Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gxg78 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T12:41:06.700Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Verbal fluency deficits in Parkinson's disease: A meta-analysis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 July 2004

JULIE D. HENRY
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of Aberdeen, Scotland
JOHN R. CRAWFORD
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of Aberdeen, Scotland

Abstract

A meta-analysis of 68 studies with a total of 4644 participants was conducted to investigate the sensitivity of tests of verbal fluency to the presence of Parkinson's disease (PD) relative to healthy controls. Both phonemic and semantic fluency were moderately impaired but neither deficit qualified as a differential deficit relative to verbal intelligence or psychomotor speed. However, PD patients were significantly more impaired on semantic relative to phonemic fluency (rs = .37 vs. .33, respectively), and confrontation naming, a test of semantic memory that imposes only minimal demands upon cognitive speed and effortful retrieval, was associated with a deficit that was of a comparable magnitude to the deficits upon each of these types of fluency. Thus, the disorder appears to be associated with particular problems with semantic memory. Tests that impose heavy demands upon switching may also be disproportionately affected. Demented and non-demented PD patients differ quantitatively but not qualitatively in terms of the relative prominence of deficits on tests of phonemic and semantic fluency. However, patients with dementia of the Alzheimer's type and demented PD patients can be differentiated from one another by the relative magnitude of deficits upon these two measures. (JINS, 2004, 10, 608–622.)

Type
Research Article
Copyright
2004 The International Neuropsychological Society

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Albert, M. (1978). Subcortical dementia. In R. Katzman, R.D. Terry, & K.L. Bick (Eds.), Alzheimer's disease, senile dementia and related disorders. New York: Raven Press.
American Psychological Association. (2001). Publication Manual of the American Psychological Association (5th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
Auriacombe, S., Grossman, M., Carvell, S., Gollomp, S., Stern, M.B., & Hurtig, H.I. (1993). Verbal fluency deficits in Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychology, 7, 182192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Azuma, T., Bayles, K.A., Cruz, R.E., Tomoeda, C.K., Wood, J.A., McGeagh, A., & Montgomery, E.B. (1997). Comparing the difficulty of letter, semantic, and name fluency tasks for normal elderly and patients with Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychology, 11, 488497.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, R. & Marsden, C. (1988). Internal versus external cues and the control of attention. Brain, 111, 323345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caltagirone, C., Carlesimo, A., Nocentini, U., & Vicari, S. (1989). Defective concept formation in Parkinsonians is independent from mental deterioration. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 52, 334337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chan, A.S., Butters, N., Paulsen, J.S., Salmon, D.P., Swenson, M.R., & Maloney, L.T. (1993). An assessment of the semantic network in patients with Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 254261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, J. (1977). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (Rev. ed.). New York: Academic Press.
Cooper, J.A., Sagar, H.J., Jordan, N., Harvey, N.S., & Sullivan, E.V. (1991). Cognitive impairment in early, untreated Parkinson's disease and its relationship to motor disability. Brain, 114, 20952122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crawford, J.R. & Henry, J.D. (in press). Assessment of executive deficits. In P.W. Halligan & N. Wade (Eds.), The effectiveness of rehabilitation for cognitive deficits. London: Oxford University Press.
Cummings, J.L. (1990). Subcortical dementia. New York: Oxford University Press.
De Long, M.R. & Georgopolis, A.P. (1981). Motor control of the basal ganglia. In J.M. Brookhart, V.B. Mountcastle, & V.B. Brooks (Eds.), Handbook of physiology. Baltimore: American Psychological Society.
Della Sala, S. (1988). Cognitive deficits of Parkinsonians and Occam's Razor. Europa Medicophysica, 24, 122.Google Scholar
Downes, J.J., Sharp, H.M., Costall, B.M., Sagar, H.J., & Howe, J. (1993). Alternating fluency in Parkinson's disease. Brain, 116, 887902.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Easterbrook, P.J., Berlin, J.A., Gopalan, R., & Mathews, D.R. (1991). Publication bias in clinical research. Lancet, 337, 867872.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Field, A.P. (2001). Meta-analysis of correlation coefficients: A Monte Carlo comparison of fixed- and random-effects models. Psychological Methods, 6, 161180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fisher, R.A. (1928). Statistical methods for research workers (2nd ed.). London: Oliver & Boyd.
Flowers, K.A., Robertson, C., & Sheridan, M.R. (1995). Some characteristics of word fluency in Parkinson's disease. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 9, 3346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Folstein, M.F., Folstein, S.E., & McHugh, P.R. (1975). Mini-mental state: A practical method for grading cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatry Research, 12, 189198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gabrieli, J.D.E., Singh, J., Stebbins, G.T., & Goetz, C.G. (1996). Reduced working memory span in Parkinson's disease: Evidence for the role of a frontostriatal system in working and strategic memory. Neuropsychology, 10, 322332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldman, W.P., Baty, J.D., Buckles, V.D., Sahrmann, S., & Morris, J.C. (1998). Cognitive and motor functioning in Parkinson disease: Subjects with and without questionable dementia. Archives of Neurology, 55, 674680.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gotham, A., Brown, R., & Marsden, C. (1988). ‘Frontal’ cognitive function in patients with Parkinson's disease ‘on’ and ‘off’ levodopa. Brain, 111, 299321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gurd, J.M. (1995). Frontal dissociations: Evidence from Parkinson's disease. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 9, 5568.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hanes, K.R., Andrewes, D.G., Smith, D.J., & Pantelis, C. (1996a). A brief assessment of executive control dysfunction: Discriminant validity and homogeneity of planning, set shift, and fluency measures. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 11, 185191.Google Scholar
Hart, S. (1988). Language and dementia: A review. Psychological Medicine, 18, 99112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heaton, R.K. (1981). Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST). Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
Hedges, L. & Olkin, I. (1985). Statistical methods for meta-analysis. New York: Academic Press.
Henry, J.D. & Crawford, J.R. (2004). A meta-analytic review of verbal fluency performance following focal cortical lesions. Neuropsychology, 18, 284295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henry, J.D., Crawford, J.R., & Phillips, L.H. (in press). Verbal fluency performance in dementia of the Alzheimer's type: A meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia.
Hodges, J.R., Salmon, D.P., & Butters, N. (1990). Differential impairment of semantic and episodic memory in Alzheimer's and Huntington's diseases: A controlled prospective study. Journal of Neurology Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 53, 10891095.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoehn, M.M. & Yahr, M.D. (1967). Parkinsonism: Onset, progression and mortality. Neurology, 17, 427442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ivory, S.J., Knight, R.G., Longmore, B.E., & Caradoc-Davies, T. (1999). Verbal memory in non-demented patients with idiopathic Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychologia, 37, 817828.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jastak, S. & Wilkinson, G.S. (1984). Wide Range Achievement Test–Revised. Wilmington, DE: Jastak Assessment Systems.
Kaplan, E.F., Goodglass, H., & Weintraub, S. (1983). The Boston Naming Test. Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger.
Laws, K.R. (1999). A meta-analytic review of Wisconsin Card Sort studies in schizophrenia: General intellectual deficit in disguise? Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 4, 135.Google Scholar
Le Bras, C., Pillon, B., Damier, P., & Dubois, B. (1999). At which steps of spatial working memory processing do striatofrontal circuits intervene in humans? Neuropsychologia, 37, 8390.Google Scholar
Lees, A. & Smith, E. (1983). Cognitive deficits in the early stages of Parkinson's disease. Brain, 106, 257270.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levin, B.E., Llabre, M.M., & Weiner, W.J. (1989). Cognitive impairments associated with early Parkinson's disease. Neurology, 39, 557561.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, F.M., Lapointe, L.L., Murdoch, B.E., & Chenery, H.J. (1998). Language impairment in Parkinson's disease. Aphasiology, 12, 193206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lezak, M.D. (1995). Neuropsychological assessment (3rd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.
Matison, R., Mayeux, R., Rosen, J., & Fahn, S. (1982). “Tip-of-the-tongue” phenomenon in Parkinson disease. Neurology, 32, 567570.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDonald, C., Brown, G.G., & Gorell, J.M. (1996). Impaired set-shifting in Parkinson's disease: New evidence from a lexical decision task. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 18, 793809.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, E. (1984). Verbal fluency as a function of a measure of verbal intelligence and in relation to different types of cerebral pathology. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 23, 5357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, E. (1985). Possible frontal impairments in Parkinson's disease: A test using a measure of verbal fluency. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 24, 211212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miyake, A., Friedman, N.P., Emerson, M.J., Witzki, A.H., & Howerter, A. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
National Research Council. (1992). Combining information: Statistical issues and opportunities for research. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
Nebes, R.D. (1989). Semantic memory in Alzheimer's disease. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 377394.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, H.E. (1982). National Adult Reading Test (NART) Test manual. Windsor, UK: NFER Nelson.
Oyebode, J.R., Barker, W.A., Blessed, G., Dick, D.J., & Britton, P.G. (1986). Cognitive functioning in Parkinson's disease in relation to prevalence of dementia and psychiatric diagnosis. British Journal of Psychiatry, 149, 720725.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perret, E. (1974). The left frontal lobe of man and the suppression of habitual responses in verbal categorical behaviour. Neuropsychologia, 12, 323330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Phillips, L.H. (1997). Do ‘frontal tests’ measure executive function? Issues of assessment and evidence from fluency tests. In P.M.A. Rabbitt (Ed.), Methodology of frontal and executive function (pp. 191213): Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
Piatt, A.L., Fields, J.A., Paolo, A.M., Kollelr, W.C., & Troster, A.I. (1999). Lexical, semantic and action verbal fluency in Parkinson's disease with and without dementia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 21, 435443.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raskin, S.A., Sliwinski, M., & Borod, J.C. (1992a). Clustering strategies on tasks of verbal fluency in Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychologia, 30, 9599.Google Scholar
Raudenbush, S.W. (1994). Random effects models. In H. Cooper & L.V. Hedges (Eds.), Handbook of research synthesis (pp. 301321). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.
Reid, W.G.J., Broe, G.A., Hely, M.A., Morris, J.G.L., Williamson, P.M., O'Sullivan, D.J., Rail, D., Genge, S., & Moss, N.G. (1989). The neuropsychology of de novo patients with idiopathic Parkinson's disease: The effects of age of onset. International Journal of Neuroscience, 48, 205217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenthal, R. (1994). Parametric measures of effect size. In H. Cooper & L.V. Hedges (Eds.), Handbook of research synthesis (pp. 231244). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.
Rosenthal, R. & DiMatteo, M.R. (2001). Meta-analysis: Recent developments in quantitative methods for literature reviews. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 5982.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosser, A. & Hodges, J.R. (1994). Initial letter and semantic category fluency in Alzheimer's disease, Huntington's disease, and progressive supranuclear palsy. Journal of Neurology Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 57, 13891394.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruff, R.M., Light, R.H., Parker, S.B., & Levin, H.S. (1997). The psychological construct of word fluency. Brain and Language, 57, 394405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salthouse, T.A. (1992). What do adult age differences in the Digit Symbl Substitution Test reflect? Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 48, 121128.Google Scholar
Shallice, T. (1988). From neuropsychology to mental structure. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
Snedecor, G.W. & Cochran, W.G. (1989). Statistical methods (8th ed.). Ames, IA: Iowa State University Press.
Stanley, T.D. (2001). Wheat from chaff: Meta-analysis as quantitative literature review. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 15, 131150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Starkstein, S.E., Preziosi, T.J., Berthier, M.L., Bolduc, P.L., Mayberg, H.S., & Robinson, R.G. (1989). Depression and cognitive impairment in Parkinson's disease. Brain, 112, 11411153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strube, M.J., Gardner, W., & Hartmann, D.P. (1985). Limitations, liabilities and obstacles in reviews of the literature: The current status of meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 5, 6378.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stuss, D.T. & Benson, D.F. (1986). The frontal lobes. New York: Raven Press.
Suhr, J.A. & Jones, R.D. (1998). Letter and semantic fluency in Alzheimer's, Huntington's, and Parkinson's dementias. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 13, 447454.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, A., Saint-Cyr, J., & Lang, A. (1986). Frontal lobe dysfunction in Parkinson's disease. Brain, 109, 845883.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Troyer, A.K. & Moscovitch, M. (1996). Clustering and switching on verbal fluency tests: Evidence from healthy controls and patients with Alzheimer's and Parkinson's Disease. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 2, 11.Google Scholar
Troyer, A.K., Moscovitch, M., Winocur, G., Leach, L., & Freedman, M. (1998). Clustering and switching on verbal fluency tests in Alzheimer's and Parkinson's disease. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 4, 137143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tsai, C.H., Lu, C.S., Hua, M.S., Lo, W.L., & Lo, S.K. (1994). Cognitive dysfunction in early onset parkinsonism. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 89, 914.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Spaendonck, K.P.M., Berger, H.J.C., Horstink, M.W.I.M., Buytenhuijs, E.L., & Cools, A.R. (1996). Executive functions and disease characteristics in Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychologia, 34, 617626.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1955). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale manual. New York: The Psychological Corporation.
Wechsler, D. (1981). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised manual. New York: The Psychological Corporation.
Weingartner, H., Burns, S., Diebel, R., & Le Witt, P.A. (1984). Cognitive impairments in Parkinson's disease: Distinguishing between effort-demanding and automatic cognitive processes. Psychiatry Research, 11, 223235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zec, R.F., Landreth, E.S., Fritz, S., Grames, E., Hasara, A., Fraizer, W., Belman, J., Wainman, S., McCool, M., O'Connell, C., Harris, R., Robbs, R., Elble, R., & Manyam, B. (1999). A comparison of phonemic, semantic, and alternating word fluency in Parkinson's disease. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 14, 255264.CrossRefGoogle Scholar