Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-p9bg8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T12:26:47.149Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The prefrontal cortex, but not the medial temporal lobe, is associated with episodic memory in middle-aged persons with HIV

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 November 2024

Laura M. Campbell*
Affiliation:
University of California San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA
Christine Fennema-Notestine
Affiliation:
University of California San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA
Erin E. Sundermann
Affiliation:
University of California San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA
Averi Barrett
Affiliation:
University of California San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA
Mark W. Bondi
Affiliation:
University of California San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA VA San Diego Healthcare System, San Diego, CA, USA
Ronald J. Ellis
Affiliation:
University of California San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA
Donald Franklin
Affiliation:
University of California San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA
Benjamin Gelman
Affiliation:
University of Texas Medical Branch, Galveston, TX, USA
Paul E. Gilbert
Affiliation:
San Diego State University, San Diego, CA, USA
Igor Grant
Affiliation:
University of California San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA
Robert K. Heaton
Affiliation:
University of California San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA
David J. Moore
Affiliation:
University of California San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA
Susan Morgello
Affiliation:
The Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, NY, USA
Scott Letendre
Affiliation:
University of California San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA
Payal B. Patel
Affiliation:
University of Washington, Seattle, WA, USA
Scott Roesch
Affiliation:
San Diego State University, San Diego, CA, USA
Raeanne C. Moore
Affiliation:
University of California San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA
*
Corresponding author: Laura M. Campbell; Email: l4campbe@health.ucsd.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective:

Identifying persons with HIV (PWH) at increased risk for Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is complicated because memory deficits are common in HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders (HAND) and a defining feature of amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI; a precursor to AD). Recognition memory deficits may be useful in differentiating these etiologies. Therefore, neuroimaging correlates of different memory deficits (i.e., recall, recognition) and their longitudinal trajectories in PWH were examined.

Design:

We examined 92 PWH from the CHARTER Program, ages 45–68, without severe comorbid conditions, who received baseline structural MRI and baseline and longitudinal neuropsychological testing. Linear and logistic regression examined neuroanatomical correlates (i.e., cortical thickness and volumes of regions associated with HAND and/or AD) of memory performance at baseline and multilevel modeling examined neuroanatomical correlates of memory decline (average follow-up = 6.5 years).

Results:

At baseline, thinner pars opercularis cortex was associated with impaired recognition (p = 0.012; p = 0.060 after correcting for multiple comparisons). Worse delayed recall was associated with thinner pars opercularis (p = 0.001) and thinner rostral middle frontal cortex (p = 0.006) cross sectionally even after correcting for multiple comparisons. Delayed recall and recognition were not associated with medial temporal lobe (MTL), basal ganglia, or other prefrontal structures. Recognition impairment was variable over time, and there was little decline in delayed recall. Baseline MTL and prefrontal structures were not associated with delayed recall.

Conclusions:

Episodic memory was associated with prefrontal structures, and MTL and prefrontal structures did not predict memory decline. There was relative stability in memory over time. Findings suggest that episodic memory is more related to frontal structures, rather than encroaching AD pathology, in middle-aged PWH. Additional research should clarify if recognition is useful clinically to differentiate aMCI and HAND.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of International Neuropsychological Society

Introduction

HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders (HAND) remain prevalent in the era of combination antiretroviral therapy (ART; 20–50%; Heaton et al., Reference Heaton, Clifford, Franklin, Woods, Ake, Vaida, Ellis, Letendre, Marcotte, Atkinson, Rivera-Mindt, Vigil, Taylor, Collier, Marra, Gelman, McArthur, Morgello, Simpson, McCutchan, Abramson, Gamst, Fennema-Notestine, Jernigan and Wong2010; Saloner & Cysique, Reference Saloner and Cysique2017). HAND is usually non-progressive (Clifford & Ances, Reference Clifford and Ances2013) and the majority of neurocognitive deficits remain in the mild-to-moderate range (Heaton et al., Reference Heaton, Clifford, Franklin, Woods, Ake, Vaida, Ellis, Letendre, Marcotte, Atkinson, Rivera-Mindt, Vigil, Taylor, Collier, Marra, Gelman, McArthur, Morgello, Simpson, McCutchan, Abramson, Gamst, Fennema-Notestine, Jernigan and Wong2010). HIV is known to particularly impact fronto-striatal systems (i.e., basal ganglia and prefrontal structures; Ances & Hammoud, Reference Ances and Hammoud2014). Similarly, HAND has been associated with fronto-striatal circuits as well as cortical structures (Alakkas et al., Reference Alakkas, Ellis, Watson, Umlauf, Heaton, Letendre, Collier, Marra, Clifford, Gelman, Sacktor, Morgello, Simpson, McCutchan, Kallianpur, Gianella, Marcotte, Grant and Fennema-Notestine2019; Ances & Hammoud, Reference Ances and Hammoud2014; Nichols et al., Reference Nichols, Gates, Soares, Moffat, Rae, Brew and Cysique2019). This fronto-striatal involvement associated with HAND is thought to account for its “subcortical” cognitive presentation, with characteristic deficits in executive functioning, learning, and memory (specifically recall, as opposed to recognition; Salmon & Bondi, Reference Salmon and Bondi2009). Indeed, such deficits are most common in HAND (Heaton et al., Reference Heaton, Franklin, Ellis, McCutchan, Letendre, LeBlanc, Corkran, Duarte, Clifford, Woods, Collier, Marra, Morgello, Mindt, Taylor, Marcotte, Atkinson, Wolfson, Gelman, McArthur, Simpson, Abramson, Gamst, Fennema-Notestine, Jernigan, Wong and Grant2011), and this “subcortical” presentation has been observed even as PWH age (Scott et al., Reference Scott, Woods, Carey, Weber, Bondi and Grant2011).

Although HAND is not typically progressive, some evidence suggests episoidic memory may be particularly affected in older PWH (Pasipanodya et al., Reference Pasipanodya, Montoya, Campbell, Hussain, Saloner, Paolillo, Jeste, Letendre, McCutchan, Heaton and Moore2019; Seider et al., Reference Seider, Luo, Gongvatana, Devlin, de la Monte, Chasman, Yan, Tashima, Navia and Cohen2014). For example, Seider et al. (Reference Seider, Luo, Gongvatana, Devlin, de la Monte, Chasman, Yan, Tashima, Navia and Cohen2014) found that verbal memory declines more rapidly with age in PWH as compared to HIV-negative comparison participants. Additionally, episodic memory decline has been associated with hippocampal atrophy in PWH (Pfefferbaum et al., Reference Pfefferbaum, Rogosa, Rosenbloom, Chu, Sassoon, Kemper, Deresinski, Rohlfing, Zahr and Sullivan2014; Wang et al., Reference Wang, Wang, Ding and Shang2015). However, most of these studies examining memory in PWH have primarily focused on delayed recall and did not consider other potential etiologies beyond HAND.

In contrast to HAND, Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is a neurodegenerative disease associated with progressive cognitive decline and functional impairment (Alzheimer’s Association, 2017). AD is characterized by the accumulation of amyloid plaques (Aβ42) and tau neurofibrillary tangles (NFT) in the brain. NFTs are thought to initially accumulate predominantly in the medial temporal lobe (MTL) and result in atrophy of the MTL and later more widespread atrophy in adjacent cortices (Jack et al., Reference Jack, Bennett, Blennow, Carrillo, Dunn, Haeberlein, Holtzman, Jagust, Jessen, Karlawish, Liu, Molinuevo, Montine, Phelps, Rankin, Rowe, Scheltens, Siemers, Snyder, Sperling, Masliah, Ryan and Silverberg2018). Given the central role of the MTL in episodic memory processes, AD typically presents with changes in memory, including learning, delayed recall, and recognition (Smith & Butts, Reference Smith, Butts, Morgan and Ricker2016), with changes in language and executive functioning sometimes also observed in the early stages of the disease (Edmonds et al., Reference Edmonds, Delano-Wood, Clark, Jak, Nation, McDonald, Libon, Au, Galasko, Salmon, Bondi and Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging2015; Kirova et al., Reference Kirova, Bays and Lagalwar2015). Brain changes associated with future cognitive decline and incident AD dementia can be observed as early as midlife (Ritchie et al., Reference Ritchie, Ritchie, Yaffe, Skoog and Scarmeas2015; Sutphen et al., Reference Sutphen, Jasielec, Shah, Macy, Xiong, Vlassenko, Benzinger, Stoops, Vanderstichele, Brix, Darby, Vandijck, Ladenson, Morris, Holtzman and Fagan2015). Additionally, longitudinal studies have shown that more subtle differences in episodic memory in midlife (e.g., worse memory performance than “non-decliners” but not necessarily in the “impaired” range) is associated with a decline in memory years later (Clark et al., Reference Clark, Koscik, Nicholas, Okonkwo, Engelman, Bratzke and Johnson2016; Kremen et al., Reference Kremen, Jak, Panizzon, Spoon, Franz, Thompson, Jacobson, Vasilopoulos, Vuoksimaa, Xian, Toomey and Lyons2014; Okonkwo et al., Reference Okonkwo, Oh, Koscik, Jonaitis, Cleary, Dowling, Bendlin, LaRue, Hermann, Barnhart, Murali, Rowley, Carlsson, Gallagher, Asthana, Sager, Christian and Johnson2014). Moreover, Jak et al. (Reference Jak, Panizzon, Spoon, Fennema-Notestine, Franz, Thompson, Jacobson, Xian, Eyler, Vuoksimaa, Toomey, Lyons, Neale, Tsuang, Dale and Kremen2015) found that worse midlife memory performance in people without HIV is associated with hippocampal atrophy.

As PWH age, they may face increased risk of AD and its precursor, amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI), due to the compounding effects of HIV and aging on the brain, as well as higher prevalence of vascular and metabolic risk factors among PWH (Milanini & Valcour, 2017; Rubin et al., Reference Rubin, Sundermann and Moore2019). Due to the overlap in memory impairments, older PWH may be misclassified as having HAND instead of being on an AD trajectory. Recent case reports have highlighted the risk of delayed diagnosis of AD in PWH, the complexities in determining the etiology of cognitive impairment, and the need for non-invasive tools to differentiate HAND and aMCI (Calcagno et al., Reference Calcagno, Celani, Trunfio, Orofino, Imperiale, Atzori, Arena, d’Ettorre, Guaraldi, Gisslen and Di Perri2021; Hellmuth et al., Reference Hellmuth, Milanini, Masliah, Tartaglia, Dunlop, Moore, Javandel, DeVaughn and Valcour2018; Morgello et al., Reference Morgello, Jacobs, Murray, Byrd, Neibart, Mintz, Meloni, Chon and Crary2018).

Comparative studies between PWH with HAND and HIV-negative participants with MCI or AD have shown differences in brain structures. Smaller hippocampal volumes were observed in MCI/AD compared to HAND, while HAND often presented with greater frontal gray matter atrophy (Milanini et al., Reference Milanini, Samboju, Cobigo, Paul, Javandel, Hellmuth, Allen, Miller and Valcour2019; Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Kwon, Esmaeili-Firidouni, Pfefferbaum, Sullivan, Javitz, Valcour and Pohl2016). However, most studies have focused solely on HAND and have not explored other etiologies of cognitive impairment in PWH (Holt et al., Reference Holt, Kraft-Terry and Chang2012; Kuhn et al., Reference Kuhn, Kaufmann, Doan, Westlye, Jones, Nunez, Bookheimer, Singer, Hinkin and Thames2018). Recent work by Sundermann et al. (Reference Sundermann, Bondi, Campbell, Gouaux, Moore, Soontornniyomkij and Moore2021) has begun to examine neuropsychological methods for identifying aMCI in PWH using adapted Jak/Bondi MCI criteria (Jak et al., Reference Jak, Bondi, Delano-Wood, Wierenga, Corey-Bloom, Salmon and Delis2009) that requires recognition memory impairment. This study found that middle-aged and older PWH classified as aMCI were 3.5 times more likely to have the presence of Aβ42 plaques associated with AD, indicating that recognition memory impairment could serve as a clinical marker of AD risk in PWH. Further research is needed to validiate the link between recognition memory and other known AD markers, such as MTL integrity, among PWH.

To address gaps in the literature and specifically examine memory in PWH with the idea that multiple etiologies could be contributory (i.e., HAND and AD), we investigated the relationship between brain integrity and aspects of epidosic memory performance (i.e., delayed recall and recognition) in middle-aged PWH aged 45–68 years. The lower age range was selected given that Alzheimer’s pathology is known to start accumulating in mid-life, the majority of PWH are in this mid-life age range (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2018), and mid-life is when the research has demonstrated early preclinical changes in memory can start to show differences in those at greater risk of AD as described above. The first aim of the study was to examine the neuroimaging correlates of delayed recall and recognition in aging PWH using structural MRI data, focusing on the MTL, basal ganglia, and prefrontal cortex. We hypothesized that 1) recognition memory would be most strongly related to MTL structures given its association in AD and 2) delayed recall would be associated with MTL structures as well as fronto-striatal circuit structures, given the reliance on retrieval strategies and the associations found in both AD and HAND. To further validate the specificity of recognition-MTL relationships, processing speed and psychomotor skills were analyzed, with the hypothesis that they would be more strongly associated with basal ganglia and prefrontal cortex structures and are relatively preserved in aMCI (Paul et al., Reference Paul, Ernst, Brickman, Yiannoutsos, Tate, Cohen and Navia2008; Weintraub et al., Reference Weintraub, Wicklund and Salmon2012; Wright et al., Reference Wright, Pyakurel, Vaida, Price, Lee, Peterson, Fuchs, Zetterberg, Robertson, Walter, Meyerhoff, Spudich and Ances2016; note, executive functioning was not selected given the overlap in both HAND and aMCI). The second aim was to examine if baseline structural neuroimaging was associated longitudinally with amnestic decline, with the hypothesis that smaller baseline MTL structures would be associated with a greater and/or faster decline in recognition and delayed recall. For all analyses, we examined covariates that may also be contributing to cognition including HIV-disease characteristics, risk factors associated with AD (e.g., APOE status), and comorbid conditions.

Methods

Participants

Our study analyzed data from individuals enrolled in the longitudinal MRI arm of the CNS HIV Anti-Retroviral Therapy Effects Research (CHARTER) program, which is a study aimed broadly at understanding the nervous system complications in HIV, particularly in relation to ART (Heaton et al., Reference Heaton, Clifford, Franklin, Woods, Ake, Vaida, Ellis, Letendre, Marcotte, Atkinson, Rivera-Mindt, Vigil, Taylor, Collier, Marra, Gelman, McArthur, Morgello, Simpson, McCutchan, Abramson, Gamst, Fennema-Notestine, Jernigan and Wong2010; Jernigan et al., Reference Jernigan, Archibald, Fennema-Notestine, Taylor, Theilmann, Julaton, Notestine, Wolfson, Letendre, Ellis, Heaton, Gamst, Franklin, Clifford, Collier, Gelman, Marra, McArthur, McCutchan, Morgello, Simpson and Grant2011). After excluding participants who did not meet inclusion/exclusion criteria as detailed below, 92 PWH between the ages of 45 to 68 years old were included in this study. All participants underwent a structural MRI scan between 2008 and 2010 using a standardized acquisition, as well as comprehensive neuropsychological, neuromedical, and neuropsychiatric evaluations; the baseline in this case was not the first visit in the CHARTER study overall but the first visit with this specific MRI protocol, which changed across the life of the study. MRI and neuropsychological, neuromedical, and neuropsychiatric evaluations were completed within 3 months (average difference from MRI to other evaluations was 6 days). Most participants (n = 91) completed at least one follow-up neuropsychological, neuromedical, and neuropsychiatric study visit occurring in approximately 6-month intervals. Participants were drawn from five of the six participating CHARTER sites (i.e., those with comparable imaging capabilities): Johns Hopkins University, the Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, University of California San Diego, University of Texas Medical Branch, and University of Washington. All CHARTER study procedures were approved by each sites university Institutional Review Boards, all participants provided written informed consent, and the research was completed in accordance with the Helsinki Declaration.

Participant characterization and inclusion/exclusion criteria

CHARTER initial inclusion criteria were minimal. To determine the extent to which non-HIV-related comorbidities have contributed to neurocognitive impairment, participants with severe “confounding” comorbidities, as defined by Frascati criteria (i.e., current HAND nosology; Antinori et al., Reference Antinori, Arendt, Becker, Brew, Byrd, Cherner, Clifford, Cinque, Epstein, Goodkin, Gisslen, Grant, Heaton, Joseph, Marder, Marra, McArthur, Nunn, Price, Pulliam, Robertson, Sacktor, Valcour and Wojna2007; Heaton et al., Reference Heaton, Clifford, Franklin, Woods, Ake, Vaida, Ellis, Letendre, Marcotte, Atkinson, Rivera-Mindt, Vigil, Taylor, Collier, Marra, Gelman, McArthur, Morgello, Simpson, McCutchan, Abramson, Gamst, Fennema-Notestine, Jernigan and Wong2010), were excluded from this project. To study the effect of aging, the age range was restricted to participants who were aged 45 or older at the time of the MRI scan. Additionally, one participant was excluded from the study given that their T1 structural MRI scan was inadequate and did not yield usable data.

Neuropsychological evaluation

At each study visit, participants completed a standardized battery of neurocognitive tests. Before neuropsychological testing, participants passed the Hiscock Digit Memory Test, a free-standing performance validity test. The test battery assessed seven cognitive domains commonly affected by HIV (Heaton et al., Reference Heaton, Clifford, Franklin, Woods, Ake, Vaida, Ellis, Letendre, Marcotte, Atkinson, Rivera-Mindt, Vigil, Taylor, Collier, Marra, Gelman, McArthur, Morgello, Simpson, McCutchan, Abramson, Gamst, Fennema-Notestine, Jernigan and Wong2010); given the aims of this study the tests of memory, processing speed, and psychomotor skills were the focus of this analysis.

Tests of memory in the CHARTER study included the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test – Revised (HVLT-R) and the Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised (BVMT-R). Tests of processing speed included WAIS-III Digit Symbol and Symbol Search, Trail Making Test Part A, and the Stroop Color Trial. Psychomotor skills were examined via the bilateral Grooved Pegboard Test. Raw delayed recognition discrimination scores on the HVLT-R and BVMT-R were converted to Z-scores (M = 0, SD = 1) that account for demographic variables (i.e., age, sex, education, and race/ethnicity) using normative data from the HIV Neurobehaviroal Research Program (Norman et al., Reference Norman, Moore, Taylor, Franklin, Cysique, Ake, Lazarretto, Vaida and Heaton2011). The two recognition discrimination Z-scores were then averaged to create a recognition composite. The recognition analyses did not meet assumptions for linear regression (i.e., evidence of heteroscedasticity, residuals were non-normally distributed). Therefore, delayed recognition was dichotomized into an impaired recognition group (i.e. < -1.0 SD below the demographically corrected mean) and an unimpaired recognition group. Given that practice effect correction was not available for delayed recognition and participants had a varying number of previous administrations, number of prior neuropsychological evaluations was included as a covariate in analyses examining delayed recognition. Raw delayed recall, processing speed, and psychomotor scores were converted to T-scores (M = 50, SD = 10) that account for demographic variables (i.e., age, sex, education, and race/ethnicity) and practice effects using normative data from the HIV Neurobehavioral Research Program (Cysique et al., Reference Cysique, Franklin, Abramson, Ellis, Letendre, Collier, Clifford, Gelman, McArthur, Morgello, Simpson, McCutchan, Grant and Heaton2011; Heaton et al., Reference Heaton, Taylor and Manly2003; Norman et al., Reference Norman, Moore, Taylor, Franklin, Cysique, Ake, Lazarretto, Vaida and Heaton2011). Practice-effect corrected T-scores from all tests in each cognitive domain were then averaged to obtain a domain T-score. The Wide Range Achievement Test-III was utilized to estimate premorbid verbal IQ.

Neuromedical evaluation

Participants completed a neuromedical evaluation at each study timepoint. HIV disease characteristics were collected from most participants at each visit including (1) current CD4+ T lymphocyte count measured via flow cytometry; (2) nadir CD4 recorded via a combination of self-report and medical records; (3) HIV RNA in plasma measured by ultra-sensitive PCR (Amplicor, Roche Diagnostic System, Indianapolis IN; lower limit of detection <50 copies/ml); (4) estimated duration of HIV disease collected via self-report; and (5) current ART regimen. Comorbid medical conditions in Table 1 were determined by self-report and/or by their taking medication for the condition. Comorbid psychiatric and substance use conditions were determined with the Composite International Diagnostic Interview (World Health Organization, 1997). Additionally, CHARTER participants have APOE genotype data (see Morgan et al. (Reference Morgan, Woods, Letendre, Franklin, Bloss, Goate, Heaton, Collier, Marra, Gelman, McArthur, Morgello, Simpson, McCutchan, Ellis, Abramson, Gamst, Fennema-Notestine, Smith, Grant, Vaida and Clifford2013) for additional information). APOE genotype was dichotomized into those at highest genetic risk of AD (i.e., the APOE ε4+ group which included genotypes ε34 and ε44) and those at approximately average or lower genetic risk of AD (i.e., the APOE ε4- group including genotypes ε22, ε23, ε33, ε24; Rasmussen et al., Reference Rasmussen, Tybjærg-Hansen, Nordestgaard and Frikke-Schmidt2018).

Table 1. Participant demographic and clinical characteristics (N = 92)

Note: a n = 91; b n = 90; c n = 88; d n = 87; HVLT-R=Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised; BVMT-R=Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised; LT = lifetime; MDD = major depressive disorder; ART = antiretroviral therapy.

Neuroimaging

MRI data were acquired on six General Electric 1.5-Tesla scanners at five site locations that participated in this MRI protocol. A three-dimensional sagittal T1-weighted spoiled gradient recalled (SPGR) acquisition was acquired: section thickness = 1.3 mm, FOV 24 cm, matrix size 256 × 256 × 124; TR = 20 ms, TE = 6 ms, flip angle = 30. In addition, coronal 2D T2 and proton density (PD) weighted fast spin echo sequences with a slice thickness of 2.0 mm were acquired and, for the present study, provided direct a measure of intracranial vault volume (ICV) as detailed in previous tissue segmentation work (Fennema-Notestine et al., Reference Fennema-Notestine, Ellis, Archibald, Jernigan, Letendre, Notestine, Taylor, Theilmann, Julaton, Croteau, Wolfson, Heaton, Gamst, Franklin, Clifford, Collier, Gelman, Marra, McArthur, McCutchan, Morgello, Simpson and Grant2013; Fennema-Notestine et al., Reference Fennema-Notestine, McEvoy, Notestine, Panizzon, Yau, Franz, Lyons, Eyler, Neale, Xian, McKenzie and Kremen2016; Jernigan et al., Reference Jernigan, Archibald, Fennema-Notestine, Taylor, Theilmann, Julaton, Notestine, Wolfson, Letendre, Ellis, Heaton, Gamst, Franklin, Clifford, Collier, Gelman, Marra, McArthur, McCutchan, Morgello, Simpson and Grant2011).

FreeSurfer (Dale et al., Reference Dale, Fischl and Sereno1999; Desikan et al., Reference Desikan, Segonne, Fischl, Quinn, Dickerson, Blacker and Killiany2006; Fischl, Reference Fischl2012) version 7.1.1 was used to obtain cortical thickness and subcortical volume measures (similar to previous work Fennema-Notestine et al., Reference Fennema-Notestine, Panizzon, Thompson, Chen, Eyler, Fischl, Franz, Grant, Jak, Jernigan, Lyons, Neale, Seidman, Tsuang, Xian, Dale, Kremen, Ashford, Rosen, Adamson, Bayley, Sabri, Furst, Black and Weiner2011; Lansing et al., Reference Lansing, Virk, Notestine, Plante and Fennema-Notestine2016). All T1 scans were visually inspected to check for proper segmentation; in addition to the one participant excluded from all analyses as described above, one participant’s hippocampal data were excluded from analysis given inaccurate probabilistic segmentation (overestimation). Neocortical thickness regions of interest included MTL structures (i.e., entorhinal and parahippocampal), and prefrontal (i.e., rostral and caudal midfrontal areas; inferior frontal regions of pars opercularis, pars triangularis, and pars orbitalis) cortical areas. Volumetric subcortical regions of interest included the hippocampus (MTL structure) as well as the basal ganglia (caudate nucleus and putamen). Each structure was analyzed separately as bilateral estimates; left and right volumes or cortical thicknesses for these regions of interest were averaged.

The differences in scanner from site to site were corrected by regressing scanner from the data prior to analyses, given that differences in estimated values across scanners have been well-documented (Fennema-Notestine et al., Reference Fennema-Notestine, Gamst, Quinn, Pacheco, Jernigan, Thal, Buckner, Killiany, Blacker, Dale, Fischl, Dickerson and Gollub2007; Jernigan et al., Reference Jernigan, Archibald, Fennema-Notestine, Taylor, Theilmann, Julaton, Notestine, Wolfson, Letendre, Ellis, Heaton, Gamst, Franklin, Clifford, Collier, Gelman, Marra, McArthur, McCutchan, Morgello, Simpson and Grant2011). For volumetric data, differences in head size were accounted for by including estimated total ICV as a covariate. Mean overall cortical thickness was included as a covariate in regional cortical thickness analyses. Additionally, age was included as a covariate to adjust for the normal age differences in the brain.

Data analytic approach

Findings were considered significant at p < 0.05. The false discovery rate method was utilized to correct for multiple-comparisons for the main analyses (Benjamini & Hochberg, Reference Benjamini and Hochberg1995). JMP Pro 16 statistical software was used for aim 1, and R version 4.2.1 software was used to examine aim 2.

Aim 1

Logistic regression was used for dichotomous recognition analyses. A series of multivariable linear regressions were used to model individual continuous outcomes (i.e., delayed recall, processing speed, psychomotor skills T-scores). Imaging variables (i.e., thickness of MTL and prefrontal structures and volume of MTL and basal ganglia structures) were tested separately.

Age and imaging covariates (i.e., ICV or mean cortical thickness) were included in every model; scanner effects were accounted for prior to these analyses as noted above. The number of prior neuropsychological evaluations was included as a covariate in recognition models. Additional covariates (i.e., demographic variables (excluding site), comorbid conditions, HIV disease characteristics, and APOE status are listed in Table 1) were selected by evaluating the bivariate relationships between potential covariates and outcomes. If a potential covariate was associated with an outcome at p < 0.10 it was then entered as a covariate. Covariates were retained in the model if the covariate remained associated with the outcome (via backwards selection) at p < 0.10. Using this methodology, AIDS status and APOE group were included as covariates in delayed recall analyses, and estimated duration of HIV disease, viral detectability, and race/ethnicity were included as covariates in psychomotor analyses. For recognition memory and processing speed no additional covariates were included.

Aim 2

This aim utilized multi-level modeling to examine dichotomous recognition and continuous delayed recall across follow-up visits. Outcomes (i.e., recognition and delayed recall) were examined separately. The “lme4 version 1.1–30” R package was used to conduct mixed-effects regressions (Bates et al., 2014). Mixed-effects logistic regression models were used to examine dichotomous recognition as the outcome. Linear mixed-effects models were used to examine continuous delayed recall as the outcome. Analyses included a random intercept and a random effect for years since baseline (modeled continuously). A cross-level interaction was used to test if baseline MTL structures (i.e., hippocampus, entorhinal cortex, parahippocampal gyrus) is associated with longitudinal recognition impairment or decline in delayed recall. Between-persons covariates included age at baseline, imaging covariate, and covariates identified in aim 1.

Results

Participant characteristics

Baseline demographic and clinical characteristics are displayed in Table 1. Delayed recognition and recall were correlated at = 0.358 (p < 0.001). Of the 18 participants who were impaired on delayed recall, 7 were impaired and 11 were not impaired on recognition.

Aim 1: Examining the neuroanatomical correlates of memory using structural neuroimaging data

Delayed Recognition

MTL and basal ganglia structures were not significantly associated with odds of being classified as having a recognition impairment—with odds ratios close to one indicating little association. Within the prefrontal cortex, thinner pars opercularis thickness was associated with greater odds of being impaired on recognition (OR = 0.336 for every one standard deviation increase in pars opercularis thickness, p = 0.012); however, this finding was just short of significance after correcting for multiple comparisons (p = 0.060). No other prefrontal cortex structures were associated with being classified as having a recognition impairment. See Table 2 for model statistics.

Table 2. Logistic and multivariable regression examining the relationship between the medial temporal lobe, prefrontal cortex, and basal ganglia and likelihood of recognition impairment

Note: Logits and odds ratios were calculated for a 1 standard deviation increase in the medial temporal lobe, prefrontal cortex, or basal ganglia volumes. Reference group for recognition is Unimpaired Recognition. All models include age, imaging covariate (ICV or mean cortical thickness), and number of previous neuropsychological assessments. Recognition represents the average demographically-corrected Z-score of the two recognition discrimination measures; impairment was defined as a Z-score < −1.0.

*p<0.05 before correcting for multiple comparisons.

**p<0.05 after correcting for multiple comparisons.

Delayed Recall

Worse delayed recall was significantly associated with thinner rostral middle frontal cortex (β = 0.40, p = 0.006) and thinner pars opercularis cortex (β = 0.46, p = 0.001). These associations remained significant after correcting for multiple comparisons (p = 0.005 and p = 0.015, respectively). See Figure 1 for significant relationships between delayed recall and prefrontal structures and Table 3 for model statistics. In these models examining the rostral middle frontal cortex and the pars operculars, AIDS diagnosis (β = 0.32, p = 0.003; β = 0.36, p < 0.001, respectively) and being in the APOE ε4+ group (β = 0.19, p = 0.061; β = 0.28, p = 0.007) were associated with worse delayed recall. Delayed recall was not significantly associated with MTL or basal ganglia structures.

Figure 1. Significant relationships between delayed recall and prefrontal cortex in the entire sample. Note. Regression lines adjust for age (centered), AIDS status (reference: non-AIDS), APOE group (reference: APOE 4- group) and mean cortical thickness (centered).

Table 3. Multivariable linear regressions examining the relationship between the medial temporal lobe, prefrontal cortex, and basal ganglia and delayed recall

Note: All models include age, imaging covariate (ICV or mean cortical thickness), AIDS Status, and APOE status.

* p<0.05 before correcting for multiple comparisons.

** p<0.05 after correcting for multiple comparisons.

Given possible effects of ART and viral detectability on cognition and the brain as well as the known effects of methamphetamine on the brain and cognition, post hoc sensitivity analyses excluded participants who were not on ART, had a detectable viral load, or those with a current methamphetamine use disorder (note, methamphetamine was the only current substance use disorder at baseline other than alcohol or cannabis). In total, 62 participants were in post hoc analyses, and the findings were similar, as thinner rostral middle frontal (β = 0.58, p = 0.004) and thinner pars opercularis (β = 0.47, p = 0.016) remained significantly associated with worse delayed recall. Other prefrontal structures were not associated with delayed recall (ps > 0.156). Note, recognition models were not re-examined given that, with these exclusions, only 7 participants were impaired on the recognition composite. We also examined if results would differ if we examined those that were impaired on memory (either recognition or recall; n = 22). Again, we did not see any involvement of the MTL (ps > 0.450), with minmal effects. Other brain regions were also non-significant, but the effect of rostral middle frontal was similar to the overall sample (β = 0.45, p = 0.440), but the effect of the pars opercularis was reduced (β = 0.13, p = 0.764).

Processing speed and psychomotor functioning

Processing speed (ps > 0.164) and psychomotor functioning (ps > 0.142) were not associated with the MTL, prefrontal cortex, or basal ganglia.

Aim 2: To examine if baseline structural neuroimaging predicts amnestic decline

Ninety-one participants had at least one follow-up visit and were included in these analyses. Participants were followed for an average of 6.5 years (range: 0.6–11.9 years) with an average of 6.6 (range: 1–11) follow-up neuropsychological assessments. Thirty-six (39.6%) with at least one follow-up visit had a consistently undetectable viral load (<50 copies/ml; not including 67 counts of missing data). Sixty-nine participants (75.8%) were on ART for all visits (excluding 16 counts of missing data across all timepoints). No participants converted to AIDS during follow-up. Regarding substance use, 16 participants (17.6%) met criteria for a drug or alcohol use disorder during the study (excluding 9 counts of missing data across all timepoints).

Delayed Recognition

Of the 12 participants who were impaired on recognition at baseline, only two were impaired across all follow-up visits. Two of the 12 participants were not impaired at any subsequent visit, and the other eight were impaired between 25% and 86% of follow-up visits. Of the 79 participants unimpaired in recognition at baseline, 44 (56%) were unimpaired for all visits. Of those that were impaired on recognition at any follow-up visit, only four participants showed consistent recognition impairment; however, these participants had a limited follow-up after the initial recognition impairment (i.e., 1–2 visits). We further examined recognition impairment by specific measure (i.e., HVLT-R and BVMT-R) and both demonstrated considerably variability across time. Recognition composite values across time are depicted in Figure 2. The simplest random-slope and random-intercept model did not converge, and some dimensions of the variance-covariance matrix were estimated at exactly zero. This result indicates poor model fit, and this method could not examine a cross-level interaction.

Figure 2. Recognition composite Z-scores and recall T-scores graphed across years since baseline. Note. Dashed line on the left panel depicts the -1 standard deviation cut-off. Participant visits below this line were impared on recogniton. Solid black line on the right panel depicts the average change in delayed recall over time (i.e., slope).

Delayed Recall

In the random slopes and random intercept model with no additional covariates, the average slope was –0.041, indicating that, on average, the delayed recall T-score decreased by 0.041 every year. The standard deviation of the slope was 0.678. In the model including all covariates (i.e., imaging outcome covariate (e.g. ICV), age, APOE group, and AIDS status), none of the years since baseline by MTL cross-level interactions were significant (ps > 0.412) indicating that MTL volumes did not moderate delayed recall performance over time. See Table 4 for full model statistics. Delayed recall scores across time and slope are depicted in Figure 2.

Table 4. Linear mixed-effects results examining whether baseline medial temporal lobe structures are associated with change in delayed recall

Note: ICV = intracranial volume.

Given that the rostral middle frontal and pars opercularis were associated with delayed recall at baseline, we conducted post hoc analyses to examine if prefrontal structures were associated with delayed recall longitudinally; none of the cross-level interactions were significant (ps > 0.157). In post hoc analyses, we also examined if those who performed in the impaired range on either recognition or recall at baseline (n = 22) were more vulnerable to decline. In the model with no covariates, the average slope was 0.566, indicating that, on average, the delayed recall T-score significantly increased by 0.566 every year (p = 0.009). MTL and significant prefonfrontal regions by time interactions were examined, and none were significant but there was a trend towards a greater entorhinal thickness being associated with greater improvement across time (beta = 0.262; p = 0.083).

Discussion

We examined the relationship between episodic memory, particularly delayed recognition and recall, and brain integrity both cross-sectionally and longitudinally in middle-aged PWH without significant confounding comorbid conditions. This is one of the first studies to use neuroimaging to specifically address the concern that accelerated cognitive aging in mid-life PWH may be due to multiple etiologies such as AD as well as use neuroimaging to inform neuropsychological findings. This study did not support our hypothesis that recognition is associated with more AD-related markers, such as the integrity of the MTL. It did demonstrate a potential association between delayed recall and recognition in middle-aged PWH in frontal systems, which is known to occur in HIV. Regarding longitudinal follow-up, the recognition discrimination composite was quite variable over time, indicating that in this middle-age range, it may not be useful in discriminating PWH with HAND compared to an early AD trajectory. Moreover, there was minimal longitudinal decline in delayed recall over multiple years among this group of middle-aged PWH, and thus we did not find evidence of an accelerated memory decline even when restricting to those at possibly greater risk of decline (i.e., those that were impaired at baseline). Taken together, episodic memory in middle-aged PWH appears to be more associated with frontally mediated involvement possibly due to HIV and, on average, we did not see concerning signs that accelerated memory decline reported in the literature is driven primarily by AD.

In our cohort of PWH, there was no significant association found between MTL structures and impairment on delayed recognition or recall. Effect sizes were minimal, indicating clinically insignificant associations. However, a thinner pars opercularis was significantly associated with greater odds of impairment in delayed recognition, although the finding fell to trend-level after correcting for multiple comparisons. Also, thicker rostral middle frontal gyrus and pars opercularis were associated with better delayed recall. This association remained when limiting the sample to virally suppressed PWH. Given that delayed recall was examined as a continuous variable, this does not imply that these prefrontal regions are associated with delayed recall impairment per se, rather with performance generally. Moreover, including overall mean cortical thickness in the models as a covariate allowed for observing associations with specific cortical regions while controlling for average overall cortical thickness. Clinical research on persons with HIV has consistently shown structural changes throughout the brain, including frontal regions, compared to persons without HIV (Ances & Hammoud, Reference Ances and Hammoud2014; Holt et al., Reference Holt, Kraft-Terry and Chang2012), along with accelerated “brain aging” in middle-aged and older PWH (Clifford et al., Reference Clifford, Samboju, Cobigo, Milanini, Marx, Hellmuth, Rosen, Kramer, Allen and Valcour2017; Milanini et al., Reference Milanini, Samboju, Cobigo, Paul, Javandel, Hellmuth, Allen, Miller and Valcour2019). Thus, it is possible that changes in the prefrontal cortex are contributing to the observed associations with memory in this population.

Comparing the results of this study to the literature on middle aged samples in persons without HIV is difficult. Many such studies examine a memory composite and do not consider possible associations between brain integrity and delayed recognition. Additionally, many studies focus on the MTL. From the limited midlife research that examines both memory and region-specific neuroimaging, there is some indication that memory is associated with several neuroimaging correlates, including integrity of the MTL. For example, the Wisconsin Registry for Alzheimer’s Prevention (WRAP) study, focusing on adults aged 40–65, has found some association between poorer memory and worse MTL indices (Doherty et al., Reference Doherty, Schultz, Oh, Koscik, Dowling, Barnhart, Murali, Gallagher, Carlsson, Bendlin, LaRue, Hermann, Rowley, Asthana, Sager, Christian, Johnson and Okonkwo2015; Schultz et al., Reference Schultz, Oh, Koscik, Dowling, Gallagher, Carlsson, Bendlin, LaRue, Hermann, Rowley, Asthana, Sager, Johnson and Okonkwo2015), but did not examine prefrontal structures. Of note, the WRAP study differs from the CHARTER demographics in terms of both race/ethnicity and sex/gender.

It is difficult to differentiate the effect of HIV itself versus lifestyle factors associated with risk of contracting HIV and the downstream effects of HIV (e.g., increased risk of cerebro/cardiovascular disease). Cysique & Brew (2019) propose that vascular abnormalities are implicated in the pathogenesis of neurocognitive impairment in PWH, and a recent meta-analysis by McIntosh et al., (2021) found that cardiovascular disease is associated with an increased risk of cognitive impairment in PWH. CVD has also been associated with brain changes in PWH (Calon et al., Reference Calon, Menon, Carr, Henry, Rae, Brew and Cysique2020; Samboju et al., Reference Samboju, Cobigo, Paul, Naasan, Hillis, Tsuei, Javandel, Valcour and Milanini2021). Several vascular risk factors were examined as covariates in the current study and were not significantly associated with cognitive outcomes, although participants with more significant vascular comorbidities (e.g., stroke or myocardial infarction) were excluded from analyses. Therefore, further exploration of vascular risk factors and their association with cognition and brain aging in PWH more broadly is warranted.

Given our finding that episodic memory in middle-aged PWH may be more likely related to frontally mediated etiologies, this pattern of association may indicate that at this age range, preclinical AD is not likely a contributor to memory functioning. However, the existing literature does not provide a good estimate of when to expect differences in memory and medial temporal structures in those that are on an AD trajectory; therefore, it is possible that the current CHARTER group is too young to appreciably detect preclinical AD effects. This is further complicated by the demographically disparate composition of our CHARTER sample from most other studies in this area (e.g., in terms of race/ethnicity representation).

Regarding longitudinal follow-up in the current study, delayed recognition impairment status was variable over time. This observation could reflect the heterogeneous and fluctuating course of HAND over time (Heaton et al., Reference Heaton, Franklin, Deutsch, Letendre, Ellis, Casaletto, Marquine, Woods, Vaida, Atkinson, Marcotte, McCutchan, Collier, Marra, Clifford, Gelman, Sacktor, Morgello, Simpson and Teshome2015). Additionally, this variability over time may be in part due to the psychometric properties of the HVLT-R and the BVMT-R. Recognition for both the BVMT-R and the HVLT-R are skewed with known ceiling effects, meaning that there is more limited variability in this measure (Benedict et al., Reference Benedict, Schretlen, Groninger and Brandt1998; Benedict, 1997). Additionally, the HVLT-R and BVMT-R test-retest reliability of delayed recognition scores show adequate test-retest stability coefficients, but test-retest reliability of recognition is less reliable than other test measures such as delayed recall (Benedict et al., Reference Benedict, Schretlen, Groninger and Brandt1998; Benedict et al., Reference Benedict, Schretlen, Groninger, Dobraski and Shpritz1996; Strauss et al., Reference Strauss, Sherman and Spreen2006; Woods et al., Reference Woods, Scott, Conover, Marcotte, Heaton and Grant2005). Lastly, it is important to again note that our dichotomous recognition “impairment” classification was not corrected for practice effects, so small fluctuations across the “impairment” cutoff could be due to practice effects rather than lack of reliability. Ultimately, combining two measures with some psychometric concerns may have led to greater variability across time; however, both measures individually were quite variable across time. This, in addition to the lack of MTL findings, leads us to conclude that recognition impairment at one timepoint in this group may not be indicative of future decline associated with neurodegenerative disease. Future studies may want to employ of additional memory tests (e.g., story memory) or those that are either somewhat harder (and thus could have greater variability) or have better psychometric properties for the recognition trial.

Notably, there was little decline in delayed recall over time in our sample of PWH. While some individuals in this group may be experiencing objective decline, on average, we did not observe a decline in age-corrected delayed recall T-scores over time. Other studies of primarily middle-aged PWH have observed a greater-than-expected effect of aging on episodic memory (e.g., in the Multicenter AIDS Cohort Study (Goodkin et al., Reference Goodkin, Miller, Cox, Reynolds, Becker, Martin, Selnes, Ostrow and Sacktor2017)), and a recent systematic review found accelerated neurocognitive aging in 75% of longitudinal studies in PWH (Aung et al., Reference Aung, Aghvinian, Gouse, Robbins, Brew, Mao and Cysique2020). Some researchers have questioned if accelerated aging could be due to a neurodegenerative cause such as AD. Emerging studies have demonstrated some possible ways to disentangle HAND and aMCI (e.g., olfaction, memory performance; Sundermann et al., Reference Sundermann, Bondi, Campbell, Gouaux, Moore, Soontornniyomkij and Moore2021a; Sundermann et al., Reference Sundermann, Fields, Saloner, Gouaux, Bharti, Murphy and Moore2021b); yet, it remains unclear if PWH are at increased risk of AD and if AD could account for some of the observed accelerated aging. For example, Milanini et al. (Reference Milanini, Javandel, Hillis, Chen, Hellmuth, Paul, Pohl and Valcour2020) showed a low frequency of amyloid positivity among virally suppressed PWH over the age of 60. However, studies using insurance enrollee data found a higher prevalence of AD and related disorders among PWH (Yang et al., Reference Yang, Beymer and Suen2019; Yu et al., Reference Yu, Raji, Giordano, Berenson, Baillargeon and Kuo2022). Overall, we did not detect clear signs of preclinical AD in our study. Again, it is not clear if these associations should be expected in a middle-aged cohort of PWH; therefore, it would be beneficial to re-examine this analysis in an older cohort of PWH.

Additional limitations should be considered. First, in terms of generalizability, the sample was predominantly male (i.e., 85.7%), which somewhat reflects of the current demographics of PWH in the United States (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2017). Nevertheless, there are known sex differences on cognition in HIV and AD (Dreyer et al., Reference Dreyer, Munsami, Williams, Andersen, Nightingale, Gouse, Joska and Thomas2022; Laws et al., Reference Laws, Irvine and Gale2018) that this project was underpowered to test. Relatedly, all participants in this analysis did not have significant “confounding” comorbid conditions and most, although not all, were on suppressive ART; therefore, these findings may not generalize to individuals that are discordant from our group. Second, this study is also limited in that our CHARTER sample does not include an HIV-negative comparison group, and many prior studies are demographically and psychosocially disparate from our group. Future studies would benefit from a demographically and psychosocially diverse HIV-negative comparison group to better understand the specificity of associations between memory and neuroimaging correlates in PWH. Third, delayed recall and recognition were examined separately rather than dichotomously splitting participants into aMCI versus non-aMCI as in Sundermann et al. (Reference Sundermann, Bondi, Campbell, Gouaux, Moore, Soontornniyomkij and Moore2021a). Examining delayed recognition and recall individually was an important first step to inform future diagnostic improvements. Additionally, examining delayed recall continuously was advantageous because it increased variability; more subtle differences observed in mid-life may not be captured by inflexible diagnostic cut-points. With that said, associations between biological markers associated with AD (e.g., olfactory dysfunction, elevated rates of Aβ42 plaques) have been found in older (age 50+) PWH using aMCI criteria (Sundermann et al., Reference Sundermann, Bondi, Campbell, Gouaux, Moore, Soontornniyomkij and Moore2021a; Sundermann et al., Reference Sundermann, Fields, Saloner, Gouaux, Bharti, Murphy and Moore2021b). Therefore, future studies could consider using a more comprehensive approach to examining episodic memory.

This study has several clinical implications. It showed that memory in these middle-aged participants was associated with prefrontal structures but not MTL structures, suggesting that episodic memory in middle-aged PWH is more associated with frontally mediated involvement in HIV rather than etiologies associated with the MTL. It will be important to replicate this study in older (e.g., aged 65 and up) PWH to examine if this trend continues into older age, particularly as greater numbers of PWH reach this age range. The findings also suggest that in middle-aged PWH with high rates of ART use and relatively limited comorbid conditions, there may not be a greater than expected decline in delayed recall. Lastly, recognition “impairment” appeared to be quite variable over time. Due to this variability over time, recognition may not serve as a good clinical marker to help distinguish aMCI from HAND in this middle-aged group; however, research examining recognition trajectories among older PHW at higher risk for AD is needed.

Acknowledgements

The CNS HIV Anti-Retroviral Therapy Effects Research was supported by awards N01 MH22005, HHSN271201000036C, HHSN271201000030C, R01 MH107345, and R24 MH129166 from the National Institutes of Health.

The CNS HIV Anti-Retroviral Therapy Effects Research (CHARTER) group is affiliated with the Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai; Johns Hopkins University; University of California, San Diego; University of Texas, Galveston; University of Washington, Seattle; Washington University, St Louis; and is headquartered at the University of California, San Diego and includes: Directors: Scott L. Letendre, M.D. Ronald J Ellis, M.D., Ph.D.; Center Manager: Donald Franklin, Jr.; Coordinating Center: Averi Barrett, M.S., Amanda Bischoff-Grethe, Ph.D., Debra Cookson, M.P.H, Clint Cushman, Matthew Dawson, Ph.D., Christine Fennema Notestine, Ph.D., Robert K. Heaton, Ph.D., Jennifer Marquie-Beck, M.P.H., David J. Moore, Ph.D., Rebecca Theilmann, Ph.D., Florin Vaida, Ph.D.; Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai Site: Susan Morgello, M.D. (MPI.), Jairo Gonzalez, Psy.D. (MPI); Johns Hopkins University Site: Leah Rubin, Ph.D. (MPI), Jennifer Coughlin, M.D. (MPI); University of California, San Diego Site: Ronald J. Ellis, M.D., Ph.D. (P.I.); University of Washington, Seattle Site: Payal Patel, M.D. (P.I.); University of Texas, Galveston Site: Benjamin Gelman, M.D., Ph.D. (P.I.).; and Washington University, St Louis Site: David Clifford, M.D. (P.I.)

The views expressed in this article are those of the authors and do not reflect the official policy or position of the United States Government.

Funding statement

This research was made possible by the National Institute on Aging (L.M.C., grant number F31 AG067869); the National Institute for Mental Health (R. K. H., grant number P30 MH62512), (I. G., grant number N01 MH2205), (I. G. grant number HHSN271201000036C), (R. K. H. and S. L., grant number R01 MH107345); the Diana Jacobs Kalman/American Federation for Aging Research (AFAR) Scholarship for Research in the Biology of Aging (L.M.C.); and the San Diego State University/UC San Diego Joint Doctoral Program’s Gerald Rosenbaum Neuropsychology Dissertation Research Award (L.M.C.).

Competing interests

RCM is a co-founder and has equity interest in KeyWise, Inc. and NeuroUX, Inc. The terms of this arrangement have been reviewed and approved by the University of California, San Diego in accordance with its conflict of interest policies.

References

Alakkas, A., Ellis, R. J., Watson, C. W.-M., Umlauf, A., Heaton, R. K., Letendre, S., Collier, A., Marra, C., Clifford, D. B., Gelman, B., Sacktor, N., Morgello, S., Simpson, D., McCutchan, J. A., Kallianpur, A., Gianella, S., Marcotte, T., Grant, I., & Fennema-Notestine, C. (2019). White matter damage, neuroinflammation, and neuronal integrity in HAND. Journal of Neurovirology, 25(1), 3241.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alzheimer’s Association (2017). 2017 Alzheimer’s disease facts and figures. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 13(4), 325373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ances, B. M., & Hammoud, D. A. (2014). Neuroimaging of HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders (HAND). Current Opinion in HIV and AIDS, 9(6), 545551.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Antinori, A., Arendt, G., Becker, J. T., Brew, B. J., Byrd, D. A., Cherner, M., Clifford, D. B., Cinque, P., Epstein, L. G., Goodkin, K., Gisslen, M., Grant, I., Heaton, R. K., Joseph, J., Marder, K., Marra, C. M., McArthur, J. C., Nunn, M., Price, R. W., Pulliam, L., Robertson, K. R., Sacktor, N., Valcour, V., & Wojna, V. E. (2007). Updated research nosology for HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders. Neurology, 69(18), 17891799. https://doi.org/10.1212/01.WNL.0000287431.88658.8b CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aung, H. L., Aghvinian, M., Gouse, H., Robbins, R. N., Brew, B. J., Mao, L., & Cysique, L. A. (2020). Is there any evidence of premature, accentuated and accelerated aging effects on neurocognition in people living with HIV? A systematic review. AIDS and Behavior, 25(3), 144.Google ScholarPubMed
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2014). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. arXiv preprint arXiv:1406.5823.Google Scholar
Benedict, R. H. (1997). Brief visuospatial memory test--revised: professional manual. PAR.Google Scholar
Benedict, R. H., Schretlen, D., Groninger, L., & Brandt, J. (1998). Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised: Normative data and analysis of inter-form and test-retest reliability. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 12(1), 4355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benedict, R. H. B., Schretlen, D., Groninger, L., Dobraski, M., & Shpritz, B. (1996). Revision of the brief visuospatial memory test: Studies of normal performance, reliability, and validity. Psychological Assessment, 8(2), 145153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B (Methodological), 57(1), 289300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Calcagno, A., Celani, L., Trunfio, M., Orofino, G., Imperiale, D., Atzori, C., Arena, V., d’Ettorre, G., Guaraldi, G., Gisslen, M., & Di Perri, G. (2021). Alzheimer dementia in people living with HIV. Neurology Clinical Practice, 11(5), e627e633. https://doi.org/10.1212/CPJ.0000000000001060 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calon, M., Menon, K., Carr, A., Henry, R. G., Rae, C. D., Brew, B. J., & Cysique, L. A. (2020). Additive and synergistic cardiovascular disease risk gactors and HIV disease markers’ effects on white matter microstructure in virally suppressed HIV. Journal of Acquired Immune Deficiency Syndrome, 84(5), 543551. https://doi.org/10.1097/QAI.0000000000002390 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Centers for Disease Control & Prevention. (2018). HIV among people aged 50 & over. Centers for Disease Control & Prevention. Retrieved from https://www.cdc.gov/hiv/group/age/olderamericans/index.html Google Scholar
Centers for Disease Control & Prevention. (2017). HIV among men in the United States. Centers for Disease Control & Prevention. Retrieved from https://www.cdc.gov/hiv/group/gender/men/index.html#refb,Google Scholar
Clark, L. R., Koscik, R. L., Nicholas, C. R., Okonkwo, O. C., Engelman, C. D., Bratzke, L. C., …Johnson, S. C. (2016). Mild cognitive impairment in late middle age in the Wisconsin registry for Alzheimer’s prevention study: Prevalence and characteristics using robust and standard neuropsychological normative data. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 31(7), 675688. https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acw024 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clifford, D. B., & Ances, B. M. (2013). HIV-associated neurocognitive disorder. Lancet Infectious Disease, 13(11), 976986. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1473-3099(13)70269-X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clifford, K. M., Samboju, V., Cobigo, Y., Milanini, B., Marx, G. A., Hellmuth, J. M., Rosen, H. J., Kramer, J. H., Allen, I. E., & Valcour, V. G. (2017). Progressive brain atrophy despite persistent viral suppression in HIV over age 60. JAIDS Journal of Acquired Immune Deficiency Syndromes, 76(3), 289297.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cysique, L. A., Franklin, D. Jr, Abramson, I., Ellis, R. J., Letendre, S., Collier, A., Clifford, D., Gelman, B., McArthur, J., Morgello, S., Simpson, D., McCutchan, J. A., Grant, I., & Heaton, R. K. (2011). Normative data and validation of a regression based summary score for assessing meaningful neuropsychological change. Journal of Clinical Experimental Neuropsychology, 33(5), 505522. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2010.535504.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dale, A. M., Fischl, B., & Sereno, M. I. (1999). Cortical surface-based analysis. I. segmentation and surface reconstruction. Neuroimage, 9(2), 179194. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.1998.0395.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Desikan, R. S., Segonne, F., Fischl, B., Quinn, B. T., Dickerson, B. C., Blacker, D., …Killiany, R. J. (2006). An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. NeuroImage, 31(3), 968980. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doherty, B. M., Schultz, S. A., Oh, J. M., Koscik, R. L., Dowling, N. M., Barnhart, T. E., Murali, D., Gallagher, C. L., Carlsson, C. M., Bendlin, B. B., LaRue, A., Hermann, B. P., Rowley, H. A., Asthana, S., Sager, M. A., Christian, B. T., Johnson, S. C., & Okonkwo, O. C. (2015). Amyloid burden, cortical thickness, and cognitive function in the Wisconsin Registry for Alzheimer’s prevention. Alzheimers Dement (Amst), 1(2), 160169. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dadm.2015.01.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dreyer, A. J., Munsami, A., Williams, T., Andersen, L. S., Nightingale, S., Gouse, H., Joska, J., & Thomas, K. G. F. (2022). Cognitive differences between men and women with HIV: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 37(2), 479496.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edmonds, E. C., Delano-Wood, L., Clark, L. R., Jak, A. J., Nation, D. A., McDonald, C. R., Libon, D. J., Au, R., Galasko, D., Salmon, D. P., Bondi, M. W., & Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging, I. (2015). Susceptibility of the conventional criteria for mild cognitive impairment to false-positive diagnostic errors. Alzheimer’s Dementia, 11(4), 415424. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2014.03.005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fennema-Notestine, C., Ellis, R. J., Archibald, S. L., Jernigan, T. L., Letendre, S. L., Notestine, R. J., Taylor, M. J., Theilmann, R. J., Julaton, M. D., Croteau, D. J., Wolfson, T., Heaton, R. K., Gamst, A. C., Franklin, D. R. Jr., Clifford, D. B., Collier, A. C., Gelman, B. B., Marra, C., McArthur, J. C., McCutchan, J. A., Morgello, S., Simpson, D. M., & Grant, I. (2013). Increases in brain white matter abnormalities and subcortical gray matter are linked to CD4 recovery in HIV infection. Journal of NeuroVirology, 19(4), 393401. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13365-013-0185-7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fennema-Notestine, C., Gamst, A. C., Quinn, B. T., Pacheco, J., Jernigan, T. L., Thal, L., Buckner, R., Killiany, R., Blacker, D., Dale, A. M., Fischl, B., Dickerson, B., & Gollub, R. L. (2007). Feasibility of multi-site clinical structural neuroimaging studies of aging using legacy data. Neuroinformatics, 5(4), 235245. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12021-007-9003-9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fennema-Notestine, C., McEvoy, L. K., Notestine, R., Panizzon, M. S., Yau, W.-Y. W., Franz, C. E., Lyons, M. J., Eyler, L. T., Neale, M. C., Xian, H., McKenzie, R. E., & Kremen, W. S. (2016). White matter disease in midlife is heritable, related to hypertension, and shares some genetic influence with systolic blood pressure. NeuroImage: Clinical, 12, 737745. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2016.10.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fennema-Notestine, C., Panizzon, M. S., Thompson, W. R., Chen, C.-H., Eyler, L. T., Fischl, B., Franz, C. E., Grant, M. D., Jak, A. J., Jernigan, T. L., Lyons, M. J., Neale, M. C., Seidman, L. J., Tsuang, M. T., Xian, H., Dale, A. M., Kremen, W. S., Ashford, J. W., Rosen, A., Adamson, M., Bayley, P., Sabri, O., Furst, A., Black, S. E., & Weiner, M. (2011). Presence of ApoE epsilon4 allele associated with thinner frontal cortex in middle age. Journal of Alzheimers Disease 26, 26(Suppl 3), 4960. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-2011-0002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fischl, B. (2012). Freesurfer. NeuroImage, 62(2), 774781. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.021.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodkin, K., Miller, E. N., Cox, C., Reynolds, S., Becker, J. T., Martin, E., Selnes, O. A., Ostrow, D. G., & Sacktor, N. C. (2017). Effect of ageing on neurocognitive function by stage of HIV infection: Evidence from the multicenter AIDS cohort study. The Lancet HIV, 4(9), e411e422. https://doi.org/10.1016/s2352-3018(17)30098-x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heaton, R. K., Clifford, D. B., Franklin, D. R. Jr, Woods, S. P., Ake, C., Vaida, F., Ellis, R. J., Letendre, S. L., Marcotte, T. D., Atkinson, J. H., Rivera-Mindt, M., Vigil, O. R., Taylor, M. J., Collier, A. C., Marra, C. M., Gelman, B. B., McArthur, J. C., Morgello, S., Simpson, D. M., McCutchan, J. A., Abramson, I., Gamst, A., Fennema-Notestine, C., Jernigan, T. L., & Wong, J., (2010). HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders persist in the era of potent antiretroviral therapy: CHARTER study. Neurology, 75(23), 208712096. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0b013e318200d727.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heaton, R. K., Franklin, D. R., Ellis, R. J., McCutchan, J. A., Letendre, S. L., LeBlanc, S., Corkran, S. H., Duarte, N. A., Clifford, D. B., Woods, S. P., Collier, A. C., Marra, C. M., Morgello, S., Mindt, M. R., Taylor, M. J., Marcotte, T. D., Atkinson, J. H., Wolfson, T., Gelman, B. B., McArthur, J. C., Simpson, D. M., Abramson, I., Gamst, A., Fennema-Notestine, C., Jernigan, T. L., Wong, J., & Grant, I. (2011). HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders before and during the era of combination antiretroviral therapy: Differences in rates, nature, and predictors. Journal of NeuroVirology, 17(1), 316. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13365-010-0006-1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heaton, R. K., Franklin, D. R., Deutsch, R., Letendre, S., Ellis, R. J., Casaletto, K., Marquine, M. J., Woods, S. P., Vaida, F., Atkinson, J. H., Marcotte, T. D., McCutchan, J. A., Collier, A. C., Marra, C. M., Clifford, D. B., Gelman, B. B., Sacktor, N., Morgello, S., Simpson, D. M., …, Teshome, M. (2015). Neurocognitive change in the era of HIV combination antiretroviral therapy: The longitudinal CHARTER study. Clinical Infectious Diseases, 60(3), 473480. https://doi.org/10.1093/cid/ciu862.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heaton, R. K., Taylor, M., & Manly, J. (2003). Demographic effects and use of demographically corrected norms with the WAIS-III and WMS-III. In David S. Tulsky, Donald H. Saklofske, Robert K. Heaton, Robert Bornstein, Mark K. Ledbetter, Gordon J. Chelune, Robert J. Ivnik, & Aurelio Prifitera (Eds.), Clinical Interpretation of the WAIS-III and WMS-III (pp. 181). Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hellmuth, J., Milanini, B., Masliah, E., Tartaglia, M. C., Dunlop, M. B., Moore, D. J., Javandel, S., DeVaughn, S., & Valcour, V. (2018). A neuropathologic diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease in an older adult with HIV-associated neurocognitive disorder. Neurocase, 24(4), 213219. https://doi.org/10.1080/13554794.2018.1530362.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holt, J. L., Kraft-Terry, S. D., & Chang, L. (2012). Neuroimaging studies of the aging HIV-1-infected brain. Journal of NeuroVirology, 18(4), 291302. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13365-012-0114-1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jack, C. R. Jr., Bennett, D. A., Blennow, K., Carrillo, M. C., Dunn, B., Haeberlein, S. B., Holtzman, D. M., Jagust, W., Jessen, F., Karlawish, J., Liu, E., Molinuevo, J. L., Montine, T., Phelps, C., Rankin, K. P., Rowe, C. C., Scheltens, P., Siemers, E., Snyder, H. M., Sperling, R., Masliah, E., Ryan, L., & Silverberg, N. (2018). NIA-AA research framework: Toward a biological definition of Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 14(4), 535562. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2018.02.018.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jak, A. J., Bondi, M. W., Delano-Wood, L., Wierenga, C., Corey-Bloom, J., Salmon, D. P., & Delis, D. C. (2009). Quantification of five neuropsychological approaches to defining mild cognitive impairment. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 17(5), 368375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jak, A. J., Panizzon, M. S., Spoon, K. M., Fennema-Notestine, C., Franz, C. E., Thompson, W. K., Jacobson, K. C., Xian, H., Eyler, L. T., Vuoksimaa, E., Toomey, R., Lyons, M. J., Neale, M. C., Tsuang, M. T., Dale, A. M., & Kremen, W. S. (2015). Hippocampal atrophy varies by neuropsychologically defined MCI among men in their 50s. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 23(5), 456465.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jernigan, T. L., Archibald, S. L., Fennema-Notestine, C., Taylor, M. J., Theilmann, R. J., Julaton, M. D., Notestine, R. J., Wolfson, T., Letendre, S. L., Ellis, R. J., Heaton, R. K., Gamst, A. C., Franklin, D. R. Jr., Clifford, D. B., Collier, A. C., Gelman, B. B., Marra, C., McArthur, J. C., McCutchan, J. A., Morgello, S., Simpson, D. M., & Grant, I. (2011). Clinical factors related to brain structure in HIV: The CHARTER study. Journal of NeuroVirology, 17(3), 248257. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13365-011-0032-7,CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirova, A. M., Bays, R. B., & Lagalwar, S. (2015). Working memory and executive function decline across normal aging, mild cognitive impairment, and Alzheimer’s disease. BioMed Research International, 2015, 748212. http://doi.org/10.1155/2015/748212.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kremen, W. S., Jak, A. J., Panizzon, M. S., Spoon, K. M., Franz, C. E., Thompson, W. K., Jacobson, K. C., Vasilopoulos, T., Vuoksimaa, E., Xian, H., Toomey, R., & Lyons, M. J. (2014). Early identification and heritability of mild cognitive impairment. International Journal of Epidemiology, 43(2), 600610. https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyt242.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhn, T., Kaufmann, T., Doan, N. T., Westlye, L. T., Jones, J., Nunez, R. A., Bookheimer, S. Y., Singer, E. J., Hinkin, C. H., & Thames, A. D. (2018). An augmented aging process in brain white matter in HIV. Human Brain Mapping, 39(6), 25322540. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24019.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lansing, A. E., Virk, A., Notestine, R., Plante, W. Y., & Fennema-Notestine, C. (2016). Cumulative trauma, adversity and grief symptoms associated with fronto-temporal regions in life-course persistent delinquent boys. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 254, 92102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2016.06.007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laws, K. R., Irvine, K., & Gale, T. M. (2018). Sex differences in Alzheimer’s disease. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 31(2), 133139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milanini, B., Javandel, S., Hillis, M., Chen, K., Hellmuth, J., Paul, R., Pohl, K. M., & Valcour, V. (2020). Rates of amyloid PET positivity in geriatric HIV: Neuroimaging/differential diagnosis. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 16, e044134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milanini, B., Samboju, V., Cobigo, Y., Paul, R., Javandel, S., Hellmuth, J., Allen, I., Miller, B., & Valcour, V. (2019). Longitudinal brain atrophy patterns and neuropsychological performance in older adults with HIV-associated neurocognitive disorder compared with early alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiology of Aging, 82, 6976. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2019.07.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milanini, B., & Valcour, V. (2017). Differentiating HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders from Alzheimer’s disease: an Emerging Issue in Geriatric NeuroHIV. Curr HIV/AIDS Reports. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11904-017-0361-0 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morgan, E. E., Woods, S. P., Letendre, S. L., Franklin, D. R., Bloss, C., Goate, A., Heaton, R. K., Collier, A. C., Marra, C. M., Gelman, B. B., McArthur, J. C., Morgello, S., Simpson, D. M., McCutchan, J. A., Ellis, R. J., Abramson, I., Gamst, A., Fennema-Notestine, C., Smith, D. M., Grant, I., Vaida, F., & Clifford, D. B. (2013). Apolipoprotein E4 genotype does not increase risk of HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders. Journal of NeuroVirology, 19(2), 150156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morgello, S., Jacobs, M., Murray, J., Byrd, D., Neibart, E., Mintz, L., Meloni, G., Chon, C., & Crary, J. (2018). Alzheimer’s disease neuropathology may not predict functional impairment in HIV: A report of two individuals. Journal of NeuroVirology, 24(5), 629637. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13365-018-0663-z.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nichols, M. J., Gates, T. M., Soares, J. R., Moffat, K. J., Rae, C. D., Brew, B. J., & Cysique, L. A. (2019). Atrophic brain signatures of mild forms of neurocognitive impairment in virally suppressed HIV infection. AIDS, 33(1), 5566. https://doi.org/10.1097/QAD.0000000000002042.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norman, M. A., Moore, D. J., Taylor, M., Franklin, D. Jr., Cysique, L., Ake, C., Lazarretto, D., Vaida, F., & Heaton, R. K. (2011). Demographically corrected norms for African Americans and caucasians on the hopkins verbal learning test-revised, brief visuospatial memory test-revised, stroop color and word test, and Wisconsin card sorting test 64-card version. Journal of Clinical Experimental Neuropsychology, 33(7), 793804. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2011.559157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Okonkwo, O. C., Oh, J. M., Koscik, R., Jonaitis, E., Cleary, C. A., Dowling, N. M., Bendlin, B. B., LaRue, A., Hermann, B. P., Barnhart, T. E., Murali, D., Rowley, H. A., Carlsson, C. M., Gallagher, C. L., Asthana, S., Sager, M. A., Christian, B. T., & Johnson, S. C. (2014). Amyloid burden, neuronal function, and cognitive decline in middle-aged adults at risk for Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of the International Neuropsychology Society, 20(4), 422433. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617714000113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pasipanodya, E. C., Montoya, J. L., Campbell, L. M., Hussain, M. A., Saloner, R., Paolillo, E. M., Jeste, D. V., Letendre, S. L., McCutchan, J. A., Heaton, R. K., & Moore, D. J. (2019). Metabolic risk factors as differential predictors of profiles of neurocognitive impairment among older HIV+ and HIV-adults: An observational study. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 36(2), 151164. http://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acz040.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paul, R. H., Ernst, T., Brickman, A. M., Yiannoutsos, C. T., Tate, D. F., Cohen, R. A., & Navia, B. A. (2008). Relative sensitivity of magnetic resonance spectroscopy and quantitative magnetic resonance imaging to cognitive function among nondemented individuals infected with HIV. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 14(5), 725733. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617708080910.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pfefferbaum, A., Rogosa, D. A., Rosenbloom, M. J., Chu, W., Sassoon, S. A., Kemper, C. A., Deresinski, S., Rohlfing, T., Zahr, N. M., & Sullivan, E. V. (2014). Accelerated aging of selective brain structures in human immunodeficiency virus infection: A controlled, longitudinal magnetic resonance imaging study. Neurobiology of Aging, 35(7), 17551768. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2014.01.008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rasmussen, K. L., Tybjærg-Hansen, A., Nordestgaard, B. G., & Frikke-Schmidt, R. (2018). Absolute 10-year risk of dementia by age, sex and APOE genotype: A population-based cohort study. Cmaj, 190(35), E1033E1041.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ritchie, K., Ritchie, C. W., Yaffe, K., Skoog, I., & Scarmeas, N. (2015). Is late-onset alzheimer’s disease really a disease of midlife? Alzheimer’s & Dementia: Translational Research & Clinical Interventions, 1(2), 122130.Google ScholarPubMed
Rubin, L. H., Sundermann, E. E., & Moore, D. J. (2019). The current understanding of overlap between characteristics of HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders and alzheimer’s disease. Journal of NeuroVirology, 25(5), 661672. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13365-018-0702-9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salmon, D. P., & Bondi, M. W. (2009). Neuropsychological assessment of dementia. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 257282.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saloner, R., & Cysique, L. A. (2017). HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders: A global perspective. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 23(9-10), 860869. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617717001102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Samboju, V., Cobigo, Y., Paul, R., Naasan, G., Hillis, M., Tsuei, T., Javandel, S., Valcour, V., & Milanini, B. (2021). Cerebrovascular disease correlates with longitudinal brain atrophy in virally suppressed older people living with HIV. Journal of Acquired Immune Deficiency Syndromes, 87(4), 10791085. https://doi.org/10.1097/QAI.0000000000002683.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schultz, S. A., Oh, J. M., Koscik, R. L., Dowling, N. M., Gallagher, C. L., Carlsson, C. M., Bendlin, B. B., LaRue, A., Hermann, B. P., Rowley, H. A., Asthana, S., Sager, M. A., Johnson, S. C., & Okonkwo, O. C. (2015). Subjective memory complaints, cortical thinning, and cognitive dysfunction in middle-age adults at risk of AD. Alzheimer’s & Dementia: Diagnosis, Assessment & Disease Monitoring, 1(1), 3340 Google Scholar
Scott, J. C., Woods, S. P., Carey, C. L., Weber, E., Bondi, M. W., & Grant, I. (2011). Neurocognitive consequences of HIV infection in older adults: An evaluation of the “cortical” hypothesis. AIDS and Behavior, 15(6), 11871196. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10461-010-9815-8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seider, T. R., Luo, X., Gongvatana, A., Devlin, K. N., de la Monte, S. M., Chasman, J. D., Yan, P., Tashima, K. T., Navia, B., & Cohen, R. A. (2014). Verbal memory declines more rapidly with age in HIV infected versus uninfected adults. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 36(4), 356367. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2014.892061.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, G., & Butts, A. (2016). Dementia. In Morgan, J. E., & Ricker, J. H. (Eds.), Textbook of Clinical Neuropsychology (pp. 717742). Taylor & Francis Google Scholar
Strauss, E., Sherman, E. M., & Spreen, O. (2006). A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration, Norms, and Commentary. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sundermann, E. E., Bondi, M. W., Campbell, L. M., Gouaux, B., Moore, R. C., Soontornniyomkij, V., & Moore, D. J. (2021a). Distinguishing amnestic mild cognitive impairment from HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders. Journal of Infectious Diseases, 224(3), 435442. https://doi.org/10.1093/infdis/jiaa760.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sundermann, E. E., Fields, A., Saloner, R., Gouaux, B., Bharti, A., Murphy, C., & Moore, D. J. (2021b). The utility of olfactory function in distinguishing early-stage alzheimer’s disease from HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders. AIDS, 35(3), 429437. https://doi.org/10.1097/QAD.0000000000002761.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sutphen, C. L., Jasielec, M. S., Shah, A. R., Macy, E. M., Xiong, C., Vlassenko, A. G., Benzinger, T. L. S., Stoops, E. E. J., Vanderstichele, H. M. J., Brix, B., Darby, H. D., Vandijck, M. L. J., Ladenson, J. H., Morris, J. C., Holtzman, D. M., & Fagan, A. M. (2015). Longitudinal cerebrospinal fluid biomarker changes in preclinical Alzheimer disease during middle age. JAMA Neurology, 72(9), 10291042. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamaneurol.2015.1285.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, M., Wang, Q., Ding, H., & Shang, H. (2015). Association of hippocampal magnetic resonance imaging with learning and memory deficits in HIV-1-deropositive patients. Journal of Acquired Immune Deficiency Syndrome, 70(4), 436443. https://doi.org/10.1097/QAI.0000000000000789.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weintraub, S., Wicklund, A. H., & Salmon, D. P. (2012). The neuropsychological profile of Alzheimer disease. Cold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Medicine, 2(4), a006171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woods, S. P., Scott, J. C., Conover, E., Marcotte, T. D., Heaton, R. K., & Grant, I. (2005). Test-retest reliability of component process variables within the hopkins verbal learning test-revised. Assessment, 12(1), 96100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
World Health Organization. (1997). Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI, version 2.1). Geneva: World Health Organization.Google Scholar
Wright, P. W., Pyakurel, A., Vaida, F. F., Price, R. W., Lee, E., Peterson, J., Fuchs, D., Zetterberg, H., Robertson, K. R., Walter, R., Meyerhoff, D. J., Spudich, S. S., & Ances, B. M. (2016). Putamen volume and its clinical and neurological correlates in primary HIV infection. AIDS, 30(11), 17891794. https://doi.org/10.1097/QAD.0000000000001103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, H. Y., Beymer, M. R., & Suen, S. C. (2019). Chronic disease onset among people living with HIV and AIDS in a large private insurance claims dataset. Scientific Reports, 9(1), 18514. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-54969-3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yu, X., Raji, M. A., Giordano, T. P., Berenson, A. B., Baillargeon, J., & Kuo, Y.-F. (2022). Prevalence of Alzheimer’s disease and related dementias among older US medicare beneficiaries with and without HIV: A successive cross-sectional study. The Lancet Healthy Longevity, 3, S6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhang, Y., Kwon, D., Esmaeili-Firidouni, P., Pfefferbaum, A., Sullivan, E. V., Javitz, H., Valcour, V., & Pohl, K. M. (2016). Extracting patterns of morphometry distinguishing HIV associated neurodegeneration from mild cognitive impairment via group cardinality constrained classification. Human Brain Mapping, 37(12), 45234538. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23326.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Participant demographic and clinical characteristics (N = 92)

Figure 1

Table 2. Logistic and multivariable regression examining the relationship between the medial temporal lobe, prefrontal cortex, and basal ganglia and likelihood of recognition impairment

Figure 2

Figure 1. Significant relationships between delayed recall and prefrontal cortex in the entire sample. Note. Regression lines adjust for age (centered), AIDS status (reference: non-AIDS), APOE group (reference: APOE 4- group) and mean cortical thickness (centered).

Figure 3

Table 3. Multivariable linear regressions examining the relationship between the medial temporal lobe, prefrontal cortex, and basal ganglia and delayed recall

Figure 4

Figure 2. Recognition composite Z-scores and recall T-scores graphed across years since baseline. Note. Dashed line on the left panel depicts the -1 standard deviation cut-off. Participant visits below this line were impared on recogniton. Solid black line on the right panel depicts the average change in delayed recall over time (i.e., slope).

Figure 5

Table 4. Linear mixed-effects results examining whether baseline medial temporal lobe structures are associated with change in delayed recall