.. 2000. Interrogative investigations: the form, meaning, and use of English interrogatives. Stanford: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
Goodall, Grant. 1987. Parallel structures in syntax: Coordination, causatives, and restructuring. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gracanin-Yuksek, Martina. 2007. On sharing. MIT dissertation.Google Scholar
Grimshaw, Jane. 1982. On the lexical representation of romance reflexive clitics. In Bresnan, Joan (ed.), The mental representation of grammatical relations, 87148. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Ha, Seungwan. 2007. Contrastive focus: Licensor for right node raising. In Elfner, Emily & Walkow, Martin (eds.), Proceedings of the 37th Annual Meeting of the North East Linguistics Society 37, 247260. University of Illinois, Urbana-Champagne.Google Scholar
Ha, Seungwan. 2008. Ellipsis, right node raising, and across-the-board constructions. Boston University dissertation.Google Scholar
Hankamer, Jorge. 1971. Deletion in coordinate structures. Yale University dissertation.Google Scholar
Hankamer, Jorge & Sag, Ivan A.. 1976. Deep and surface anaphora. Linguistic Inquiry 7.3, 391426.Google Scholar
Hardt, Daniel. 1993. Verb phrase ellipsis: Form, meaning, and processing. University of Pennsylvania dissertation.Google Scholar
Hartmann, Katharina. 2000. Right node raising and gapping: Interface conditions on prosodic deletion. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Hedier, Antoine, Su, Peijia & Hemforth, Barbara. 2021. Sœurs de même taille: Attachement d’une phrase relative aux syntagmes nominaux coordonnés. Langages 223.3, 125141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, Jong-Bok, Runner, Jeffrey T.. 2022. Pseudogapping in English: A direct interpretation approach. The Linguistic Review 20222094. doi:https://doi.org/10.1515/tlr-2022-2094. (last accessed 2022-12-06).Google Scholar
Kayne, Richard S. 1994. The antisymmetry of syntax. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kehler, Andrew. 2000. Coherence and the resolution of ellipsis. Linguistics and Philosophy 23, 533575.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kertz, Laura. 2013. Verb phrase ellipsis: The view from information structure. Language 89.3, 390428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, Christina S. & Runner, Jeffrey T.. 2018. The division of labor in explanations of verb phrase ellipsis. Linguistics and Philisophy 41, 4185. doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s10988-017-9220-0. (last accessed 2022-12-06).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koenig, Jean-Pierre. 1999. Lexical relations. Stanford, CA: CSLI.Google Scholar
Kuno, Susumo. 1981. The syntax of comparative clauses. Papers from the Seventeenth Regional Meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society 136155.Google Scholar
Labelle, Marie. 2008. The French reflexive and reciprocal se. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 26, 833876.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levine, Robert. 1985. Right node (non-)raising. Linguistic Inquiry 16.3, 492497.Google Scholar
Levine, Robert D. & Hukari, Thomas E.. 2006. The unity of unbounded dependency constructions. Stanford University: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
McCawley, James D. 1988. The syntactic phenomena of English. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Mehler, Jacques. 1963. Some effects of grammatical transformations on the recall of English sentences. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 2.4, 346351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Melis, Ludo. 1990. La voie pronominale. la systématique des tours pronominaux en Français moderne. Louvain la neuve: Duculot.Google Scholar
Merchant, Jason. 2013. Voice and ellipsis. Linguistic Inquiry 44, 77108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, Philip. 1992. Clitics and constituents in phrase structure grammar. New York: Garland.Google Scholar
Miller, Philip. 2014. A corpus study of pseudogapping and its theoretical consequences. In Pinon, Christopher (ed.), Empirical issues in formal syntax and semantics, 7390. Paris: CSSP.Google Scholar
Miller, Philip & Sag, Ivan A.. 1997. French clitic movement without clitics or movement. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 15, 573639.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Müller, Stefan, Abeillé, Anne, Borsley, Robert D. & Koenig, Jean-Pierre (eds.). 2021. Head Driven Phrase Structure Grammar (Empirically Oriented Theoretical Morphology and Syntax 9). Berlin: Language Science Press. doi:10.5281/zenodo.5543318. (last accessed 2022-12-06).Google Scholar
Müller, Stefan. 2001. The passive as a lexical rule. In Kathol, A. & Flickinger, D. (eds.), Proceedings of the 7th HPSG Conference, 247266. Stanford: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
Moltmann, Friederike. 1992. Coordination and comparatives. MIT dissertation.Google Scholar
Paape, Dario, Hemforth, Barbara & Vasishth, Shravan. 2018. Processing of ellipsis with garden-path antecedents in French and German: Evidence from eye tracking. PlosOne 13. doi:https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0198620. (last accessed 2022-12-06).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pollard, Carl J. & Sag, Ivan A.. 1994. Head-driven phrase structure grammar. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Poppels, Till & Kehler, Andrew. 2019. Reconsidering asymmetries in voice-mismatched VP-ellipsis. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics 4.1, 60. doi:https://doi.org/10.5334/gjgl.738. (last accessed 2022-12-06).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Postal, Paul M. 1974. On raising. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Pullum, Geoffrey K. & Zwicky, Arnold M.. 1986. Phonological resolution of syntactic feature conflict. Language 64, 751773.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rooth, Mats. 1992. A theory of focus interpretation. Natural Language Semantics 1, 75116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ross, John Robert. 1967. Constraints on variables in syntax. MIT dissertation.Google Scholar
Sabbagh, Joseph. 2007. Ordering and linearizing rightward movement. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 25, 349401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sabbagh, Joseph. 2014. Right-node raising. Language and Linguistic Compass 8.1, 2435.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sadler, Louisa & Spencer, Andrew. 2001. Syntax as an exponent of morphological features. In Booij, Geert & Van Maarle, Jaap (eds.), Yearbook of morphology 2000, 7196. Dordrecht: Springer.Google Scholar
Sells, Peter, Zaenen, Annie & Zec, Draga. 1987. Reflexivization variation: Relations between syntax, semantics and lexical structure. In Zec, Draga, Iida, Masayo & Wechsler, Stephen (eds.), Working papers in grammatical theory and discourse structure, 169238. Stanford: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
Shiraishi, Aoi, Abeillé, Anne, Hemforth, Barbara & Miller, Philip. 2019. Verbal mismatch in right-node raising. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics 4.1, 114. 126. doi:https://doi.org/10.5334/gjgl.843. (last accessed 2022-12-06).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steedman, Mark. 1996. Surface structure and interpretation. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Thuilier, Juliette, Abeillé, Anne, Crabbé, Benoit & Grant, Margaret. 2020. French word order: The role of animacy. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics 6.1, 55. doi:https://doi.org/10.5334/gjgl.1155. (last accessed 2022-12-06).Google Scholar
Wehrli, Eric. 1986. On some properties of French clitic se. In Borer, Hagit (ed.), The syntax of pronominal clitics, 263283. New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wexler, Kenneth & Culicover, Peter W.. 1980. Formal principles of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Whitman, Philip Neal. 2009. Right-node wrapping. In Hinrichs, Erhard & Nerbonne, John (eds.), Theory and evidence in semantics, 235256. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
Wilder, Chris. 1999. Right-node raising and the LCA. In Bird, Sonya, Carnie, Andrew, Haugen, Jason D. & Norquest, Peter (eds.), Proceedings of the 18th West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics, 586598.Google Scholar
Yatabe, Shûichi. 2001. The syntax and semantics of left-node raising in Japanese. In Flickinger, Dan & Kathol, Andreas (eds.), Proceedings of the 7th International Conference on Head-Driven Phrase Structure Grammar, 325344. Stanford: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
Yatabe, Shûichi. 2012. Comparison of the ellipsis-based theory of non-constituent coordination with its alternatives. In Müller, Stefan (ed.), Proceedings of the 19th International Conference on Head-Driven Phrase Structure Grammar, Chungnam National University Daejeon, 453473.Google Scholar
Zribi-Hertz, Anne & Abeillé, Anne. 2021. La construction passive. In Abeillé, Anne & Godard, Danièle (eds.), La grande grammaire du français, 210222. Arles: Actes Sud.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1 Passive structure from Merchant (2013)

Figure 1

Figure 2 Active structure from Merchant (2013)

Figure 2

Figure 3 Simplified RNR analysis with multidominance

Figure 3

Table 1 RNR with voice mismatch in frTenTen12 corpus.

Figure 4

Figure 4 (Color online) Results of Experiment 1 on VP RNR

Figure 5

Figure 5 Active structure with VoiceP

Figure 6

Figure 6 Passive structure with VoiceP

Figure 7

Figure 7 Reflexive structure with VoiceP

Figure 8

Figure 8 (Color online) Results of Experiment 2 on PP RNR

Figure 9

Figure 9 Analysis of French RNR with voice mismatch