Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-hc48f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-24T02:41:33.141Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Which Psychosocial Factors Best Predict Cognitive Performance in Older Adults?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  31 March 2014

Laura B. Zahodne*
Affiliation:
Cognitive Neuroscience Division, Department of Neurology and Taub Institute for Research on Alzheimer’s Disease and The Aging Brain, Columbia University College of Physicians and Surgeons, New York, New York
Cindy J. Nowinski
Affiliation:
Department of Medical Social Sciences, Northwestern University Feinberg School of Medicine, Chicago, Illinois
Richard C. Gershon
Affiliation:
Department of Medical Social Sciences, Northwestern University Feinberg School of Medicine, Chicago, Illinois Department of Preventive Medicine, Northwestern University Feinberg School of Medicine, Chicago, Illinois
Jennifer J. Manly
Affiliation:
Cognitive Neuroscience Division, Department of Neurology and Taub Institute for Research on Alzheimer’s Disease and The Aging Brain, Columbia University College of Physicians and Surgeons, New York, New York
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Laura B. Zahodne, Sergievsky Center/Taub Institute, Columbia University College of Physicians and Surgeons, 630 West 168th Street, P & S Box 16, New York, NY 10032. E-mail: lbz2105@columbia.edu

Abstract

Negative affect (e.g., depression) is associated with accelerated age-related cognitive decline and heightened dementia risk. Fewer studies examine positive psychosocial factors (e.g., emotional support, self-efficacy) in cognitive aging. Preliminary reports suggest that these variables predict slower cognitive decline independent of negative affect. No reports have examined these factors in a single model to determine which best relate to cognition. Data from 482 individuals 55 and older came from the normative sample for the NIH Toolbox for the Assessment of Neurological and Behavioral Function. Negative and positive psychosocial factors, executive functioning, working memory, processing speed, and episodic memory were measured with the NIH Toolbox Emotion and Cognition modules. Confirmatory factor analysis and structural equation modeling characterized independent relations between psychosocial factors and cognition. Psychosocial variables loaded onto negative and positive factors. Independent of education, negative affect and health status, greater emotional support was associated with better task-switching and processing speed. Greater self-efficacy was associated with better working memory. Negative affect was not independently associated with any cognitive variables. Findings support the conceptual distinctness of negative and positive psychosocial factors in older adults. Emotional support and self-efficacy may be more closely tied to cognition than other psychosocial variables. (JINS, 2014, 20, 1–9)

Type
Research Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bandura, A. (1989). Regulation of cognitive processes through perceived self-efficacy. Developmental Psychology, 25, 729735. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.25.5.729CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beaumont, J.L., Havlik, R., Cook, K.F., Hays, R.D., Wallner-Allen, K., … Gershon, R. (2013). Norming plans for the NIH Toolbox. Neurology, 80, S87S92. doi:10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182872e70Google Scholar
Bennett, D.A., Schneider, J.A., Tang, Y., Arnold, S.E., & Wilson, R.S. (2006). The effect of social networks on the relation between Alzheimer’s disease pathology and level of cognitive function in old people: A longitudinal cohort study. Lancet Neurology, 5, 406412. doi:10.1016/S1474-4422(06)70417-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society. Series B, Statistical Methodology, 57, 289300. doi:10.1214/aos/1013699998Google Scholar
Bunge, S.A., Kahn, I., Wallis, J.D., Miller, E.K., & Wagner, A.D. (2003). Neural circuit subserving the retrieval and maintenance of abstract rules. Journal of Neurophysiology, 90, 34193428. doi:10.1152/jn.00910.2002Google Scholar
Carretti, B., Borella, E., Zavagnin, M., & De Beni, R. (2011). Impact of metacognition and motivation on the efficacy of strategic memory training in older adults: Analysis of specific, transfer, and maintenance effects. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 52, 192197. doi:10.1016/j.archger.2010.11.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Charles, S.T., Reynolds, C.A., & Gatz, M. (2001). Age-related differences and change in positive and negative affect over 23 years. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 136151. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.80.1.136Google Scholar
Collins, A.L., Glei, D.A., & Goldman, N. (2009). The role of life satisfaction and depressive symptoms in all-cause mortality. Psychology and Aging, 24, 696702. doi:10.1037/a0016777Google Scholar
Costa, P.T. Jr., Zonderman, A.B., McCrae, R.R., Cornoni-Huntley, J., Locke, B.Z., & Barbano, H.E. (1987). Longitudinal analyses of psychological well-being in a national sample: Stability of mean levels. Journal of Gerontology, 42, 5055.Google Scholar
Diener, E. (2000). Subjective well-being. The science of happiness and a proposal for a national index. American Psychologist, 55, 3443. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.34Google Scholar
Dolcos, S., MacDonald, S.W.S., Braslavsky, A., Camicioli, R., & Dixon, R.A. (2012). Mild cognitive impairment is associated with selected functional markers: Integrating concurrent, longitudinal, and stability effects. Neuropsychology, 26, 209223. doi:10.1037/a0026760Google Scholar
Gershon, R.C., Wagster, M.V., Hendrie, H.C., Fox, N.A., Cook, K.F., & Nowinski, C.J. (2013). NIH Toolbox for assessment of neurological and behavioral function. Neurology, 80, S2S6. doi:10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182872e5fGoogle Scholar
Gerstorf, D., Lövdén, M., Rӧcke, C., Smith, J., & Lindenberger, U. (2007). Well-being affects changes in perceptual speed in advanced old age: Longitudinal evidence for a dynamic link. Developmental Psychology, 43, 705718. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.43.3.705CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glymour, M.M., Weuve, J., Fay, M.E., Glass, T., & Berkman, L.F. (2008). Social ties and cognitive recovery after stroke: Does social integration promote cognitive resilience? Neuroepidemiology, 31, 1020. doi:10.1159/000136646CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grewal, R., Cote, J.A., & Baumgartner, H. (2004). Multicollinearity and measurement error in structural equation models: Implications for theory testing. Marketing Science, 23, 519529. doi:10.1287/mksc.1040.0070Google Scholar
Hogan, M.J. (2003). Divided attention in older but not younger adults is impaired by anxiety. Experimental Aging Research, 29, 111136.Google Scholar
Lang, F.R., & Heckhausen, J. (2001). Perceived control over development and subjective well-being: Differential benefits across adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 509523. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.3.509CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lӧvdén, M., Ghisletta, P., & Lindenberger, U. (2005). Social participation attenuates decline in perceptual speed in old and very old age. Psychology and Aging, 20, 423434. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.20.3.423CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDermott, L.M., & Ebmeier, K.P. (2009). A meta-analysis of depression severity and cognitive function. Journal of Affective Disorders, 119, 18. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2009.04.022Google Scholar
Onoda, K., Okamoto, Y., Nakashima, K., Nittono, H., Ura, M., & Yamawaki, S. (2009). Decreased central anterior cingulate cortex activity is associated with reduced social pain during emotional support. Social Neuroscience, 4, 443454. doi:10.1080/17470910902955884CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ownby, R.L., Crocco, E., Acevedo, A., John, V., & Loewenstein, D. (2006). Depression and risk for Alzheimer disease: Systematic review, meta-analysis, and metaregression analysis. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63, 530538. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.63.5.530Google Scholar
Petkus, A.J., Gum, A.M., & Wetherell, J.L. (2013). Anxiety and cognitive impairment in homebound older adults. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 28, 989990.Google Scholar
Rock, P.L., Roiser, J.P., Riedel, W.J., & Blackwell, A.D. (2013). Cognitive impairment in depression: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine ([Epub ahead of print].Google Scholar
Rossi, A.S., & Rossi, P.H. (1990). Of human bonding: Parent-child relations across the life course. New York: Aldine de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Royall, D.R., Palmer, R., Chiodo, L.K., & Polk, M.J. (2012). Depressive symptoms predict longitudinal change in executive control but not memory. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 27, 8996. doi:10.1002/gps.2697CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rushworth, M.F., Hadland, K.A., Paus, T., & Sipila, P.K. (2002). Role of the human medial frontal cortex in task switching: A combined fMRI and TMS study. Journal of Neurophysiology, 87, 25772592. doi:10.1152/jn.00812.2001Google Scholar
Salsman, J.M., Butt, Z., Pilkonis, P.A., Cyranowski, J.M., Zill, N., … Cella, D. (2013). Emotion assessment using the NIH Toolbox. Neurology, 80, S76S86. doi:10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182872e11CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salthouse, T.A. (2010). Selective review of cognitive aging. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 16, 754760. doi:10.1017/S1355617710000706Google Scholar
Seeman, T., McAvay, G., Merrill, S., Albert, M., & Rodin, J. (1996). Self-efficacy beliefs and change in cognitive performance: MacArthur studies of successful aging. Psychology and Aging, 11, 538551. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.11.3.538Google Scholar
Seeman, T.E., Berkman, L.F., Charpentier, P.A., Blazer, D.G., Albert, M.S., & Tinetti, M.E. (1995). Behavioral and psychological predictors of physical performance: MacArthur studies of successful aging. Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 50, 177183. doi:10.1093/gerona/50A.4.M177Google Scholar
Seeman, T.E., Lusignolo, T.M., Albert, M., & Berkman, L. (2001). Social relationships, social support, and patterns of cognitive aging in healthy, high-functioning older adults: MacArthur Studies of Successful Aging. Health psychology, 20, 243255. doi:10.1037/0278-6133.20.4.243Google Scholar
Seignourel, P.J., Kunik, M.E., Snow, L., Wilson, N., & Stanley, M. (2008). Anxiety in dementia: A critical review. Clinical Psychology Review, 28, 10711082Google Scholar
Shi, Y., Zhou, X., Muller, H.J., & Schubert, T. (2011). The neural implementation of task rule activation in the task-cuing paradigm: An event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage, 51, 12531264. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.097Google Scholar
Sinoff, G., & Werner, P. (2003). Anxiety disorder and accompanying subjective memory loss in the elderly as a predictor of future cognitive decline. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 18, 951959.Google Scholar
Small, B.J., Dixon, R.A., McArdle, J.J., & Grimm, K.J. (2012). Do changes in lifestyle engagement moderate cognitive decline in normal aging? Evidence from the Victoria Longitudinal Study. Neuropsychology, 26, 144155.Google Scholar
Stern, Y. (2012). Cognitive reserve in Ageing and Alzheimer’s. Lancet Neurology, 11, 10061012. doi:10.1016/S1474-4422(12)70191-6Google Scholar
Watson, D., & Tellegen, A. (1985). Toward a consensual structure of mood. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 219235. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.98.2.219Google Scholar
Weintraub, S., Dikmen, S.S., Heaton, R.K., Tulsky, D.S., Zelazo, P.D., … Gershon, R.C. (2013). Cognition assessment using the NIH Toolbox. Neurology, 80, S54S64. doi:10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182872dedCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Windsor, T.D., & Anstey, K.J. (2008). A longitudinal investigation of perceived control and cognitive performance in young, midlife and older adults. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 15, 744763. doi:10.1080/13825580802348570Google Scholar
Ybarra, O., Burnstein, E., Winkielman, P., Keller, M.C., Manis, M., Chan, E., & Rodriguez, J. (2008). Mental exercising through simple socializing: social interaction promotes general cognitive functioning. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 248259. doi:10.1177/0146167207310454Google Scholar
Yochim, B.P., Mueller, A.E., & Segal, D.L. (2013). Late life anxiety is associated with decreased memory and executive functioning in community dwelling older adults. Journal of Anxiety Dixorders, 27, 567575.Google Scholar
Zakzanis, K.K., Leach, L., & Kaplan, E. (1998). On the nature and pattern of neurocognitive function in major depressive disorder. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 11, 111119.Google Scholar