Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-lnqnp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T08:55:25.599Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Longitudinal change in serial position scores in older adults with entorhinal and hippocampal neuropathologies

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 September 2022

Kristina M. Gicas*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, York University, Toronto, Canada
William G. Honer
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, Canada
Sue E. Leurgans
Affiliation:
Department of Neurological Sciences, Rush University Medical Center, Chicago, USA
Robert S. Wilson
Affiliation:
Department of Neurological Sciences, Rush University Medical Center, Chicago, USA Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Rush University Medical Center, Chicago, USA
Patricia A. Boyle
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Rush University Medical Center, Chicago, USA
Julie A. Schneider
Affiliation:
Department of Pathology, Rush University Medical Center, Chicago, USA
David A. Bennett
Affiliation:
Department of Neurological Sciences, Rush University Medical Center, Chicago, USA
*
Corresponding author: Kristina Gicas, email: kgicas@yorku.ca
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective:

Serial position scores on verbal memory tests are sensitive to early Alzheimer’s disease (AD)-related neuropathological changes that occur in the entorhinal cortex and hippocampus. The current study examines longitudinal change in serial position scores as markers of subtle cognitive decline in older adults who may be in preclinical or at-risk states for AD.

Methods:

This study uses longitudinal data from the Religious Orders Study and the Rush Memory and Aging Project. Participants (n = 141) were included if they did not have dementia at enrollment, completed follow-up assessments, and died and were classified as Braak stage I or II. Memory tests were used to calculate serial position (primacy, recency), total recall, and episodic memory composite scores. A neuropathological evaluation quantified AD, vascular, and Lewy body pathologies. Mixed effects models were used to examine change in memory scores. Neuropathologies and covariates (age, sex, education, APOE e4) were examined as moderators.

Results:

Primacy scores declined (β = −.032, p < .001), whereas recency scores increased (β = .021, p = .012). No change was observed in standard memory measures. Greater neurofibrillary tangle density and atherosclerosis explained 10.4% of the variance in primacy decline. Neuropathologies were not associated with recency change.

Conclusions:

In older adults with hippocampal neuropathologies, primacy score decline may be a sensitive marker of early AD-related changes. Tangle density and atherosclerosis had additive effects on decline. Recency improvement may reflect a compensatory mechanism. Monitoring for changes in serial position scores may be a useful in vivo method of tracking incipient AD.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © INS. Published by Cambridge University Press, 2022

Introduction

Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is defined pathologically by the presence of beta-amyloid and neurofibrillary tau tangles and is a major contributor to dementia (DeTure & Dickson, Reference DeTure and Dickson2019; Matthews et al., Reference Matthews, Brayne, Lowe, McKeith, Wharton and Ince2009). Clinical dementia due to AD, however, is the endpoint manifestation of pathological progression that unfolds over decades (Dickerson et al., Reference Dickerson, Stoub, Shah, Sperling, Killiany, Albert, Hyman, Blacker and Detoledo-Morrell2011; Villemagne et al., Reference Villemagne, Burnham, Bourgeat, Brown, Ellis and Salvado2013). Advances in the detection of amyloid and tau biomarkers in vivo now provide further support for the presence of AD pathology years before cognitive impairment is detectable (Tanner & Rabinovici, Reference Tanner and Rabinovici2021; Younes et al., Reference Younes, Albert, Moghekar, Soldan, Pettigrew and Miller2019). Yet, it remains challenging to differentiate cognitively normal persons who are in a preclinical state of AD. It is increasingly recognized that AD pathologies may lie on a continuum where progression is associated with a multitude of factors including age, APOE e4 status, cognitive reserve, genetics, and comorbidities (Dubois et al., Reference Dubois, Hampel, Feldman, Scheltens, Aisen, Andrieu, Bakardjian, Benali, Bertrm, Blennow, Broich, Cavedo, Crutch, Dartigues, Duyckaerts, Epelbaum, Frisoni, Gauthier, Genthon, Gouw, Habert, Holtzman, Kivipelto, Lista, Molinuevo, O'Bryant, Rabinovici, Rowe, Salloway, Schneider, Sperling, Teichmann, Carrillo, Cummings and Jack2016).

The entorhinal cortex and hippocampus are among the earliest regions to be affected by AD pathology, followed by a predictable pattern of disease progression extending to the neocortical regions of the temporal lobe and then more diffusely to frontal cortical regions (Braak & Del Tredici, Reference Braak and Del Tredici2015, Reference Braak and Del Tredici2020). The emergence of cognitive impairments track with disease progression such that the medial temporal lobe-dependent memory functions are among the first to decline and remain one of the best predictors of a later dementia diagnosis (Mortamais et al., Reference Mortamais, Ash, Harrison, Kaye, Kramer, Randolph, Pose, Albala, Ropacki, Ritchie and Ritchie2017). However, in the preclinical stage of AD, composite memory scores do not yet meet the threshold for clinical impairment, nor do they show any significant association with hippocampal structure (Toledo et al., Reference Toledo, Bjerke, Chen, Rozycki, Jack, Weiner, Arnold, Reiman, Davatzikos, Shaw and Trojanowski2015). The routine cognitive scores that are relied upon to diagnose mild cognitive impairment (MCI) or dementia therefore lack sensitivity to detect more subtle cognitive changes that may take place in the at-risk or preclinical states when AD pathology is limited to the entorhinal cortex and hippocampus.

Upon closer inspection, there is ample evidence to support subtle cognitive changes in preclinical AD. Meta-analytic work has revealed small neuropsychological differences by biomarker status in cognitively normal older adults, with successive memory decrements noted for amyloid positive persons and those who are amyloid positive with tau pathology or neurodegeneration (Han et al., Reference Han, Nguyen, Stricker and Nation2017). Thomas et al. (Reference Thomas, Edmonds, Eppig, Salmon and Bondi2018) have operationalized early subtle cognitive decline to identify persons with impairment on process scores (emphasizing how information was processed) who are classified as “cognitively normal” based on their routine total or composite cognitive test scores. Persons who met criteria for subtle cognitive decline showed faster amyloid accumulation and entorhinal cortical thinning compared to cognitively normal persons, whereas the MCI group showed faster hippocampal atrophy (Thomas et al., Reference Thomas, Bangen, Weigand, Edmonds, Wong, Cooper, Delano-Wood and Bond2020). These findings suggest that subtle cognitive changes may track with early neuropathological changes in preclinical AD.

In line with the process score approach, serial position scores in verbal memory may be particularly sensitive to early neuropathological changes in the medial temporal lobe region. Serial position effects refer to the tendency to better remember items at the beginning (primacy) and end (recency) of a supraspan wordlist (Murdock, Reference Murdock1962). It is proposed that primacy items benefit from a greater opportunity for rehearsal and therefore become hippocampal-dependent as they are transferred to and retrieved from long-term memory stores, whereas recency items are held in and retrieved directly from short-term working memory stores (Atkinson & Shiffrin, Reference Atkinson and Shiffrin1968; Malmberg et al., Reference Malmberg, Raaijmakers and Shiffrin2019). Anatomically, this explanation is supported by functional neuroimaging that shows differential activation of hippocampal and frontoparietal regions when recalling primacy versus recency items, respectively (Talmi et al., Reference Talmi, Grady, Goshen-Gottstein and Moscovitch2005), as well as structural studies linking poorer primacy recall to hippocampal lesions and volume reductions (Bruno et al., Reference Bruno, Grothe, Nierenberg, Zetterberg, Blennow, Teipel and Pomara2015; Chander et al., Reference Chander, Foo, Yong, Lim, Tan, Wen, Ng, Hameed, Ting, Zhou and Kandiah2018; Hermann et al., Reference Hermann, Seidenberg, Wyler, Davies, Christeson, Moran and Stroup1996). Clinically, the serial position effect is altered in Alzheimer’s dementia, characterized by a diminished primacy effect with a relatively more pronounced recency effect (Bayley et al., Reference Bayley, Salmon, Bondi, Bui, Olichney, Delis, Thomas and Thal2000; Cunha et al., Reference Cunha, Guerreiro, de Mendonça, Oliveira and Santana2012; Howieson et al., Reference Howieson, Mattek, Seeyle, Dodge, Wasserman, Zitzelberger and Kaye2011). Primacy scores have predicted transition to MCI and to Alzheimer’s dementia with intervals from 18 months up to 13 years (Egli et al., Reference Egli, Beck, Berres, Foldi, Monsch and Sollberger2014; Talamonti et al., Reference Talamonti, Koscik, Johnson and Bruno2019). Another serial position-derived memory score, the recency ratio, has shown association with cerebrospinal fluid markers of amyloid beta in persons with MCI who were considered at risk for AD (Bruno et al., Reference Bruno, Gleason, Koscik, Pomara, Zetterberg, Blennow and Johnson2019). These findings extend to verbal material presented in a story format, whereby primacy scores, but not total recall scores, predicted PET global amyloid burden in individuals without cognitive impairment (Bruno et al., Reference Bruno, Mueller, Betthauser, Chin, Engelman, Christian, Koscik and Johnson2021).

Understanding how early and subtle memory changes track with AD pathologies could be critical to early detection of disease, with implications for clinical trials eligibility and behavioural interventions. We previously reported an association between primacy performance measured years before death and an increasing burden of hippocampal neuropathologies using gold-standard post-mortem evaluations (Gicas et al., Reference Gicas, Honer, Wilson, Boyle, Leurgans, Schneider and Bennett2020). We also found that primacy performance discriminated between clinical diagnoses proximate to death, providing robust support for the sensitivity of serial position scores to AD-related changes in the brain. The current study extends our prior work in two important ways. First, we are examining serial position scores longitudinally to identify subtle decline in the absence of decline on routine total recall and composite memory scores in older adults who did not have dementia at study enrollment. Second, we are focusing on a subsample of older adults who may be in the preclinical stage of AD based on autopsy evidence of tau pathology circumscribed to the entorhinal cortex and hippocampus, consistent with Braak stages I and II (Braak et al., Reference Braak, Alafuzoff, Arzberger, Kretzschmar and Del Tredici2006). This approach intentionally excludes persons who may demonstrate more objective memory changes associated with MCI and dementia, which typically coincide with Braak stage III when neurofibrillary tangles expand to the neocortex (Nelson et al., Reference Nelson, Alafuzoff, Bigio, Bouras, Braak, Cairns, Castellani, Crain, Davies, Del Tredici, Duyckaerts, Frosch, Haroutunian, Hof, Hulette, Hyman, Iwatsubo, Jellinger, Jicha, Kövari, Kukull, Leverenz, Love, Mackenzie, Mann, Masliah, McKee, Montine, Morris, Schneider, Sonnen, Thal, Trojanowski, Troncoso, Wisniewski, Woltjer and Beach2012). It is hypothesized that a select decline in the primacy score, but not recency or total recall scores, will be observed. A secondary aim is to investigate the role of specific AD pathologies in serial position score change over time. It is hypothesized that tangle density, but not neuritic plaques, will be associated with a steeper rate of primacy score change. This follows from observations that tau tangles may be the primary drivers of cognitive changes in Alzheimer’s dementia (Arnsten et al., Reference Arnsten, Datta, Del Tredici and Braak2021; Nelson et al., Reference Nelson, Alafuzoff, Bigio, Bouras, Braak, Cairns, Castellani, Crain, Davies, Del Tredici, Duyckaerts, Frosch, Haroutunian, Hof, Hulette, Hyman, Iwatsubo, Jellinger, Jicha, Kövari, Kukull, Leverenz, Love, Mackenzie, Mann, Masliah, McKee, Montine, Morris, Schneider, Sonnen, Thal, Trojanowski, Troncoso, Wisniewski, Woltjer and Beach2012). Given that age-related cognitive changes and dementia are typically the result of mixed brain pathologies, we also explored the role of commonly co-occurring vascular pathologies and Lewy bodies in relation to serial position trajectories.

Method

Participants

This study used data from two parallel longitudinal clinical-pathologic cohort studies; the Religious Orders Study (ROS) and the Rush Memory and Aging Project (MAP; see overview of studies in Bennett et al., Reference Bennett, Buchman, Boyle, Barnes, Wilson and Schneider2018). Participants enrolled in ROS were older nuns, priests, and brothers recruited from multiple regions across the United States beginning in 1994. MAP participants included community-dwelling older adults who were recruited from retirement facilities, churches, and social service agencies in Northeastern Illinois beginning in 1997. Eligibility for enrollment in ROS or MAP included consent to participant in annual assessments and signing an Anatomical Gift Act for organ donation at death. All participants signed a Repository Consent to allow their data to be shared. Ethics approvals for ROS and MAP were obtained from an Institutional Review Board of Rush University Medical Center. This research was completed in accordance with the Helsinki Declaration.

A total of 3678 participants enrolled in ROS and MAP completed a baseline cognitive evaluation at the time of the database curation (March 3, 2021). Eligibility for these analyses include (1) no dementia at study enrollment; (2) at least one follow-up evaluation, a complete CERAD (Consortium to Establish a Registry for AD) Word List Memory test; (3) a full neuropathological evaluation; (4) Braak stage I or II; (5) TDP-43 Stage 0 or 1; (6) no hippocampal sclerosis; and (7) no or low likelihood of pathologic AD based on the modified National Institute of Aging (NIA)-Reagan criteria (Bennett et al., Reference Bennett, Schneider, Arvanitakis, Kelly, Aggarwal, Shah and Wilson2006a; The National Institute on Aging, and Reagan Institute Working Group on Diagnostic Criteria for the Neuropathological Assessment of Alzheimer’s Disease, 1997). We excluded cases classified as being in advanced TDP-43 stages (i.e., inclusions in the hippocampus and neocortical regions) and/or with hippocampal sclerosis as these neuropathologies are known to impact episodic memory (Brenowitz et al., Reference Brenowitz, Monsell, Schmitt, Kukull and Nelson2015; Nag et al., Reference Nag, Yu, Wilson, Chen, Bennett and Schneider2017), and we wished to more clearly evaluate the specific impacts of early stage AD pathology circumscribed to the entorhinal cortex and hippocampus. The flow chart in Figure 1 delineates the sample composition based on these criteria. The final analytic sample consisted of 141 participants who were on average 76.7 years old (SD = 6.7; range = 64.5–95.7) at study entry, with a mean of 17.1 years of education (SD = 3.6; range = 10–28); 51.1% were female, 96.5% were white, and 12.9% had at least one APOE ϵ4 allele. Participants were followed for a mean of 8.2 years (SD = 4.5, range = 1.0–22.1) and died at a mean age of 84.9 (SD = 6.7; range = 69.7–101.0).

Figure 1. Flow chart delineating participant inclusion and exclusion.

Cognitive evaluation

Participants completed an annual neuropsychological evaluation in a standardized format involving a comprehensive test battery to assess functioning in the domains of episodic memory, semantic memory, working memory, visuospatial ability, and perceptual speed (Wilson et al., Reference Wilson, Yang, Yu, Leurgans, Capuano, Schneider, Bennett and Boyle2019). For the current study, serial position scores were computed for each annual evaluation using the CERAD Word List Memory test (Morris et al., Reference Morris, Heyman, Mohs, Hughes, van Belle, Fillenbaum, Mellits and Clark1989). This test is composed of a list of 10 semantically unrelated words that are repeated across three trials with varying word order. Participants are asked to immediately recall as many words as they can after each trial. Following a several minute delay, participants are asked to recall as many list words as they can (CERAD Word List Recall). Within each of the three immediate recall trials, the first three words comprise the primacy region and the last three words comprise the recency region. Serial position scores were computed as the proportion correctly recalled for each region (possible range = 0–1) by summing the total number of correct responses in a given region across the three trials divided by the total number of words presented in that region (i.e., primacy words = three per trial = nine total; (Moser et al., Reference Moser, Deisenhammer, Marksteiner, Papousek, Fink and Weiss2014)). An immediate recall total score was computed by summing the number of correctly recalled words across the three trials. A delayed recall total score was defined as the total number of words recalled on the delay trial. Raw scores were converted to Z-score units using the baseline mean and standard deviation of the full combined sample. To provide a broader index of episodic memory, a composite score was derived by averaging the Z-scores (using baseline mean and standard deviation) from the CERAD Word List Memory, Word List Recall, and Word List Recognition tests; Logical Memory A immediate and delayed recall (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1987); and the East Boston Story immediate and delayed recall (Albert et al., Reference Albert, Smith, Scherr, Taylor, Evans and Funkenstein1991). A composite score was only calculated if at least half of the individual scores were non-missing.

A clinical diagnosis was rendered for each annual visit classifying individuals as either having: (1) no cognitive impairment; (2) MCI; (3) Alzheimer’s dementia; and/or (4) other dementia (no clinical evidence of Alzheimer’s dementia). Classification was based on a three-stage system that involved computer scoring of the completed neuropsychological test battery, clinical judgment by a neuropsychologist blinded to participant demographics, and diagnostic classification by a clinician (neurologist, geriatrician, or geriatric nurse practitioner) after review of all available information. Details of the diagnostic procedures were previously reported (Bennett et al., Reference Bennett, Wilson, Schneider, Evans, Beckett, Aggarwal, Barnes, Fox and Bach2002; Bennett et al., Reference Bennett, Schneider, Arvanitakis, Kelly, Aggarwal, Shah and Wilson2006a, Reference Bennett, Schneider, Aggarwal, Arvanitakis, Shah, Kelly, Fox, Cochran, Arends, Treinkman and Wilson2006b). Diagnoses of MCI and dementia were based on criteria outlined by the National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke and the Alzheimer’s Disease and Related Disorders Association (NINCDS/ADRDA; McKhann et al., Reference McKhann, Drachman, Folstein, Katzman, Price and Stadlan1984).

Clinical diagnosis at the time of death was made by a neurologist using all available clinical data while blinded to postmortem data. Ambiguous cases were adjudicated. Classification of the most likely clinical cognitive diagnosis at death included MCI (one impaired domain) or dementia and the respective cause, as outlined above. Additionally, Alzheimer’s dementia diagnoses were classified as possible or probable in accord with NINCDS/ADRDA criteria (McKhann et al., Reference McKhann, Drachman, Folstein, Katzman, Price and Stadlan1984). Individuals with no impaired domains were classified as having no cognitive impairment.

Neuropathological evaluation

Participants who died underwent brain autopsies, with full details reported elsewhere (Bennett et al., Reference Bennett, Schneider, Arvanitakis, Kelly, Aggarwal, Shah and Wilson2006a). The standard protocol involved removal of the brainstem and cerebellum, followed by cutting one hemisphere into 1cm coronal slabs that were immediately frozen. The other hemisphere was fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde for 3 to 21 days and subsequently cut into 1 cm coronal slabs. Regional blocks of tissue were embedded in paraffin, sliced into 6 μm sections, and mounted to glass slides for microscopic evaluation by a neuropathologist blinded to all clinical information.

Bielschowsky silver stain was used to visualize neurofibrillary tangles and neuritic plaques in the frontal, temporal, parietal, entorhinal, and hippocampal regions. Braak staging was performed by assessing the distribution and severity of neurofibrillary tangles across the five regions. Relevant to the current study, Braak stages I and II were defined as having neurofibrillary tangles confined predominately to the entorhinal cortex, with some limited involvement of the hippocampal CA1/CA2 subfields (Braak et al., Reference Braak, Alafuzoff, Arzberger, Kretzschmar and Del Tredici2006). Quantitative measures of neurofibrillary tangle and neuritic plaque burden were each derived by a count of pathologies in five regions (midfrontal, midtemporal, inferior parietal, entorhinal, and hippocampus) divided by the corresponding standard deviation, and then averaged across regions to yield a summary measure. Data must be present for at least two of the five regions to compute a summary burden score.

Immunocytochemistry and image analysis were used to quantify the percentage of cortex occupied by amyloid beta within eight regions (superior frontal, anterior cingulate, calcarine, angular gyrus, inferior temporal, midfrontal, entorhinal, and hippocampus). A global amyloid level measure was derived by averaging values across the eight regions, with at least four or more regional measures required for calculation. Cerebral amyloid angiopathy (CAA) was assessed with immunostaining in four regions (midfrontal, midtemporal, parietal, and calcarine) and quantified using the protocol outlined by Love et al. (Reference Love, Chalmers, Ince, Esiri, Attems, Jellinger, Yamada, McCarron, Minett, Matthews, Greenberg, Mann and Kehoe2014) based on amyloid deposition in meningeal and parenchymal vessels, scored from 0 to 4 and averaged across regions (Boyle et al., Reference Boyle, Yu, Nag, Leurgans, Wilson, Bennett and Schneider2015). Additional immunocytochemistry using an antibody detecting abnormally phosphorylated tau protein was performed on the eight regions noted above to quantify neurofibrillary tangle density (mm2) by stereology. The overall mean density was calculated by averaging across regions where data were presented for at least four of the regions. TDP-43 staging was performed via immunocytochemistry in eight brain regions (amygdala, entorhinal, hippocampus CA1, hippocampus dentate gyrus, anterior temporal pole, midtemporal, orbital frontal, and midfrontal). The presence of TDP-43 cytoplasmic inclusions in neurons and glia in each region were evaluated and successively classified as follows: stage 1 = none; stage 2 = amygdala; stage 3 = amygdala and limbic; stage 4 = amygdala, limbic, and neocortical (Nag et al., Reference Nag, Yu, Boyle, Leurgans, Bennett and Schneider2018). Cortical Lewy body disease was diagnosed using α-synuclein immunostaining to identify limbic-type and neocortical-type intracytoplasmic structures.

Cerebral atherosclerosis ratings of large vessels were made by visual inspection of the Circle of Willis. Severity was graded based on the number of arteries involved and the extent of each artery involvement, collapsed into four levels: 0 = none or possible; 1 = mild (typically less than 25% artery involvement, without significant occlusion); 2 = moderate (up to half of visualized arteries, less than 50% occlusion of any single artery); 3 = severe (more than half of visualized arteries and/or more than 75% occlusion of one or more arteries) (Arvanitakis et al., Reference Arvanitakis, Capuano, Leurgans, Buchman, Bennett and Schneider2017). Arteriolosclerosis was rated based on histological changes to small vessels observed in the anterior basal ganglia using a semiquantitative rating system collapsed to four levels: 0 = none; 1 = mild; 2 = moderate; 3 = severe) (Buchman et al., Reference Buchman, Leurgans, Nag, Bennett and Schneider2011). Presence of one or more gross chronic cerebral infarcts and one or more chronic microinfarcts were identified via visual inspection by a neuropathologist blinded to clinical data (Arvanitakis et al., Reference Arvanitakis, Leurgans, Barnes, Bennett and Schneider2011). Hippocampal sclerosis was examined in a unilateral coronal section of the midhippocampus and deemed present if there was evidence of severe neuronal loss and gliosis in CA1 and/or the subiculum (Nag et al., Reference Nag, Yu, Capuano, Wilson, Leurgans, Bennett and Schneider2015). Apolipoprotein (APOE) genotyping was completed using DNA from blood samples or postmortem brain tissue. For the current study, APOE ϵ4 positive status was defined as ϵ2/4, ϵ3/4 or ϵ4/4.

Statistical analysis

Data were examined for normality using visual inspection of histograms for continuous variables. Paired-samples t-tests were used to evaluate mean differences in serial position scores at the first and last available cognitive evaluations. Pearson correlations were used to examine linear relationships between continuous variables. To test the hypothesis that primacy scores would show select decline, a series of linear mixed effects models with random intercepts (for subjects) and slopes (for time) were conducted. Serial position scores (primacy, recency), as well as total scores (immediate, delayed) and the episodic memory composite score, were entered as repeated measures outcomes in separate models. Time was measured in years from the baseline evaluation. Covariates included age at baseline, sex (male, female), total years of education, and APOE ϵ4 status. All possible interactions between time and covariates were examined. A sex by education interaction was also examined to capture the intersection of these factors that contribute to gender identity. Only interactions that were significant at p < .05 were retained for inclusion in the final reported models. The assumptions of normality of residuals and random effects were evaluated.

A follow-up series of linear mixed effects models were conducted to screen for potential pathological moderators of primacy and recency trajectories. AD-related pathologies (each of neurofibrillary tangle density, neuritic plaques, beta-amyloid level), vascular pathologies (each of atherosclerosis, arteriolosclerosis, CAA, gross chronic infarcts, chronic microinfarcts), neocortical Lewy bodies, and early stage TDP-43 were examined independently in a total of 20 separate models (ten for each primacy and recency outcome), adjusted for the aforementioned covariates, including the age by sex interaction term for the recency model only. Square root transformations were applied to measures of neurofibrillary tangle density and beta-amyloid to correct for non-normality. All other neuropathologies were dichotomized to reflect presence or absence. Interaction terms (neuropathology*time) that were significant at p < .10 were retained for inclusion in the final moderation model. A more liberal alpha level was adopted at the screening step to ensure inclusion of the broadest range of pathologies in the final model, recognizing that some of these may exert effects in the presence of other pathologies.

A sensitivity analysis was conducted by re-running the main analyses after removing participants who met criteria for MCI across the first three visits. This was done to ensure that participants with MCI were not primarily accounting for any observed changes in memory. The Pseudo-R 2 was used to describe effect sizes of moderators by quantifying the proportion of variance explained in the random effects. Effect sizes are interpreted as small (0.02), medium (0.13), and large (0.26) based on Cohen’s squared multiple correlation change. The alpha level was set to .05. Analyses were conducted in R (R Core Team, 2020) using the lme4 package (Bates et al., Reference Bates, Mächler, Bolker and Walker2015).

Results

Participants were excluded from the analyses if they were missing data on neuropathological staging variables required for classification of early stage AD (see Figure 1). In the final sample (n = 141), one participant was missing data on APOE ϵ4 status and was excluded from the longitudinal analyses. At baseline, 83.7% (n = 118) of the sample included participants who were considered cognitively unimpaired and 16.3% (n = 23) of participants had MCI. Based on cognitive status most proximate to death, 61.7% (n = 87) of participants were cognitively unimpaired, 27.0% (n = 38) of participants had MCI, and 11.3% (n = 16) of participants had dementia. A descriptive summary of the distribution of the pathologies is provided in Table 1.

Table 1. Descriptive statistics of the distribution of neuropathologies

There was a weak correlation between primacy and recency scores at baseline (r = .17, p = .041) and a moderate correlation between scores at the last cognitive evaluation prior to death (r = .27, p = .002). The serial position profiles at baseline and at the last evaluation are depicted in Figure 2, stratified by clinical diagnosis (cognitive impairment [MCI or dementia] vs. no cognitive impairment [NCI]). Paired samples t-tests of baseline scores indicate that the middle score was lower compared to the primacy and recency scores in both the NCI (primacy: t = 8.39, p < .001; recency: t = -6.65, p < .001) and MCI (primacy: t = 3.10, p = .005; recency: t = -2.82, p = .010) groups, whereas there was no difference between mean primacy and recency scores in either group (p-values > .280). When the serial position profiles were examined based on the last scores, the pattern for the NCI group was similar to baseline; the middle score was lower compared to the primacy (t = 4.51, p < .001) and recency (t = -5.25, p < .001) scores, with no difference between mean primacy and recency scores (t = -0.87, p = .386). By contrast, for the group with MCI or dementia, the pattern differed from baseline; the primacy score (t = -2.19, p = .034) and middle score (t = -5.41, p < .001) were both significantly lower compared to the recency score.

Figure 2. Serial position profiles for the baseline cognitive evaluation (left panel) and last available cognitive evaluation (right panel) stratified by cognitive status (NCI = not cognitive impaired; MCI = cognitively impaired; DEM = dementia). Scores represent the proportion of words recalled in each of the primacy, middle, and recency regions. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals.

The results of the linear mixed effects models examining cognitive trajectories are reported in Table 2. After adjusting for the effects of demographics and APOE ϵ4 status, a decline in primacy scores was observed. By contrast, an increase in recency scores was observed over the same time period. None of the covariates significantly interacted with time and therefore these interaction terms were removed from the models. A sex by education interaction was evident only for the recency model whereby the association between higher years of education and better recency performance was stronger for females compared to males. No changes in immediate or delayed total recall, or episodic memory composite scores were observed.

Table 2. Mixed effects models for trajectories of change in cognitive outcome

Note. n = 140 for all models. APOE = apolipoprotein E.

The Sex*Education interaction term was only significant for Model 2, and therefore it was not included in the other models.

In follow-up mixed effects models examining neuropathological moderators of primacy score trajectories, tangle density and atherosclerosis were retained for inclusion in the final model (see Table 3). Higher tangle density and the presence of atherosclerosis were independently associated with greater decline in primacy scores (see Figures 3 and 4). Probing of the interaction terms indicated that change in primacy scores was significant at the mean (0.78; β = −.031, SE = .009, p < .001) and one SD above the mean (1.24; β = −.047, SE = .012, p < .001) for tangle density. Change in primacy scores was also significant in the presence of atherosclerosis (β = −.043, SE = .010, p <. 001), but not in its absence. The addition of these neuropathologies to the model and their respective interactions with time explained 10.4% of the variance in primacy score change over time, corresponding to a small-to-medium effect size. A post hoc 3-way interaction between tangles, atherosclerosis, and time was conducted to determine if pathologies interacted to accelerate decline in primacy scores, but this was not statistically significant and therefore excluded from the final model. Recency score trajectories were not moderated by any of the neuropathologies (see Supplementary Table 1 for results of all moderation analyses). Results of the sensitivity analyses largely remained unchanged from the main analyses. In a subsample of 80 participants who were considered free from cognitive impairment across the first three visits, primacy scores declined (β = −.046, SE = .012, p = .001) and recency scores increased over time (β = .026, SE = .009, p = .010). No other significant changes in total or composite memory scores were observed. In the final moderation model, only atherosclerosis (β = −.064, SE = .028, p = .027) but not tangle density (p =.271) moderated the primacy score trajectories.

Table 3. Final mixed effects moderation model for primacy score trajectories

Note. n = 140. Number of observations = 1094; APOE = apolipoprotein E.

Figure 3. Change in primacy scores over time as a function of neurofibrillary tangle density (mean +/- 1SD).

Figure 4. Change in primacy scores over time as a function of atherosclerosis (present or absent).

Discussion

In a sample of older adults who were free of dementia at study enrollment and who later showed evidence of limited, early-stage entorhinal and hippocampal Alzheimer’s pathologies at death, we observed alterations in serial position scores over a period of up to 22 years. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to examine longitudinal change in serial position scores. As hypothesized, the primacy score showed significant decline over time, whereas total recall and composite memory scores did not show any substantial change. An unexpected increase in recency scores was noted and may reflect compensatory mechanisms or practice effects. Consistent with our prediction, greater neurofibrillary tangle density was associated with faster primacy score decline. The presence of atherosclerosis was also independently associated with faster primacy score decline, indicating potential additive effects of vascular disease on subtle cognitive decline.

The finding of selective primacy score decline complements existing literature that indicates alterations in serial position effects, mostly primacy performance, can predict future decline in global cognitive functioning among older adults who are considered cognitively normal (Bruno et al., Reference Bruno, Reiss, Petkova, Sidtis and Pomara2013, Reference Bruno, Reichert and Pomara2016), those with MCI (Egli et al., Reference Egli, Hirni, Taylor, Berres, Regeniter, Gass, Monsch and Sollberger2015), and those with probable AD (Marra et al., Reference Marra, Silveri and Gainotti2000). Reduced primacy performance has also been linked with subsequent decline in subjective and performance-based functional measures of daily living in older adults without dementia (Weitzner et al., Reference Weitzner, Divers and Calamia2021), providing convergent evidence for the utility of primacy performance in predicting clinically relevant functional changes. Importantly, the primacy score appears to be uniquely suited to differentiating early stage (i.e., MCI (Cunha et al., Reference Cunha, Guerreiro, de Mendonça, Oliveira and Santana2012) or at-risk for AD (La Rue et al., Reference La Rue, Hermann, Jones, Johnson, Asthana and Sager2008)) groups compared to healthy controls. By contrast, the effect sizes from meta-analytic work suggest that immediate recall (g = -2.29) and delayed recall (g = -2.79) scores better differentiate Alzheimer’s dementia from healthy persons compared to the primacy score (g = -1.61; (Weitzner & Calamia, Reference Weitzner and Calamia2020)). Our current findings support the notion that in the earliest stages of AD, it is likely that primacy performance is especially vulnerable to an accumulating burden of neurofibrillary tangles in the entorhinal and hippocampal region that is critical for the encoding of primacy list words in long-term memory stores; however, the overall level of memory performance may be otherwise maintained for a period of time through compensation that involves recruitment of alternate networks (Colangeli et al., Reference Colangeli, Boccia, Verde, Guariglia, Bianchini and Piccardi2016; Scheller et al., Reference Scheller, Minkova, Leitner and Klöppel2014; see further discussion below). This so-called maintenance period may in fact reflect a very protracted phase of relatively “silent” pathophysiological changes, which is supported by evidence for significant time gaps between detection of cerebrospinal fluid total tau level and routine memory score changepoints (∼19–23 years) prior to dementia onset (Younes et al., Reference Younes, Albert, Moghekar, Soldan, Pettigrew and Miller2019).

The unexpected increase of the recency score is notable, and if this finding is replicated in the context of primacy score decline, it may explain the absence of early change in total recall and composite memory scores as being masked by opposing process score effects. While most studies report a general preservation of the recency in the context of MCI and Alzheimer’s dementia (Weitzner & Calamia, Reference Weitzner and Calamia2020), an increased recency effect seems somewhat counterintuitive. One possibility is this reflects a practice effect that may be apparent given participants did not meet criteria for dementia at study enrolment. An alternative explanation is that this increase may reflect a compensatory mechanism by which individuals who are facing progressively greater memory encoding difficulties due to hippocampal dysfunction may engage a learning strategy that relies on short-term memory to maintain performance (Greene et al., Reference Greene, Baddeley and Hodges1996). This idea is best captured by Bruno et al. (Reference Bruno, Reichert and Pomara2016, Reference Bruno, Koscik, Woodard, Pomara and Johnson2018) who developed the recency ratio to demonstrate that individuals with Alzheimer’s dementia show an overreliance on recency recall during immediate recall trials, but this effect is lost upon delayed recall because recency items were previously accessed solely from working memory stores. Only two other aging studies have reported findings that support this interpretation whereby there was better recency recall in persons with MCI and dementia (Jones et al., Reference Jones, Greer and Cox2011) compared to persons without dementia. While there is ample neuroimaging evidence to support functional compensation in older adults (Colangeli et al., Reference Colangeli, Boccia, Verde, Guariglia, Bianchini and Piccardi2016; Scheller et al., Reference Scheller, Minkova, Leitner and Klöppel2014), more work is needed comparing the functional dynamics of primacy and recency performance in vivo between healthy and clinical groups.

The moderating role of pathologies on primacy decline helps elucidate the differential contributions that AD and vascular disease have to cognitive impairment. We found an increasing density of neurofibrillary tangles in the brain was associated with a steeper rate of primacy decline, but no such association was observed for neuritic plaques or global amyloid burden. Our findings complement PET and autopsy studies that show tau pathology is more strongly associated with cognitive functioning than amyloid pathology (Buckley et al., Reference Buckley, Hanseeuw, Schultz, Vannini, Aghjayan, Properzi, Jackson, Mormino, Rentz, Sperling, Johnson and Amariglio2017; Chen et al., Reference Chen, Cassady, Adams, Harrison, Baker and Jagust2021; Nelson et al., Reference Nelson, Alafuzoff, Bigio, Bouras, Braak, Cairns, Castellani, Crain, Davies, Del Tredici, Duyckaerts, Frosch, Haroutunian, Hof, Hulette, Hyman, Iwatsubo, Jellinger, Jicha, Kövari, Kukull, Leverenz, Love, Mackenzie, Mann, Masliah, McKee, Montine, Morris, Schneider, Sonnen, Thal, Trojanowski, Troncoso, Wisniewski, Woltjer and Beach2012), and that tau pathology may be more sensitive to cognitive changes in preclinical AD (Ossenkoppele et al., Reference Ossenkoppele, Smith, Ohlsson, Strandberg, Mattsson, Insel, Palmqvist and Hansson2019).

It is unclear why the presence of atherosclerosis, but not other vascular pathologies, was related to primacy decline. Previous work in ROS/MAP found that the effects of AD pathology and atherosclerosis on cognition may emerge earlier compared to other age-related neuropathologies (Boyle et al., Reference Boyle, Yang, Yu, Leurgans, Capuano, Schneider, Wilson and Bennett2017). Cerebrovascular disease, however, is remarkably heterogeneous in terms of its pathological distribution and therefore its relationship with clinical outcomes is likely dependent upon on the unique ways in which focal lesions perturb structural and functional networks (ter Telgte et al., Reference ter Telgte, van Leijsen, Wiegertjes, Klijn, Tuladhar and de Leeuw2018). Our findings showed that AD and vascular pathologies have independent effects on primacy decline, which remains consistent with more comprehensive neuropathological analyses carried out in the overall ROS/MAP cohorts (Boyle et al., Reference Boyle, Yu, Wilson, Leurgans, Schneider and Bennett2018, Reference Boyle, Wang, Yu, Wilson, Dawe, Arfanakis, Schneider and Bennett2021). The broader literature is somewhat mixed with respect to whether comorbid neuropathologies exert additive or synergistic effects on cognition (Kapasi et al., Reference Kapasi, DeCarli and Schneider2017). While we did not observe an interaction effect of neurofibrillary tangles and atherosclerosis on primacy decline, identification of such effects may require a larger sample size. Nonetheless, there remains a continued need to examine pathological interactions in preclinical and at-risk groups because converging evidence from large-scale clinical-pathologic cohort studies indicate that dementia predominantly arises as a function of mixed pathologies in the brain (Au et al., Reference Au, Seshadri, Knox, Beiser, Himali, Cabral, Sanford, Green, Wolf and McKee2012; Rahimi & Kovacs, Reference Rahimi and Kovacs2014; White et al., Reference White, Edland, Hemmy, Montine, Zarow, Sonnen, Uyehara-Lock, Gelber, Ross, Petrovitch, Masaki, Lim, Launer and Montine2016).

Limitations to the current study must be noted. The subsample of cases selected for this study were individuals who died in old age and met criteria for being in Braak stage I or II upon autopsy. We labeled this group as being in the putative preclinical stage of AD. However, we cannot determine whether these individuals would have progressed to a more advanced Braak stage and eventually met criteria for pathologic AD had they not died, or if their pathological profile might be better characterized as a primary age-related tauopathy (Crary et al., Reference Crary, Trojanowski, Schneider, Abisambra, Abner, Alafuzoff, Arnold, Attems, Beach, Bigio, Cairns, Dickson, Gearing, Grinberg, Hof, Hyman, Jellinger, Jicha, Kovacs, Knopman, Kofler, Kukull, Mackenzie, Masliah, McKee, Montine, Murray, Neltner, Santa-Maria, Seeley, Serrano-Pozo, Shelanski, Stein, Takao, Thal, Toledo, Tronscoso, Vonsattel, White, Wisniewski, Woltjer, Yamada and Nelson2014; Dubois et al., Reference Dubois, Hampel, Feldman, Scheltens, Aisen, Andrieu, Bakardjian, Benali, Bertrm, Blennow, Broich, Cavedo, Crutch, Dartigues, Duyckaerts, Epelbaum, Frisoni, Gauthier, Genthon, Gouw, Habert, Holtzman, Kivipelto, Lista, Molinuevo, O'Bryant, Rabinovici, Rowe, Salloway, Schneider, Sperling, Teichmann, Carrillo, Cummings and Jack2016). Second, we only examined the presence or absence of co-morbid pathologies. Follow-up investigations should localize and quantify the severity of vascular pathologies to obtain a more nuanced understanding of the impacts on cognition. We also excluded cases with advanced TDP-43 but it is commonly comorbid with AD pathology and thus future investigations should take this into consideration when examining preclinical and at-risk for AD groups (Huang et al., Reference Huang, Zhou, Tu, Ba, Huang, Huang and Luo2020). Finally, our sample was small and consisted predominantly of highly educated White persons who were in the late stages of life and therefore our findings may not generalize well to the broader aging population. The main strengths of this study are the high rates of participation in annual follow-up and autopsy, yielding rich longitudinal clinical data linked to gold-standard assessments of brain disease, thereby increasing internal validity.

This work highlights the unique sensitivity of serial position scores in detecting subtle cognitive changes that otherwise evade detection on standard neuropsychological assessments in the preclinical or at-risk state of AD. In conjunction with routine neuropsychological scores, a process score approach that integrates primacy performance with existing approaches for defining subtle cognitive decline (Thomas et al., Reference Thomas, Edmonds, Eppig, Salmon and Bondi2018) could help identify individuals at risk of progressing to Alzheimer’s dementia much earlier and thereby widen the window of opportunity for pharmacologic and lifestyle intervention. Future studies should consider examining primacy effects in the context of the AT(N) biomarker framework, as recent work has proposed adding a measure of cognitive status could help improve prediction of cognitive decline in adults without dementia (O’Shea et al., Reference O’Shea, Thomas, Asken, Lee, Davis, Malloy, Salloway and Correia2021). More broadly, an exciting direction for this work will be to harness digital process scores from technologically advanced assessment tools that enable us to uncover novel cognitive parameters with enhanced sensitivity to preclinical AD (Au et al., Reference Au, Piers and Devine2017).

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617722000595

Acknowledgements

We thank the faculty and staff of the Rush Alzheimer’s Disease Center, and the participants in ROSMAP.

Author contributions

KMG: study conceptualization and design, data analysis and interpretation, writing original draft; WGH: study design, review, editing; SEL: study design, data interpretation, review, editing; RSW: data acquisition, study design, review, editing; PAB: data acquisition, study design, review, editing, funding acquisition; JAS: data acquisition, review, editing; DAB: data acquisition, review, editing, funding acquisition.

Funding statement

ROSMAP was supported by the following NIH grants: P30AG10161, P30AG72975, R01AG15819, R01AG17917, R01AG034374. ROSMAP data can be requested at https://www.radc.rush.edu.

Conflicts of interest

None.

References

Albert, M., Smith, L. A., Scherr, P. A., Taylor, J. O., Evans, D. A., & Funkenstein, H. H. (1991). Use of brief cognitive tests to identify individuals in the community with clinically diagnosed Alzheimer’s disease. International Journal of Neuroscience, 57, 167178. https://doi.org/10.3109/00207459109150691 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arnsten, A. F. T., Datta, D., Del Tredici, K., & Braak, H. (2021). Hypothesis: Tau pathology is an initiating factor in sporadic Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 17, 115124. https://doi.org/10.1002/alz.12192 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arvanitakis, Z., Capuano, A. W., Leurgans, S. E., Buchman, A. S., Bennett, D. A., & Schneider, J. A. (2017). The relationship of cerebral vessel pathology to brain microinfarcts. Brain Pathology, 27, 7785. https://doi.org/10.1111/bpa.12365 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arvanitakis, Z., Leurgans, S. E., Barnes, L. L., Bennett, D. A., & Schneider, J. A. (2011). Microinfarct pathology, dementia, and cognitive systems. Stroke, 42, 722727. https://doi.org/10.1161/STROKEAHA.110.595082 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Atkinson, R. C., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1968). Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 2, 89195. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60422-3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Au, R., Piers, R. J., & Devine, S. (2017). How technology is reshaping cognitive assessment: Lessons from the Framingham Heart Study. Neuropsychology, 31, 846861. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000411 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Au, R., Seshadri, S., Knox, K., Beiser, A., Himali, J. J., Cabral, H. J., Sanford, A., Green, R. C., Wolf, P. A., &  McKee, A. C. (2012). The Framingham brain donation program: Neuropathology along the cognitive continuum. Current Alzheimer Research, 9, 673686. https://doi.org/10.2174/156720512801322609 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. C. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-efffects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67, 148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bayley, P. J., Salmon, D. P., Bondi, M. W., Bui, B. K., Olichney, J., Delis, D. C., Thomas, R. G., & Thal, L. J. (2000). Comparison of the serial position effect in very mild Alzheimer’s disease, mild Alzheimer’s disease, and amnesia associated with electroconvulsive therapy. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6, 290298.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bennett, D. A., Buchman, A. S., Boyle, P. A., Barnes, L. L., Wilson, R. S., & Schneider, J. A. (2018). Religious orders study and rush memory and aging project. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 64, S161S189. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-179939 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bennett, D. A., Schneider, J. A., Aggarwal, N. T., Arvanitakis, Z., Shah, R. C., Kelly, J. F., Fox, J. H., Cochran, E. J., Arends, D., Treinkman, A. D., & Wilson, R. S., (2006b). Decision rules guiding the clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease in two community-based cohort studies compared to standard practice in a clinic-based cohort study. Neuroepidemiology, 27, 169176. https://doi.org/10.1159/000096129 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bennett, D. A., Schneider, J. A., Arvanitakis, Z., Kelly, J. F., Aggarwal, N. T., Shah, R. C., & Wilson, R. S. (2006a). Neuropathology of older persons without cognitive impairment from two community-based studies. Neurology, 66, 18371844. https://doi.org/10.1212/01.wnl.0000219668.47116.e6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bennett, D. A., Wilson, R. S., Schneider, J. A., Evans, D. A., Beckett, L. A., Aggarwal, N. T., Barnes, L. L., Fox, J. H., & Bach, J. (2002). Natural history of mild cognitive impairment in older persons. Neurology, 59, 198205. https:.doi.org/10.1212/WNL.59.2.198 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boyle, P. A., Wang, T., Yu, L., Wilson, R. S., Dawe, R., Arfanakis, K., Schneider, J. A., & Bennett, D. A. (2021). To what degree is late life cognitive decline driven by age-related neuropathologies? Brain, 144, 21662175. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awab092 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boyle, P. A., Yang, J., Yu, L., Leurgans, S. E., Capuano, A. W., Schneider, J. A., Wilson, R. S., & Bennett, D. A. (2017). Varied effects of age-related neuropathologies on the trajectory of late life cognitive decline. Brain, 140, 804812. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/aww341 Google ScholarPubMed
Boyle, P. A., Yu, L., Nag, S., Leurgans, S., Wilson, R. S., Bennett, D. A., & Schneider, J. A. (2015). Cerebral amyloid angiopathy and cognitive outcomes in community-based older persons. Neurology, 85, 19301936. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000002175 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boyle, P. A., Yu, L., Wilson, R. S., Leurgans, S. E., Schneider, J. A., & Bennett, D. A. (2018). Person-specific contribution of neuropathologies to cognitive loss in old age. Annals of Neurology, 83, 7483. https://doi.org/10.1002/ana.25123 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Braak, H., Alafuzoff, I., Arzberger, T., Kretzschmar, H., & Del Tredici, K. (2006). Staging of Alzheimer disease-associated neurofibrillary pathology using paraffin sections and immunocytochemistry. Acta Neuropathologica, 112, 389404. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00401-006-0127-z CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Braak, H., & Del Tredici, K. (2015). The preclinical phase of the pathological process underlying sporadic Alzheimer’s disease. Brain, 138, 28142833. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awv236 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Braak, H., & Del Tredici, K. (2020). From the entorhinal region via the prosubiculum to the dentate fascia: Alzheimer disease-related neurofibrillary changes in the temporal allocortex. Journal of Neuropathology & Experimental Neurology, 79, 163175. https://doi.org/10.1093/jnen/nlz123 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brenowitz, W. D., Monsell, S. E., Schmitt, F. A., Kukull, W. A., & Nelson, P. T. (2015). Hippocampal sclerosis of aging is a key Alzheimer’s disease mimic: clinical-pathologic correlations and comparisons with both Alzheimer’s disease and non-taupathic frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 39, 691702. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-131880 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruno, D., Gleason, C. E., Koscik, R. L., Pomara, N., Zetterberg, H., Blennow, K., & Johnson, S. C. (2019). The recency ratio is related to CSF amyloid beta 1-42 levels in MCI-AD. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 34, 415419. https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.5029 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruno, D., Grothe, M. J., Nierenberg, J., Zetterberg, H., Blennow, K., Teipel, S. J., & Pomara, N. (2015). A study on the specificity of the association between hippocampal volume and delayed primacy performance in cognitively intact elderly individuals. Neuropsychologia, 69, 18. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.01.025 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruno, D., Koscik, R. L., Woodard, J. L., Pomara, N., & Johnson, S. C. (2018). The recency ratio as predictor of early MCI. International Psychogeriatrics, 30, 18831888. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1041610218000467 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruno, D., Mueller, K. D., Betthauser, T., Chin, N., Engelman, C. D., Christian, B., Koscik, R. L., & Johnson, S. C. (2021). Serial position effects in the logical memory test: Loss of primacy predicts amyloid positivity. Journal of Neuropsychology, 15, 448461. https://doi.org/10.1111/jnp.12235 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruno, D., Reichert, C., & Pomara, N. (2016). The recency ratio as an index of cognitive performance and decline in elderly individuals. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 38, 967973. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2016.1179721 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruno, D., Reiss, P. T., Petkova, E., Sidtis, J. J., & Pomara, N. (2013). Decreased recall of primacy words predicts cognitive decline. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 28, 95103. https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acs116 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buchman, A. S., Leurgans, S. E., Nag, S., Bennett, D. A., & Schneider, J. A. (2011). Cerebrovascular disease pathology and Parkinsonian signs in old age. Stroke, 42, 31833189. https://doi.org/10.1161/STROKEAHA.111.623462 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buckley, R. F., Hanseeuw, B., Schultz, A. P., Vannini, P., Aghjayan, S. L., Properzi, M. J., Jackson, J. D., Mormino, E. C., Rentz, D. M., Sperling, R. A., Johnson, K. A., & Amariglio, R. E. (2017). Region-specific association of subjective cognitive decline with tauopathy independent of global β-amyloid burden. JAMA Neurology, 74, 14551463. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamaneurol.2017.2216 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chander, R. J., Foo, H., Yong, T., Lim, L., Tan, J., Wen, M.-C., Ng, A., Hameed, S., Ting, S., Zhou, J., & Kandiah, N. (2018). Serial position effects differ between Alzheimer’s and vascular features in mild cognitive impairment. Aging, 10, 38663880. https://doi.org/10.18632/aging.101678 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, X., Cassady, K. E., Adams, J. N., Harrison, T. M., Baker, S. L., & Jagust, W. J. (2021). Regional tau effects on prospective cognitive change in cognitively normal older adults. The Journal of Neuroscience, 41, 366375. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2111-20.2020 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colangeli, S., Boccia, M., Verde, P., Guariglia, P., Bianchini, F., & Piccardi, L. (2016). Cognitive reserve in healthy aging and Alzheimer’s disease: A meta-analysis of fMRI Studies. American Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease and Other Dementias, 31, 443449. https://doi.org/10.1177/1533317516653826 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crary, J. F., Trojanowski, J. Q., Schneider, J. A., Abisambra, J. F., Abner, E. L., Alafuzoff, I., Arnold, S. E., Attems, J., Beach, T. G., Bigio, E. H., Cairns, N. J., Dickson, D. W., Gearing, M., Grinberg, L. T., Hof, P. R., Hyman, B. T., Jellinger, K., Jicha, G. A., Kovacs, G. G., Knopman, D. S., Kofler, J., Kukull, W. A., Mackenzie, I. R., Masliah, E., McKee, A., Montine, T. J., Murray, M. E., Neltner, J. H., Santa-Maria, I., Seeley, W. W., Serrano-Pozo, A., Shelanski, M. L., Stein, T., Takao, M., Thal, D. R., Toledo, J. B., Tronscoso, J. C., Vonsattel, J. P., White, C. L., Wisniewski, T., Woltjer, R. L., Yamada, M., & Nelson, P. T. (2014). Primary age-related tauopathy (PART): A common pathology associated with human aging. Acta Neuropathologica, 128, 755766. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00401-014-1349-0 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cunha, C., Guerreiro, M., de Mendonça, A., Oliveira, P. E., & Santana, I. (2012). Serial position effects in Alzheimer’s disease, mild cognitive impairment, and normal aging: Predictive value for conversion to dementia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 34, 841852. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2012.689814 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeTure, M. A., & Dickson, D. W. (2019). The neuropathological diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease. Molecular Degeneration, 14, 32. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13024-019-0333-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dickerson, B. C., Stoub, T. R., Shah, R. C., Sperling, R. A., Killiany, R. J., Albert, M. S., Hyman, B. T., Blacker, D., & Detoledo-Morrell, L. (2011). Alzheimer-signature MRI biomarker predicts AD dementia in cognitively normal adults. Neurology, 76, 13951402. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182166e96 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dubois, B., Hampel, H., Feldman, H. H., Scheltens, P., Aisen, P., Andrieu, S., Bakardjian, H., Benali, H., Bertrm, L., Blennow, K., Broich, K., Cavedo, E., Crutch, S., Dartigues, J.-F., Duyckaerts, C., Epelbaum, S., Frisoni, G. B., Gauthier, S., Genthon, R., Gouw, A. A., Habert, M-O., Holtzman, D. M., Kivipelto, M., Lista, S., Molinuevo, J.-L., O'Bryant, S. E., Rabinovici, G. D., Rowe, C., Salloway, S., Schneider, L. S., Sperling, R., Teichmann, M., Carrillo, M. C., Cummings, J., JackJr., C. R. , & from the Proceedings of the Meeting of the International Working Group (IWG) and the American Alzheimer’s Association on “The Preclinical State of AD” (2016). Preclinical Alzheimer’s disease: Definition, natural history, and diagnostic criteria. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 12, 292323. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2016.02.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Egli, S. C., Beck, I. R., Berres, M., Foldi, N. S., Monsch, A. U., & Sollberger, M. (2014). Serial position effects are sensitive predictors of conversion from MCI to Alzheimer’s disease dementia. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 10, S420S424. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2013.09.012 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Egli, S. C., Hirni, D. I., Taylor, K. I., Berres, M., Regeniter, A., Gass, A., Monsch, A. U., & Sollberger, M. (2015). Varying strength of cognitive markers and biomarkers to predict conversion and cognitive decline in an early-stage-enriched mild cognitive impairment sample. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 44, 625633. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-141716 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gicas, K. M., Honer, W. G., Wilson, R. S., Boyle, P. A., Leurgans, S. E., Schneider, J. A., & Bennett, D. A. (2020). Association of serial position scores on memory tests and hippocampal-related neuropathologic outcomes. Neurology, 95, e3303e3312. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000010952 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greene, J. D. W., Baddeley, A. D., & Hodges, J. R. (1996). Analysis of the episodic memory deficit in early Alzheimer’s disease: Evidence from the doors and people test. Neuropsychologia, 34, 537551. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(95)00151-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Han, S. D., Nguyen, C. P., Stricker, N. H., & Nation, D. A. (2017). Detectable neuropsychological differences in early preclinical Alzheimer’s disease: A meta-analysis. Neuropsychology Review, 27, 305325. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-017-9345-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hermann, B. P., Seidenberg, M., Wyler, A., Davies, K., Christeson, J., Moran, M., & Stroup, E. (1996). The effects of human hippocampal resection on the serial position curve. Cortex, 32, 323334. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0010-9452(96)80054-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howieson, D. B., Mattek, N., Seeyle, A. M., Dodge, H. H., Wasserman, D., Zitzelberger, T., & Kaye, J. A. (2011). Serial position effects in mild cognitive impairment. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 33, 292299. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2010.516742 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huang, W., Zhou, Y., Tu, L., Ba, Z., Huang, J., Huang, N., & Luo, Y. (2020). TDP-43: From Alzheimer’s disease to limbic-predominant age-related TDP-43 encephalopathy. Frontiers in Molecular Neuroscience, 13, 26. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnmol.2020.00026 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jack, C. R., Bennett, D. A., Blennow, K., Carrillo, M. C., Dunn, B., Haeberlein, S. B., Holtzman, D. M., Jagust, W., Jessen, F., Karlawish, J., Liu, E., Molinuevo, J. L., Montine, T., Phelps, C., Rankin, K. P., Rowe, C. C., Scheltens, P., Siemers, E., Snyder, H. M., & Sperling, R. (2018). NIA-AA research framework: Toward a biological definition of Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 14, 535562. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2018.02.018 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, S. N., Greer, A. J., & Cox, D. E. (2011). Learning characteristics of the CERAD word list in an elderly VA sample. Applied Neuropsychology, 18, 157163. https://doi.org/10.1080/09084282.2011.595441 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kapasi, A., DeCarli, C., & Schneider, J. A. (2017). Impact of multiple pathologies on the threshold for clinically overt dementia. Acta Neuropathologica, 134, 171186. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00401-017-1717-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
La Rue, A., Hermann, B., Jones, J. E., Johnson, S., Asthana, S., & Sager, M. A. (2008). Effect of parental family history of Alzheimer’s disease on serial position profiles. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 4, 285290. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2008.03.009 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Love, S., Chalmers, K., Ince, P., Esiri, M., Attems, J., Jellinger, K., Yamada, M., McCarron, M., Minett, T., Matthews, F., Greenberg, S., Mann, D., & Kehoe, P. G. (2014). Development, appraisal, validation and implementation of a consensus protocol for the assessment of cerebral amyloid angiopathy in post-mortem brain tissue. American Journal of Neurodegenerative Disease, 3, 1932.Google ScholarPubMed
Malmberg, K. J., Raaijmakers, J. G. W., & Shiffrin, R. M. (2019). 50 years of research sparked by Atkinson and Shiffrin (1968). Memory & Cognition, 47, 561574. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-019-00896-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marra, C., Silveri, M. C., & Gainotti, G. (2000). Predictors of cognitive decline in the early stage of probable Alzheimer’s disease. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 11, 212218. https://doi.org/10.1159/000017239 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matthews, F. E., Brayne, C., Lowe, J., McKeith, I., Wharton, S. B., & Ince, P. (2009). Epidemiological pathology of dementia: Attributable-risks at death in the medical research council cognitive function and ageing study. PLoS Medicine, 6, e1000180. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1000180 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McKhann, G., Drachman, D., Folstein, M., Katzman, R., Price, D., & Stadlan, E. M. (1984). Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: Report of the NINCDS-ADRDA work group under the auspices of department of health and human services task force on Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology, 34, 939944. https://doi.org/10.1212/wnl.34.7.939 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morris, J. C., Heyman, A., Mohs, R. C., Hughes, J. P., van Belle, G., Fillenbaum, G., Mellits, E. D., & Clark, C. (1989). The consortium to establish a registry for Alzheimer’s disease (CERAD). Part I. Clinical and neuropsychological assessment of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology, 39, 11591165. https://doi.org/10.1212/wnl.39.9.1159 Google Scholar
Mortamais, M., Ash, J. A., Harrison, J., Kaye, J., Kramer, J., Randolph, C., Pose, C., Albala, B., Ropacki, M., Ritchie, C. W., & Ritchie, K. (2017). Detecting cognitive changes in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease: A review of its feasibility. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 13, 468492. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2016.06.2365 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moser, B., Deisenhammer, E. A., Marksteiner, J., Papousek, I., Fink, A., & Weiss, E. M. (2014). Serial position effects in patients with mild cognitive impairment and early and moderate Alzheimer’s disease compared with healthy comparison subjects. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 37, 1926. https://doi.org/10.1159/000351675 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murdock, B. B. Jr. (1962). The serial position effect of free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 482488. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0045106 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nag, S., Yu, L., Boyle, P. A., Leurgans, S. E., Bennett, D. A., & Schneider, J. A. (2018). TDP-43 pathology in anterior temporal pole cortex in aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Acta Neuropathologica Communications, 6, 33. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40478-018-0531-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nag, S., Yu, L., Capuano, A. W., Wilson, R. S., Leurgans, S. E., Bennett, D. A., & Schneider, J. A. (2015). Hippocampal sclerosis and TDP-43 pathology in aging and Alzheimer disease. Annals of Neurology, 77, 942952. https://doi.org/10.1002/ana.24388 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nag, S., Yu, L., Wilson, R. S., Chen, E-Y., Bennett, D. A., & Schneider, J. A. (2017). TDP-43 pathology and memory impairment in elders without pathologic diagnoses of AD or FTLD. Neurology, 88, 653660. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000003610 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, P. T., Alafuzoff, I., Bigio, E. H., Bouras, C., Braak, H., Cairns, N. J., Castellani, R. J., Crain, B. J., Davies, P., Del Tredici, K., Duyckaerts, C., Frosch, M. P., Haroutunian, V., Hof, P. R., Hulette, C. M., Hyman, B. T., Iwatsubo, T., Jellinger, K. A., Jicha, G. A., Kövari, E., Kukull, W. A., Leverenz, J. B., Love, S., Mackenzie, I. R., Mann, D. M., Masliah, E., McKee, A. C., Montine, T. J., Morris, J. C., Schneider, J. A., Sonnen, J. A., Thal, D. R., Trojanowski, J. Q., Troncoso, J. C., Wisniewski, T., Woltjer, R. L., & Beach, T. G. (2012). Correlation of Alzheimer disease neuropathologic changes with cognitive status: A review of the literature. Journal of Neuropathology and Experimental Neurology, 71, 362381. https://doi.org/10.1097/NEN.0b013e31825018f7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Shea, D. M., Thomas, K. R., Asken, B., Lee, A. K. W., Davis, J. D., Malloy, P. F., Salloway, S. P., Correia, S., & for the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. (2021). Adding cognition to AT(N) models improves prediction of cognitive and functional decline. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 13, e12174. https://doi.org/10.1002/dad2.12174 Google Scholar
Ossenkoppele, R., Smith, R., Ohlsson, T., Strandberg, O., Mattsson, N., Insel, P. S., Palmqvist, S., & Hansson, O. (2019). Associations between tau, Aβ, and cortical thickness with cognition in Alzheimer disease. Neurology, 92, e601e612. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000006875 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rahimi, J., & Kovacs, G. G. (2014). Prevalence of mixed pathologies in the aging brain. Alzheimer’s Research & Therapy, 6, 82. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13195-014-0082-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Core Team. (2020). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. https://www.R-project.org/.Google Scholar
Scheller, E., Minkova, L., Leitner, M., & Klöppel, S. (2014). Attempted and successful compensation in preclinical and early manifest neurodegeneration – a review of task fMRI studies. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 5, 132. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00132 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Talamonti, D., Koscik, R., Johnson, S., & Bruno, D. (2019). Predicting early mild cognitive impairment with free recall: The primacy of primacy. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 35, 133142. https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acz013 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Talmi, D., Grady, C. L., Goshen-Gottstein, Y., & Moscovitch, M. (2005). Neuroimaging the serial position curve. A test of single-store versus dual-store models. Psychological Science, 16, 716723. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01601.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tanner, J. A., & Rabinovici, G. D. (2021). Relationship between tau and cognition in the evolution of Alzheimer’s disease: new insights from tau PET. Journal of Nuclear Medicine, 62, 612613. https://doi.org/10.2967/jnumed.120.257824 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
ter Telgte, A., van Leijsen, E. M. C., Wiegertjes, K., Klijn, C. J. M., Tuladhar, A. M., & de Leeuw, F.-E. (2018). Cerebral small vessel disease: From a focal to a global perspective. Nature Reviews Neurology, 14, 387398. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41582-018-0014-y CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
The National Institute on Aging, and Reagan Institute Working Group on Diagnostic Criteria for the Neuropathological Assessment of Alzheimer’s Disease. (1997). Consensus recommendations for the postmortem diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiology of Aging, 18, S1S2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomas, K. R., Bangen, K. J., Weigand, A. J., Edmonds, E. C., Wong, C. G., Cooper, S., Delano-Wood, L., Bondi, M. W., & for theAlzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. (2020). Objective subtle cognitive difficulties predict future amyloid accumulation and neurodegeneration. Neurology, 94, e397e406. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000008838 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomas, K. R., Edmonds, E. C., Eppig, J., Salmon, D. P., Bondi, M. W., & for the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. (2018). Using neuropsychological process scores to identify subtle cognitive decline and predict progression to mild cognitive impairment. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 64, 195204. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-180229 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toledo, J. B., Bjerke, M., Chen, K., Rozycki, M., Jack, C. R., Weiner, M. W., Arnold, S. E., Reiman, E. M., Davatzikos, C., Shaw, L. M., Trojanowski, J. Q., & for the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. (2015). Memory, executive, and multidomain subtle cognitive impairment: Clinical and biomarker findings. Neurology, 85, 144153. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000001738 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Villemagne, V. L., Burnham, S., Bourgeat, P., Brown, B., Ellis, K. A., Salvado, O., Szoeke, C., Macaulay, S. L., Martins, R., Maruff, P., Ames, D., Rowe, C. C., Masters, C. L., & for the Australian Imaging Biomarkers and Lifestyle (AIBL) Research Group. (2013). Amyloid β deposition, neurodegeneration, and cognitive decline in sporadic Alzheimer’s disease: A prospective cohort study. Lancet Neurology, 12, 357367. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(13)70044-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (1987). Wechsler memory scale-revised: Manual. Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Weitzner, D. S., & Calamia, M. (2020). Serial position effects on list learning tasks in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology, 34, 467478. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000620 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weitzner, D. S., Divers, R., & Calamia, M. (2021). The incremental validity of primacy as a predictor of everyday functioning. Neuropsychology, 36(1), 2334. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000778 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
White, L. R., Edland, S. D., Hemmy, L. S., Montine, K. S., Zarow, C., Sonnen, J. A., Uyehara-Lock, J. H., Gelber, R. P., Ross, G. W., Petrovitch, H., Masaki, K. H., Lim, K. O., Launer, L. J., & Montine, T. J. (2016). Neuropathologic comorbidity and cognitive impairment in the Nun and Honolulu-Asia aging studies. Neurology, 86, 10001008. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000002480 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, R. S., Yang, J., Yu, L., Leurgans, S. E., Capuano, A. W., Schneider, J. A., Bennett, D. A., & Boyle, P. A. (2019). Postmortem neurodegenerative markers and trajectories of decline in cognitive systems. Neurology, 92, e831e840. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000006949 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Younes, L., Albert, M., Moghekar, A., Soldan, A., Pettigrew, C., & Miller, M. I. (2019). Identifying changepoints in biomarkers during the preclinical phase of Alzheimer’s disease. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 11, 74. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2019.00074 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. Flow chart delineating participant inclusion and exclusion.

Figure 1

Table 1. Descriptive statistics of the distribution of neuropathologies

Figure 2

Figure 2. Serial position profiles for the baseline cognitive evaluation (left panel) and last available cognitive evaluation (right panel) stratified by cognitive status (NCI = not cognitive impaired; MCI = cognitively impaired; DEM = dementia). Scores represent the proportion of words recalled in each of the primacy, middle, and recency regions. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals.

Figure 3

Table 2. Mixed effects models for trajectories of change in cognitive outcome

Figure 4

Table 3. Final mixed effects moderation model for primacy score trajectories

Figure 5

Figure 3. Change in primacy scores over time as a function of neurofibrillary tangle density (mean +/- 1SD).

Figure 6

Figure 4. Change in primacy scores over time as a function of atherosclerosis (present or absent).

Supplementary material: File

Gicas et al. supplementary material

Table S1

Download Gicas et al. supplementary material(File)
File 15.4 KB