Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t8hqh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-24T08:47:58.817Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Using rodent data to elucidate dopaminergic mechanisms of ADHD: Implications for human personality

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  31 January 2024

Gail Tripp
Affiliation:
Human Developmental Neurobiology Unit, Okinawa Institute of Science and Technology Graduate University, Okinawa, Japan
Jeff Wickens*
Affiliation:
Neurobiology Research Unit, Okinawa Institute of Science and Technology Graduate University, Okinawa, Japan
*
Corresponding author: Jeff Wickens; Email: wickens@oist.jp
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

An altered behavioral response to positive reinforcement has been proposed to be a core deficit in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). The spontaneously hypertensive rat (SHR), a congenic animal strain, displays a similarly altered response to reinforcement. The presence of this genetically determined phenotype in a rodent model allows experimental investigation of underlying neural mechanisms. Behaviorally, the SHR displays increased preference for immediate reinforcement, increased sensitivity to individual instances of reinforcement relative to integrated reinforcement history, and a steeper delay of reinforcement gradient compared to other rat strains. The SHR also shows less development of incentive to approach sensory stimuli, or cues, that predict reward after repeated cue-reward pairing. We consider the underlying neural mechanisms for these characteristics. It is well known that midbrain dopamine neurons are initially activated by unexpected reward and gradually transfer their responses to reward-predicting cues. This finding has inspired the dopamine transfer deficit (DTD) hypothesis, which predicts certain behavioral effects that would arise from a deficient transfer of dopamine responses from actual rewards to reward-predicting cues. We argue that the DTD predicts the altered responses to reinforcement seen in the SHR and individuals with ADHD. These altered responses to reinforcement in turn predict core symptoms of ADHD. We also suggest that variations in the degree of dopamine transfer may underlie variations in personality dimensions related to altered reinforcement sensitivity. In doing so, we highlight the value of rodent models to the study of human personality.

Type
Review Paper
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a prevalent disorder defined by persistent and developmentally inappropriate levels of inattention, and/or hyperactivity and impulsivity. Although generally considered a neurodevelopmental disorder with a neurobiological and genetic basis, the pathophysiology of ADHD is unknown. According to the fifth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM-V) diagnosis is based on the presence of a number of reported behavioral symptoms, in some combination of inattention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity, together with functional impairment (Stein, Lund, & Nesse, Reference Stein, Lund and Nesse2013). The International Classification of Diseases-11 (ICD-11) uses the same symptoms to define ADHD with the addition of an impulsive trait description that in DSM-5 is represented by specific symptoms (Gomez, Chen, & Houghton, Reference Gomez, Chen and Houghton2023).

Like many psychiatric disorders, ADHD is thought to be the product of multiple interacting causes acting on multiple brain mechanisms. The DSM-V and ICD-11 criteria provide a reliable communication system providing reference categories for biomedical and psychological research. However, these classification systems are not meant to imply that disorders like ADHD are discrete with specific causes and biomarkers and distinct boundaries (Stein et al., Reference Stein, Lund and Nesse2013). All the symptoms of ADHD occur in typically developing individuals to some extent. For example, from the DSM-5 one can select symptoms like: “Is often forgetful in daily activities”; “Often loses things necessary for tasks and activities”; “Often fails to give close attention to details”; “Often talks excessively”; and “Often interrupts or intrudes on others (e.g., butts into conversations or games).” In isolation, such behaviors are not uncommon in the wider population. Empirically, ADHD-like symptoms are distributed throughout the population on a continuum (Arcos-Burgos & Acosta, Reference Arcos-Burgos and Acosta2007). The differences between individuals with ADHD and typically developing individuals are not qualitative but quantitative.

The continuum of symptoms raises the possibility that ADHD could be viewed as an exaggeration of normal personality traits. It may be more accurate to consider ADHD as a continuous phenotype rather than a categorical “with” or “without” ADHD dichotomy (Levy, Hay, McStephen, Wood, & Waldman, Reference Levy, Hay, McStephen, Wood and Waldman1997). A dimensional model (Hierarchical Taxonomy of Psychopathology, HiToP) has been proposed, which includes ADHD, and unlike the traditional classification systems typified by DSM-5 and ICD-11, uses dimensions instead of symptom clusters.

To date, however, ADHD has not been consistently located within the HiToP model (Mullins-Sweatt et al., Reference Mullins-Sweatt, Bornovalova, Carragher, Clark, Corona Espinosa, Jonas and Watts2022). For example, within HiToP, ADHD is included under the antisocial subfactor, which combines disinhibited externalizing and antagonistic externalizing spectra, along with antisocial personality disorder, oppositional defiant disorder, conduct disorder, and intermittent explosive disorder (Mullins-Sweatt et al., Reference Mullins-Sweatt, Bornovalova, Carragher, Clark, Corona Espinosa, Jonas and Watts2022). In contrast to the symptoms of antisocial personality disorder (such as acting recklessly, breaking laws without caring about consequences, and disregarding responsibilities), the ADHD symptoms listed in DSM-5 and ICD-11 definitions do not necessarily cause conflict with others in the same way (De Pauw & Mervielde, Reference De Pauw and Mervielde2011). Although individuals with ADHD may have difficulty following the rules, it is not for want of trying. In addition, ADHD has been found to be a relatively weak indicator of externalizing factors (Carragher et al., Reference Carragher, Krueger, Eaton, Markon, Keyes, Blanco and Hasin2014) and to also involve internalizing spectra (Bozhilova, Michelini, Kuntsi, & Asherson, Reference Bozhilova, Michelini, Kuntsi and Asherson2018; Nigg, Karalunas, Feczko, & Fair, Reference Nigg, Karalunas, Feczko and Fair2020). Thus, ADHD may not be well placed under the antisocial subfactor in HiToP. Further work is needed to develop an appropriate dimensional concept for ADHD.

In an ideal framework for conceptualizing ADHD, dimensions would be based on neurobiological causes. This is beyond the present state of the art in neurobiological research. However, there are many results of neurobiological research that are highly relevant to understanding the pathophysiology of ADHD and it is useful to include them in theoretical approaches and emerging dimensional frameworks. For example, we and others have suggested that many of the symptoms of ADHD arise from an altered sensitivity to reinforcement (Catania, Reference Catania2005; Iaboni, Douglas, & Baker, Reference Iaboni, Douglas and Baker1995; Sagvolden, Aase, Zeiner, & Berger, Reference Sagvolden, Aase, Zeiner and Berger1998; Tripp & Wickens, Reference Tripp and Wickens2008, Reference Tripp and Wickens2009; Wickens & Tripp, Reference Wickens and Tripp1998; Williams & Dayan, Reference Williams and Dayan2005).

Central to our thesis is the role of dopamine in positive reinforcement, and by implication, in altered reinforcement sensitivity in ADHD and related personality traits. Although widely regarded as a “reward molecule” in the popular literature, the behavioral and physiological effects of dopamine are complex and deeply involved in multiple fundamental aspects of brain function (Wise, Reference Wise2004). Here we focus on the implications of two aspects of dopamine function: the firing patterns of dopamine neurons in response to reward and reward-predicting cues (Schultz, Reference Schultz2002), and the timing-sensitive effects of dopamine on the strength of synaptic connections at the cellular level (Reynolds, Hyland, & Wickens, Reference Reynolds, Hyland and Wickens2001; Shindou, Shindou, Watanabe, & Wickens, Reference Shindou, Shindou, Watanabe and Wickens2019). While these findings from animal studies may seem remote from human psychopathology and personality theories, behavioral characteristics are profoundly affected by subtle alterations in these mechanisms. We review dopamine dynamics in positive reinforcement in section 1. We then describe the behavioral characteristics of a rat with altered sensitivity to reinforcement in section 2. Finally, we consider the implications of altered behavioral characteristics for healthy humans and individuals with ADHD in section 3.

1. Dopamine dynamics and timing in positive reinforcement

1.1 Transfer of the dopamine response from reward to reward-predicting cues

Extensive human and animal research over the past few decades has revealed a central role for dopamine in positive reinforcement. In non-human primates and rodents, experiments have shown that dopamine neurons in the midbrain are activated by unexpected primary rewards, such as sips of juice or pieces of apple delivered at random times (Ljungberg, Apicella, & Schultz, Reference Ljungberg, Apicella and Schultz1992; Schultz, Apicella, & Ljungberg, Reference Schultz, Apicella and Ljungberg1993; Schultz, Dayan, & Montague, Reference Schultz, Dayan and Montague1997). However, when these primary rewards are repeatedly preceded by a sensory stimulus (such as a sound tone or light), the response of the dopamine neurons to the reward decreases, and the response to the associated sensory stimuli increases (Day, Roitman, Wightman, & Carelli, Reference Day, Roitman, Wightman and Carelli2007; Ljungberg et al., Reference Ljungberg, Apicella and Schultz1992; Pan, Schmidt, Wickens, & Hyland, Reference Pan, Schmidt, Wickens and Hyland2005; Schultz et al., Reference Schultz, Apicella and Ljungberg1993, Reference Schultz, Dayan and Montague1997). After repeated pairing with reward, the previously neutral sensory stimuli become “reward-predicting cues” and cause dopamine release, while over the same period, dopamine release in response to the actual reward decreases. We refer to this as “transfer” of the dopamine response from reward to reward-predicting cues (see Fig. 1a).

Figure 1. Transfer of dopamine response from actual reward to cues predicts behavioral characteristics. Traces show idealized dopamine signal in normal (a) and hypothesized dopamine transfer deficit (b). In both cases, unexpected primary reward causes a dopamine response. Normally, after repeated pairing of cue and reward, the dopamine response transfers to the cue. When there is a dopamine transfer deficit the cue response fails to develop as strongly as normal, and the response to the actual reward persists. Compared to normal rat strains, the SHR shows a dopamine transfer deficit. This is associated with characteristic of immediate over-delayed reward. In humans, a dopamine transfer deficit may give rise to symptoms of ADHD. These can be viewed as extremes of normal variations in individual personality traits.

1.2 Actions of dopamine at the behavioral level

Without knowing what its effects are, knowledge about the release of dopamine and its transfer from rewards to cues has little explanatory power. Several distinguishable, but not mutually exclusive, hypotheses have been proposed for the actions of dopamine, namely: positive reinforcement, reward, hedonia, incentive motivation, and conditioned reinforcement (Wise, Reference Wise2004).

The most established hypothesis is that dopamine mediates positive reinforcement, which by definition increases the frequency of responses that it follows. Depleting dopamine or blocking its action reduces the reinforcing effects of food, water, or direct stimulation of dopamine cells (Beninger & Freedman, Reference Beninger and Freedman1982; Lynch & Wise, Reference Lynch and Wise1985; Wise, Reference Wise2006). Under such conditions, learning new responses is prevented, previously trained responses decline in frequency, and acquisition of Pavlovian stimulus-reward associations is reduced (Darvas, Wunsch, Gibbs, & Palmiter, Reference Darvas, Wunsch, Gibbs and Palmiter2014). Conversely, stimulating the dopamine-rich areas of the brain (ventral tegmental area and substantia nigra pars compacta of the midbrain and their projections to the nucleus accumbens and dorsal striatum) is sufficient to cause positive reinforcement (Corbett & Wise, Reference Corbett and Wise1980; Kim et al., Reference Kim, Baratta, Yang, Lee, Boyden and Fiorillo2012; Wise & Bozarth, Reference Wise and Bozarth1984). Thus the dopamine neurons of the midbrain are both necessary and sufficient for positive reinforcement of responses.

The positive reinforcement hypothesis of dopamine action suggests that dopamine acts on the memory traces of past responses to increase their frequency in the future. The reward hypothesis, on the other hand, refers to proactive effects of dopamine on future actions. “Reward” in this context refers to stimuli that evoke approach to the stimulus. Dopamine release energizes future responses and approaches to behavior (Gallistel, Stellar, & Bubis, Reference Gallistel, Stellar and Bubis1974). Dopamine is not necessary for producing these responses but increases their vigor or reduces their latency (Wise, Reference Wise2004).

A third effect of dopamine is related to incentive motivation. This effect refers to the way sensory stimuli can acquire incentive properties or value by prior association with reward (Berridge, Reference Berridge2000). Sensory stimuli that have developed incentive properties can powerfully control behavior (Bindra, Reference Bindra1974). Such stimuli elicit orientation and approach toward them. They can also produce positive reinforcement in the absence of actual reward, in which case they are considered conditioned reinforcers. The transfer of dopamine release from the time of actual reward to the time of the reward-predicting cue may be what gives these sensory stimuli incentive value.

Another theory of dopamine function concerns its role in pleasure. The dopamine hedonia theory is less well-supported. Although rewards are a source of pleasure, elevations of brain dopamine are not strongly correlated with subjective pleasure (Berridge, Reference Berridge2000, Reference Berridge2007). On the other hand, decreased dopamine release in the striatum in response to rewards is associated with reduced ability to experience pleasure (anhedonia) in depression (Belujon & Grace, Reference Belujon and Grace2017; Phillips et al., Reference Phillips, Walsh, Zurcher, Lalush, Kinard, Tseng and Dichter2023) and there is evidence that dopamine receptors are important in the action of antidepressants (Willner, Hale, & Argyropoulos, Reference Willner, Hale and Argyropoulos2005).

These theories concerning the action of dopamine are at the level of behavior and subjective experience. However, it is also important to consider the underlying neural mechanisms by which dopamine produces these effects and the requirements for activation of those mechanisms. In broad anatomical terms, the dopamine neurons of the midbrain project to cortical and subcortical areas, most densely to the dorsal striatum, and to a lesser extent, the ventral striatum of the basal ganglia (Swanson, Reference Swanson1982). At the cellular level, dopamine neurons make synaptic contacts on the same neurons and at the same location as the excitatory inputs to those regions (Smith, Bennett, Bolam, Parent, & Sadikot, Reference Smith, Bennett, Bolam, Parent and Sadikot1994) indicating that dopamine acts as a modulator of other synaptic inputs.

1.3 Actions of dopamine at the cellular level

Many pieces of evidence indicate that the positive reinforcement effects of dopamine are mediated by strengthening of synaptic connections between the cerebral cortex and the striatum. Dopamine facilitates long-term potentiation of these corticostriatal synaptic connections under certain conditions (Calabresi, Picconi, Tozzi, & Di Filippo, Reference Calabresi, Picconi, Tozzi and Di Filippo2007; Centonze, Picconi, Gubellini, Bernardi, & Calabresi, Reference Centonze, Picconi, Gubellini, Bernardi and Calabresi2001; Pawlak & Kerr, Reference Pawlak and Kerr2008; Reynolds et al., Reference Reynolds, Hyland and Wickens2001; Shen, Flajolet, Greengard, & Surmeier, Reference Shen, Flajolet, Greengard and Surmeier2008; Shindou et al., Reference Shindou, Shindou, Watanabe and Wickens2019; Wickens, Reference Wickens2008; Wickens, Begg, & Arbuthnott, Reference Wickens, Begg and Arbuthnott1996; Yagishita et al., Reference Yagishita, Hayashi-Takagi, Ellis-Davies, Urakubo, Ishii and Kasai2014). At the cellular level, a three-term contingency has been demonstrated, which requires activation of excitatory synaptic input from the cortex, firing of the postsynaptic striatal neuron, and release of dopamine. This conjunction of activity related to sensory inputs, action, and dopamine results in long-lasting strengthening of corticostriatal synaptic connections (Wickens et al., Reference Wickens, Begg and Arbuthnott1996). Behaviorally reinforcing electrical stimulation of the brain also causes dopamine-dependent strengthening of corticostriatal synaptic connections (Reynolds et al., Reference Reynolds, Hyland and Wickens2001).

1.4 Relating the synaptic eligibility trace and the behavioral delay of reinforcement gradient

Electrophysiological experiments have shown that at the cellular level, there is a narrow time window of a few seconds during which dopamine is effective at strengthening synaptic connections (Cassenaer & Laurent, Reference Cassenaer and Laurent2012; He et al., Reference He, Huertas, Hong, Tie, Hell, Shouval and Kirkwood2015; Shindou et al., Reference Shindou, Shindou, Watanabe and Wickens2019; Yagishita et al., Reference Yagishita, Hayashi-Takagi, Ellis-Davies, Urakubo, Ishii and Kasai2014). If dopamine is released a few seconds too early, or too late, it is ineffective in promoting strengthening of neural connections. We refer to this time window as a synaptic eligibility trace.

At the behavioral level, there is also a restricted time window during which positive reinforcement can affect response learning (Renner, Reference Renner1964; Tarpy & Sawabini, Reference Tarpy and Sawabini1974). Reinforcement is more effective at shorter delays than at longer delays, and a delay reduces the effect of positive reinforcement on learning (Critchfield & Lattal, Reference Critchfield and Lattal1993; Dickinson, Watt, & Griffiths, Reference Dickinson, Watt and Griffiths1992; Lattal & Gleeson, Reference Lattal and Gleeson1990). This relation between delay and effectiveness of reinforcement is known as the delay of reinforcement gradient (Tarpy & Sawabini, Reference Tarpy and Sawabini1974). With natural rewards and no additional cues, the delay of reinforcement gradient has a half-life on the order of tens of seconds (Perin, Reference Perin1943).

In contrast to the delay of reinforcement gradient, the synaptic eligibility trace has a much shorter time course of a few seconds (Shindou et al., Reference Shindou, Shindou, Watanabe and Wickens2019). However, under conditions of delayed reinforcement, a sensory cue that reliably precedes reward can facilitate learning by anticipatory release of dopamine and thus reduce the effects of the delay of actual reward (Renner, Reference Renner1964; Tarpy & Sawabini, Reference Tarpy and Sawabini1974). Even when there is not an obvious sensory cue, the organism’s response, or the delay itself, can act as a predictive cue (Ferster, Reference Ferster1953; Garrud, Goodall, & Mackintosh, Reference Garrud, Goodall and Mackintosh1981; Winstanley, Theobald, Cardinal, & Robbins, Reference Winstanley, Theobald, Cardinal and Robbins2004). The predictive cue “bridges” the delay (Cardinal, Winstanley, Robbins, & Everitt, Reference Cardinal, Winstanley, Robbins and Everitt2004; Grice, Reference Grice1948) and, thus, there is timely release of dopamine at the cellular level. If this bridging mechanism is disabled by removing all predictive cue signals, for example, by using direct stimulation of brain dopamine neurons as the reinforcer, the effects of reinforcement are reduced by delays of as little as one second (Black, Belluzzi, & Stein, Reference Black, Belluzzi and Stein1985). Thus, it seems that the critical timing requirement for dopamine-dependent plasticity at the cellular level can be met, even when there is a more prolonged delay of reinforcement at the behavioral level, by the transfer of dopamine release from the reward to the reward-predicting cues.

1.5 The dopamine transfer deficit hypothesis

We have suggested above that transfer of the dopamine signal from reward to reward-predicting cue ensures that dopamine release occurs at the right time to strengthen synaptic connections at the cellular level, even when the behavioral reinforcer is delayed. However, the success of this depends on the ability to learn the cue-reward association and complete the transfer of the dopamine signal to the cue. We have previously considered the possible consequences of failure to learn the cue-reward association. We refer to this as the dopamine transfer deficit (DTD) hypothesis (Tripp & Wickens, Reference Tripp and Wickens2008, Reference Tripp and Wickens2009), as illustrated in Fig. 1b. Other authors have also proposed that reduced dopamine functioning causes altered processing of reward in individuals with ADHD (Levy, Reference Levy1991; Sagvolden, Johansen, Aase, & Russell, Reference Sagvolden, Johansen, Aase and Russell2005) in ADHD. However, DTD is unique in focusing specifically on the timing of the phasic dopamine response.

In developing the DTD hypothesis, we tried to predict the behavioral characteristics that would result from deficient transfer of the dopamine response from rewards to cues. In a condition where dopamine release did not develop in response to a reward-predicting cue, the dopamine signal at the cellular level would be delayed until the actual reward occurred. Under such conditions, dopamine-dependent strengthening of connections would be reduced or not occur at all. Several predictions follow:

1.6 Delay of reinforcement gradient

In the absence of dopamine release by reward-predicting cues, even very short delays of a few seconds would reduce the reinforcing effect of the delayed reinforcer. Thus, deficient transfer of the dopamine response from rewards to cues is predicted to cause a steeper delay of reinforcement gradient.

1.7 Partial reinforcement effects

Under conditions of partial reinforcement – a schedule in which not every response is reinforced – dopamine release by cues that are present on every trial would normally provide continuous reinforcement at the cellular level. In the absence of such dopamine release, learning under partial reinforcement will be slower. Furthermore, it is well established that although acquisition of learning is slower under partial reinforcement, the learning that is acquired is more resistant to extinction. This is called the partial reinforcement extinction effect (Myers, Reference Myers1960). The DTD hypothesis predicts slower learning under partial reinforcement and faster extinction (less behavioral persistence) of learned behavior because of reduced dopamine response to reward-predicting cues when reinforcement is stopped (Tripp & Wickens, Reference Tripp and Wickens2008).

1.8 Integration of reinforcement history

Behavior is not only controlled by individual instances of reinforcement but also by an internal representation of the integrated reinforcement history (Killeen & Sitomer, Reference Killeen and Sitomer2003; Okouchi & Lattal, Reference Okouchi and Lattal2006), so that responses that typically result in reinforcement are selected over those that most recently resulted in reinforcement (Tripp & Alsop, Reference Tripp and Alsop1999). Failure of dopamine transfer would cause increased sensitivity to individual instances of reinforcement.

In light of these predictions in the following sections, we review behavioral studies on the spontaneously hypertensive rat (SHR) model for ADHD, and humans with ADHD, before considering the implications of variations in dopamine transfer for symptoms and personality dimensions of individuals with ADHD and typically developing individuals.

2. SHR behavior

Rodents with genetically determined behavioral characteristics provide opportunities for experimental study of brain mechanisms underlying those behavioral characteristics. Moreover, although they are also complex organisms, experimental animals can be bred selectively to express specific behavioral traits. Inbred strains provide homogeneity of genetic makeup, and cross-breeding can be used to determine if characteristics are genetically linked. Animal models also provide otherwise unattainable invasive and repeated measurements important for identifying underlying neural mechanisms. These have been particularly successful in the neurobiological investigation of mechanisms for positive reinforcement.

Here we focus on the SHR, a transgenic strain with, among other characteristics, an altered sensitivity to delay of reinforcement (Johansen, Killeen, & Sagvolden, Reference Johansen, Killeen and Sagvolden2007; Orduna, Reference Orduna2015; Sutherland et al., Reference Sutherland, Alsop, McNaughton, Hyland, Tripp and Wickens2009; Wickens, Hyland, & Tripp, Reference Wickens, Hyland and Tripp2011). The SHR was originally developed as a genetic animal model for hypertension (Okamoto & Aoki, Reference Okamoto and Aoki1963). During selective breeding for hypertension, by chance, some distinct behavioral characteristics became fixed in the SHR genome (Hendley, Atwater, Myers, & Whitehorn, Reference Hendley, Atwater, Myers and Whitehorn1983; McCarty & Kopin, Reference McCarty and Kopin1979; Sagvolden, Hendley, & Knardahl, Reference Sagvolden, Hendley and Knardahl1992; Wultz & Sagvolden, Reference Wultz and Sagvolden1992). These behavioral characteristics include altered responses to reinforcement (Hill, Herbst, & Sanabria, Reference Hill, Herbst and Sanabria2012; Johansen et al., Reference Johansen, Killeen and Sagvolden2007; Sagvolden, Reference Sagvolden2000; Sagvolden, Metzger et al., Reference Sagvolden, Metzger, Schiorbeck, Rugland, Spinnangr and Sagvolden1992), impulsivity (Adriani, Caprioli, Granstrem, Carli, & Laviola, Reference Adriani, Caprioli, Granstrem, Carli and Laviola2003; Aparicio, Hennigan, Mulligan, & Alonso-Alvarez, Reference Aparicio, Hennigan, Mulligan and Alonso-Alvarez2019; Bizot et al., Reference Bizot, Chenault, Houze, Herpin, David, Pothion and Trovero2007; Fox, Hand, & Reilly, Reference Fox, Hand and Reilly2008; Gonzalez-Barriga & Orduna, Reference Gonzalez-Barriga and Orduna2022; Sagvolden, Russell, Aase, Johansen, & Farshbaf, Reference Sagvolden, Russell, Aase, Johansen and Farshbaf2005; Sanabria & Killeen, Reference Sanabria and Killeen2008), and inattention (Aparicio et al., Reference Aparicio, Hennigan, Mulligan and Alonso-Alvarez2019; Sagvolden, Reference Sagvolden2000; Sagvolden, Metzger et al., Reference Sagvolden, Metzger, Schiorbeck, Rugland, Spinnangr and Sagvolden1992; Sagvolden, Pettersen, & Larsen, Reference Sagvolden, Pettersen and Larsen1993).

Consistent with DTD hypothesis, the SHR displays a higher sensitivity to delay of reinforcement than comparison strains (Johansen et al., Reference Johansen, Killeen and Sagvolden2007; Johansen, Sagvolden, & Kvande, Reference Johansen, Sagvolden and Kvande2005; Sagvolden, Reference Sagvolden2000; Sagvolden, Metzger et al., Reference Sagvolden, Metzger, Schiorbeck, Rugland, Spinnangr and Sagvolden1992) and a stronger preference for immediate over-delayed reward (Fox et al., Reference Fox, Hand and Reilly2008; Hand, Fox, & Reilly, Reference Hand, Fox and Reilly2006; Sutherland et al., Reference Sutherland, Alsop, McNaughton, Hyland, Tripp and Wickens2009). This higher sensitivity to delay of reward in the SHR, relative to comparison strains, may be due to underlying differences in dopamine transfer to reward-predicting cues.

In other rat strains, there is also evidence that individual differences in responses to rewards and reward-predicting cues are associated with different patterns of behavior. For example, animals display differences in their tendency to approach and interact with reward-predicting cues. Approaching and interacting with the reward-predicting cue is called sign-tracking (Davey & Cleland, Reference Davey and Cleland1982), while approaching the location of the reward itself when the cue appears is called goal-tracking (Boakes, Reference Boakes, David and Hurwitz1977).

Dopamine responses to reward-predicting cues and reward locations have been measured in Sprague-Dawley rats selectively bred for sign-tracking or goal-tracking behavior. These measures have shown that animals with a lower phasic dopamine response to reward-predicting cues and higher phasic dopamine response to reward delivery display more goal-tracking behavior, and conversely, animals with a higher striatal dopamine response to reward-predicting cues and lower dopamine response to reward delivery display more sign-tracking behavior (Flagel et al., Reference Flagel, Clark, Robinson, Mayo, Czuj, Willuhn and Akil2012). Since SHRs are more likely to perform like goal-trackers (Silic, Aggarwal, Liyanagama, Tripp, & Wickens, Reference Silic, Aggarwal, Liyanagama, Tripp and Wickens2023), their dopamine response to reward-predicting cues is of particular relevance.

Several differences in dopamine function have been reported in the SHR. They include lower basal dopamine levels (Fujita et al., Reference Fujita, Okutsu, Yamaguchi, Nakamura, Adachi, Saigusa and Koshikawa2003), decreased release of dopamine, and faster time course of dopamine clearance after release (Miller et al., Reference Miller, Pomerleau, Huettl, Russell, Gerhardt and Glaser2012). This faster clearance in the SHR may be due to elevated dopamine transporter expression (Roessner et al., Reference Roessner, Sagvolden, Dasbanerjee, Middleton, Faraone, Walaas and Bock2010; Watanabe et al., Reference Watanabe, Fujita, Ito, Okada, Kusuoka and Nishimura1997). We have recently reported differences in phasic dopamine release in response to reward and reward-predicting cues in Sprague-Dawley (SD) and SHR strains using fast-scan cyclic voltammetry during a simple classical conditioning paradigm (Li, Huang, Chen, Hyland, & Wickens, Reference Li, Huang, Chen, Hyland and WickensSubmitted). In these experiments, a previously neutral sensory cue was paired with rewarding electrical stimulation of dopamine cells. We found less phasic dopamine release in response to electrical stimulation of dopamine cells in the SHR than in the SD rats. Further, the SHR showed less transfer of the phasic dopamine response from reward to cues over successive trials. These findings indicate altered dopaminergic dynamics in the SHR compared to the SD, which might contribute to differences in their behavioral response to cues and rewards. However, further work is needed for a better understanding of phasic dopamine signaling in the SHR.

3. Translation from rodent model to human personality and ADHD

3.1 Steeper delay of reinforcement gradient in rodent models and ADHD

Evaluating an animal model for ADHD requires careful attention to the symptoms of the human disorder. One way to ensure a connection between non-human animal and human studies is to use the same task for both. In some cases, nearly identical tasks have been used in studies of SHRs and humans with ADHD. These include a rat task adapted for use in children, and conversely, a human task adapted for use in rats.

An example of a rat task adapted for children is a fixed-interval (FI) schedule that has been extensively used in the investigation of behavioral characteristics of the SHR (Sagvolden et al., Reference Sagvolden, Pettersen and Larsen1993). In that schedule, rewards are delivered for the first response emitted after a fixed period of time has elapsed, but not for responses emitted earlier. The FI is then restarted for a number of repetitions. In such schedules, there is usually a decrease in responses after the reward, followed by an increase in responses toward the end of the FI.

The characteristic pattern of FI responding has been interpreted as a reflection of the delay of reinforcement gradient. The basis for this interpretation is that a reinforcer not only increases the probability of the response that produced it but to a lesser extent also increases the probability of earlier responses (Catania, Reference Catania1971; Johansen et al., Reference Johansen, Killeen, Russell, Tripp, Wickens, Tannock and Sagvolden2009; Killeen, Reference Killeen2011). This is because, in the FI schedule, the earlier responses in the interval are separated from the reinforcer delivery by a longer delay than the responses occurring later. Assuming that the effects of reinforcement are distributed according to a delay of reinforcement gradient, the earlier responses will be strengthened less than the later responses. Thus, after learning, the earlier responses occur at a lower rate than the later responses (Catania, Sagvolden, & Keller, Reference Catania, Sagvolden and Keller1988; Wearden & Lejeune, Reference Wearden and Lejeune2006). An example is shown in Fig. 2b. The increase in responding over the FI is, therefore, treated as an indirect measure of the delay of reinforcement gradient.

Figure 2. Delay of reinforcement gradient interpretation of fixed-interval responding. (a) Classic representation of the control exerted by delayed reinforcers. Habit strength is plotted as a function of time of reinforcement, based on the formula ${1 \over L} = {0.22^{ - 0.215t}} - 0.0188t + 0.32 $ fitted to experimentally measured latency or responses, L, for reinforcement at different delay times, t, (Perin, Reference Perin1943). (b) Example of FI responding showing the increase in response rate as the time of reinforcer delivery approaches for four different strains of rat. GH, Genetically Hypertensive, SHR, Spontaneously Hypertensive, WKY, Wistar Kiyoto, WI, Wistar. Redrawn from Wickens et al. (Reference Wickens, Macfarlane, Booker and McNaughton2004).

On the FI schedule, SHRs produce more responses than comparison strains, with responses increasing more steeply over the FI (Sagvolden, Hendley et al., Reference Sagvolden, Hendley and Knardahl1992; Sagvolden et al., Reference Sagvolden, Pettersen and Larsen1993). This finding is robust and has been replicated in other laboratories (Orduna, Reference Orduna2015; Wickens, Macfarlane, Booker, & McNaughton, Reference Wickens, Macfarlane, Booker and McNaughton2004). Thus, their FI behavior indicates that the SHRs have a greater sensitivity to delay of reinforcement than comparison strains.

On a similar FI schedule, children with ADHD made significantly more responses during the FI than typically developing children, with increased responses toward the end of the FI (Sagvolden et al., Reference Sagvolden, Aase, Zeiner and Berger1998). As in the SHR, this was interpreted as indication of a steeper-than-normal delay of reinforcement gradient in children with ADHD, operating over a similar timescale of seconds (Sagvolden et al., Reference Sagvolden, Aase, Zeiner and Berger1998).

3.2 Increased preference for immediate reinforcement in rodent models and ADHD

The converse example of a human task that was subsequently adapted for rats is a signal-detection task developed for studying sensitivity to delay of reinforcement in children with ADHD (Tripp & Alsop, Reference Tripp and Alsop2001). In this task, children had to press one of two buttons to identify one of two similar stimuli. Correct identification of one stimulus was immediately reinforced, followed by a 3.5 s delay before the start of the next trial. The other stimulus was reinforced after a 3.5 s delay. Both typically developing children and children with ADHD showed a bias toward immediate reinforcement, but the bias in the ADHD group was greater than in controls. These results showed that in this task, children with ADHD are not more sensitive to delay itself, as suggested by the delay aversion hypothesis (Sonuga-Barke, Taylor, Sembi, & Smith, Reference Sonuga-Barke, Taylor, Sembi and Smith1992), but specifically have higher sensitivity to delay of reinforcement.

In the animal version of the task, rats were rewarded for lever pressing one of two available levers on each trial (Sutherland et al., Reference Sutherland, Alsop, McNaughton, Hyland, Tripp and Wickens2009). One lever was associated with immediate delivery of a food pellet, and the other lever with delivery of a food pellet after a 10-s delay. Compared to their control strain, SHRs showed a greater bias toward immediate reinforcement. These results show that, like children with ADHD, SHRs have higher sensitivity to delay of reinforcement than comparison strains.

Surprisingly, genetically hypertensive (GH) rats showed similarly greater bias toward immediate reinforcement than the comparison strain. The GH rat was derived independently of the SHR by a similar process of selective breeding for high blood pressure (Simpson et al., Reference Simpson, Phelan, Clark, Jones, Gresson, Lee and Bird1973; Smirk & Hall, Reference Smirk and Hall1958). Like the SHR (Hendley et al., Reference Hendley, Atwater, Myers and Whitehorn1983), the genetically determined behavioral characteristics of the GH rat are dissociated from high blood pressure (Wickens et al., Reference Wickens, Macfarlane, Booker and McNaughton2004). Nonetheless, the GH rat shows FI responding (Wickens et al., Reference Wickens, Macfarlane, Booker and McNaughton2004) and sensitivity to delay of reinforcement (Sutherland et al., Reference Sutherland, Alsop, McNaughton, Hyland, Tripp and Wickens2009) similar to the SHR. In addition, the GH rat also showed a tendency to be more influenced by individual instances of reinforcement (Sutherland et al., Reference Sutherland, Alsop, McNaughton, Hyland, Tripp and Wickens2009).

3.3 Steeper delay discounting in rodent models and ADHD

Individuals with ADHD tend to choose small immediate rewards over larger delayed rewards in both choice delay and temporal discounting paradigms (Antrop et al., Reference Antrop, Stock, Verte, Wiersema, Baeyens and Roeyers2006; Firestone & Douglas, Reference Firestone and Douglas1975; Hoerger & Mace, Reference Hoerger and Mace2006; Jackson & MacKillop, Reference Jackson and MacKillop2016; Marx, Hacker, Yu, Cortese, & Sonuga-Barke, Reference Marx, Hacker, Yu, Cortese and Sonuga-Barke2021; Patros et al., Reference Patros, Alderson, Kasper, Tarle, Lea and Hudec2016; Scheres et al., Reference Scheres, Dijkstra, Ainslie, Balkan, Reynolds, Sonuga-Barke and Castellanos2006; Sonuga-Barke et al., Reference Sonuga-Barke, Taylor, Sembi and Smith1992). Although this is also true of typically developing individuals, the tendency is greater in individuals with ADHD. Most human studies have used temporal discounting tasks, which are also known as intertemporal choice or delay discounting tasks (Marx et al., Reference Marx, Hacker, Yu, Cortese and Sonuga-Barke2021; Sonuga-Barke, Sergeant, Nigg, & Willcutt, Reference Sonuga-Barke, Sergeant, Nigg and Willcutt2008; Sonuga-Barke et al., Reference Sonuga-Barke, Taylor, Sembi and Smith1992). These tasks measure the tendency to place less value on rewards that are delayed, often modeled as a discount curve indicating the subjective value of a reward as a function of delay to its receipt.

Although often lumped together with delay of reinforcement, there are fundamental differences in delay of reinforcement and measures of temporal discounting, both in concept and in task design. In delay of reinforcement tasks, the subject (whether human or animal) receives a reinforcer after a delay and the effect of the delay on future responses is measured. In contrast, rewards and delays in human temporal discounting tasks are usually hypotheticals and communicated verbally: they have never and never will be experienced by the individual (Killeen, Reference Killeen2011). Presumably, the decision to delay discounting tasks is based on experience of actual delay of reinforcement in the past, combined with a prediction of how the hypothetical reward will feel in the future after an imagined delay. For example, in such tasks, the participant is given a series of choices between small immediate and larger delayed rewards (such as $50 now or $100 in a year). In the rodent tasks, rats make choices among food rewards that they have been trained to associate with different delays.

Comparison of delay discounting measures between rat strains has yielded equivocal results. Some researchers have reported steeper discounting rates in the SHRs (Aparicio et al., Reference Aparicio, Hennigan, Mulligan and Alonso-Alvarez2019; Bizot et al., Reference Bizot, Chenault, Houze, Herpin, David, Pothion and Trovero2007; Carbajal et al., Reference Carbajal, Bounmy, Harrison, Nolen, Regan, Williams and Sable2023; Fox et al., Reference Fox, Hand and Reilly2008; Orduna, Reference Orduna2015). Others have reported no difference between SHR and comparison strains (Adriani et al., Reference Adriani, Caprioli, Granstrem, Carli and Laviola2003; Garcia & Kirkpatrick, Reference Garcia and Kirkpatrick2013; Gonzalez-Barriga & Orduna, Reference Gonzalez-Barriga and Orduna2022; Ramos, Lopez-Tolsa, Sjoberg, & Pellon, Reference Ramos, Lopez-Tolsa, Sjoberg and Pellon2019).

It is unlikely that rodents decide based on rather than imagined and discounted future reward, raising questions about whether the rodent tasks measure the same constructs as human tasks (Hayden, Reference Hayden2016). The responses of rodents (and possibly humans) may, for example, indicate the effect of conditioned reinforcers established during past experiences of delayed outcomes (Killeen, Reference Killeen2011; Smith, Southern, & Kirkpatrick, Reference Smith, Southern and Kirkpatrick2023). Humans and rodents show similarly shaped discounting curves, but the discounting rate in humans is several orders of magnitude slower (months) than that measured in animals (seconds) (Hayden, Reference Hayden2016). Nevertheless, the study of delay discounting in animals has provided useful information about the neural substrates involved (Fobbs & Mizumori, Reference Fobbs and Mizumori2017).

3.4 DTD and symptoms of ADHD

The core behavioral differences predicted by the DTD hypothesis are described in Section 1. They include a steeper delay of reinforcement gradient, slower learning under partial reinforcement, faster extinction of learned behavior, a reduced partial reinforcement extinction effect (as recently demonstrated; Hulsbosch et al., Reference Hulsbosch, Beckers, De Meyer, Danckaerts, Van Liefferinge, Tripp and Van der Oord2023), and increased sensitivity to individual instances of reinforcement. From these core behavioral differences – which are also present in the SHR – we have previously proposed that DTD would cause impulsivity and inattention symptoms described in the diagnostic criteria for ADHD in DSM and ICD systems (Tripp & Wickens, Reference Tripp and Wickens2008).

The symptoms predicted by the DTD hypothesis include the following: Greater control of behavior by immediate rather than delayed reinforcement would lead to less on-task behavior in situations of infrequent reinforcement. In the absence of constant supervision, unscheduled reinforcing events would control behavior. For example, a child in the classroom may find more reinforcement in looking out the window than attending to schoolwork, for which reward is delayed and infrequent. This would produce symptoms of inattention such as failing to finish tasks and reluctance to engage in tasks requiring sustained effort in the absence of reinforcement. In situations of delayed reinforcement, DTD would also lead to impulsive behaviors such as difficulty awaiting turns or intruding on others because of the immediate reinforcement of such actions.

3.5 Personality dimensions relevant to the DTD hypothesis

As clinicians and neurobiologists, we are inclined to view personality as an effect of psychological processes on behavior, based on underlying neurobiological mechanisms. In this view, the correlational structure detected in personality research may reflect the “common cause” of variations in underlying neural mechanisms. However, the empirical basis for the structure of most personality models is self-reporting and analysis of correlations among items, rather than behavioral tests or putative neural mechanisms. Although personality traits can be said to “predict” behavioral characteristics in a statistical sense, such correlations do not establish the direction of causality. Here we suggest that variations in the transfer of phasic dopamine signals from reward to predictive cues, through its effect on core processes related to timing of behavioral reinforcement, is a potential driver of dimensions that are missing or understated in current personality models.

If ADHD is viewed as an exaggeration of normal personality traits, lying at different points on the same continua as typically developing individuals (the “spectrum” model), then we might expect the relevant personality dimensions and ADHD symptoms to share a common neurobiological basis. However, a meta-analysis examining personality dimensions of the Five-Factor Model (which defines five dimensions: neuroticism, agreeableness, conscientiousness, extraversion and openness) in relation to ADHD symptoms concluded that although there was some support for the spectrum model, “the shared variance for all significant relations between personality and ADHD was never more than 50% suggesting that the spectrum model alone does not provide sufficient explanation for the association between personality and ADHD” (Gomez & Corr, Reference Gomez and Corr2014). We would argue that this may indicate that one or more relevant dimensions are missing from the personality model. The DTD hypothesis suggests some facets of those dimensions.

Although the DTD hypothesis (Tripp & Wickens, Reference Tripp and Wickens2008) was originally developed from considering the implications of a putative “deficiency” of transfer of dopamine responses from reward to reward predictive cues, the degree of transfer is likely to vary between individuals. Such variations in association of cues with reward would lead to corresponding variations in specific behavioral characteristics. For example, variation in dopamine response to reward-predicting cues would affect the degree of perseverance in pursuit of goals in the absence of continuous reinforcement. Similarly, variation in the integration of reinforcement history would lead to a tendency to be distracted by individual instances of reward. Also, variations in the delay of reinforcement gradient would lead to different degrees of impulsive choice. Thus, the DTD hypothesis predicts variations in impulsivity, distractibility, and persistence, according to individual differences in the degree of dopamine transfer. These behavioral characteristics do not exactly align with the personality dimensions derived from the Five-Factor model, although there is some overlap, as we discuss in the following section.

3.6 Impulsivity, delay of reinforcement gradients, and temporal discounting

Impulsivity is itself a broad concept that includes multiple dimensions. For example, Whiteside and Lynam (Reference Whiteside and Lynam2003) proposed that impulsivity includes sensation seeking, lack of premeditation, lack of perseverance, negative urgency, and positive urgency. Alternatively, MacKillop et al. (Reference MacKillop, Weafer, Gray, Oshri, Palmer and de Wit2016) suggest that the latent structure among multiple measures of impulsivity has three broad categories, namely impulsive choice; impulsive action; and impulsive personality traits, reflecting self-reported attributions of self-regulatory capacity. Such self-report scales focus more on the tendency to act without forethought and inability to inhibit responses, rather sensitivity to delay of reward. However, self-report measures of impulsivity personality traits have weak relations to delay discounting (Bobova, Finn, Rickert, & Lucas, Reference Bobova, Finn, Rickert and Lucas2009; Janis & Nock, Reference Janis and Nock2009; Odum, Reference Odum2011a). Therefore, we suggest that the facets of impulsivity, most relevant to the DTD hypothesis, are impulsive choice and lack of perseverance.

Initial studies of the relationship between delay discounting and personality suggested that delay discounting was related to neuroticism and conscientiousness (Mahalingam, Stillwell, Kosinski, Rust, & Kogan, Reference Mahalingam, Stillwell, Kosinski, Rust and Kogan2014; Manning et al., Reference Manning, Hedden, Wickens, Whitfield-Gabrieli, Prelec and Gabrieli2014). However, in a large sample, Yeh et al. (Reference Yeh, Myerson and Green2021) found that discounting was not correlated with neuroticism or conscientiousness scales, and suggested that delay discounting is an important individual difference characteristic in its own right. Thus, the delay discounting rate may be related to an additional personality dimension that is not included In the Big Five, namely impulsivity (Odum, Reference Odum2011b). Consistent with this, delay discounting measures are proving useful in understanding links between neural systems and behavior in healthy individuals as well as understanding psychopathology (Lempert, Steinglass, Pinto, Kable, & Simpson, Reference Lempert, Steinglass, Pinto, Kable and Simpson2019).

3.7 Persistence

Resistance to extinction – in other words, persistent effort in the absence of reinforcement – is likely to depend on dopamine transfer. As mentioned above, the DTD hypothesis predicted a reduced partial reinforcement extinction effect in children with ADHD, which was recently supported by an experimental study (Hulsbosch et al., Reference Hulsbosch, Beckers, De Meyer, Danckaerts, Van Liefferinge, Tripp and Van der Oord2023). In a meta-analytic review of ADHD and personality measures, inattention was associated with a lack of perseverance (Gomez, Stavropoulos, Watson, Brown, & Chen, Reference Gomez, Stavropoulos, Watson, Brown and Chen2022), and the association between ADHD and perseverance was moderated by age (stronger in children than adults) and source (stronger in clinical samples than community samples). Thus, persistence appears likely to be a facet of personality dimensions related to variations in dopamine transfer.

3.8 Other personality dimensions

There are some suggestions that extraversion may be related to functional variation in dopamine responses that attach incentive value to reward-predicting cues. For example, Depue and Collins (Reference Depue and Collins1999) argue that variation in encoding of incentive salience – the intensity of stimulus representations that have become associated with reward through experience – is the main source of individual differences in extraversion. Consistent with this hypothesis studies measuring reward prediction (Smillie, Cooper, & Pickering, Reference Smillie, Cooper and Pickering2011) or reward sensitivity (Blain, Sassenberg, Xi, Zhao, & DeYoung, Reference Blain, Sassenberg, Xi, Zhao and DeYoung2021) have shown positive associations with measures of extraversion. More extraverted individuals show a greater preference for immediate rewards (Hirsh, Morisano, & Peterson, Reference Hirsh, Morisano and Peterson2008; Ostaszewski, Reference Ostaszewski1996). However, although an association of ADHD with higher extraversion has been found in some studies (Gomez & Corr, Reference Gomez and Corr2014), the finding is inconsistent across studies and the association may be limited to self-reports (Nigg et al., Reference Nigg, John, Blaskey, Huang-Pollock, Willcutt, Hinshaw and Pennington2002).

In relation to the remaining personality dimensions, Gomez and Corr (Reference Gomez and Corr2014) concluded, on the basis of a meta-analysis of 40 data sets, that ADHD symptoms of inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity were associated with measures of conscientiousness, agreeableness, and neuroticism. Consistent findings have been reported from more recent analyses (Jacobsson, Hopwood, Soderpalm, & Nilsson, Reference Jacobsson, Hopwood, Soderpalm and Nilsson2021). There is no obvious relationship between these personality dimensions and variations in dopamine transfer, although conscientiousness might tap into persistence and agreeableness into distractibility.

4. Conclusion

Animal research into the neural mechanisms of reward-related learning has shown that phasic dopamine release associated with unexpected rewards will transfer to cues that predict rewards. This process can imbue cues with incentive value. Sensory cues with incentive value have advantageous effects such as sustaining on-task behavior when reinforcement is delayed, infrequent, or discontinued. Failure to develop such cue-reward associations can lead to a preference for immediacy, increased sensitivity to delay of reinforcement and individual instances of reinforcement, and more rapid extinction of responses in the absence of continued reinforcement. The SHR has provided a useful genetic model enabling investigation of the neural mechanisms underlying these behavioral characteristics. Largely inspired by findings from animal research, we proposed the DTD hypothesis. Several predictions of this hypothesis have been tested and confirmed in both SHRs and humans. Here we further suggest that the behavioral characteristics predicted by variations in dopamine transfer underlie certain dimensions of human personality that may not feature in current personality models. These include variations in impulsivity and persistence. We hope that the insights from rodent models will encourage future studies combining behavioral measures of altered reinforcement sensitivity with symptom and personality measures in individuals with ADHD and typically developing individuals. Such studies may lead to refinement of personality dimensions by inclusion of neurobiological variations in reinforcement mechanisms.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the OIST Graduate University subsidy fund.

Competing interests

None.

Footnotes

This is a contribution to the special issue on animal personality.

References

Adriani, W., Caprioli, A., Granstrem, O., Carli, M., & Laviola, G. (2003). The spontaneously hypertensive-rat as an animal model of ADHD: Evidence for impulsive and non-impulsive subpopulations. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 27, 639651. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2003.08.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Antrop, I., Stock, P., Verte, S., Wiersema, J. R., Baeyens, D., & Roeyers, H. (2006). ADHD and delay aversion: The influence of non-temporal stimulation on choice for delayed rewards. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 11521158. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01619.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aparicio, C. F., Hennigan, P. J., Mulligan, L. J., & Alonso-Alvarez, B. (2019). Spontaneously hypertensive (SHR) rats choose more impulsively than Wistar-Kyoto (WKY) rats on a delay discounting task. Behavioural Brain Research, 364, 480493. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2017.09.040 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arcos-Burgos, M., & Acosta, M. T. (2007). Tuning major gene variants conditioning human behavior: The anachronism of ADHD. Current Opinion in Genetics & Development, 17, 234238. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gde.2007.04.011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belujon, P., & Grace, A. A. (2017). Dopamine system dysregulation in major depressive disorders. International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 20, 10361046. https://doi.org/10.1093/ijnp/pyx056 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beninger, R. J., & Freedman, N. L. (1982). The use of two operants to examine the nature of pimozide-induced decreases in responding for brain stimulation. Physiological Psychology, 10, 409412. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03332973 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berridge, K. C. (2000). Measuring hedonic impact in animals and infants: Microstructure of affective taste reactivity patterns. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 24, 173198. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0149-7634(99)00072-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berridge, K. C. (2007). The debate over dopamine’s role in reward: The case for incentive salience. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 191, 391431. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-006-0578-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bindra, D. (1974). A motivational view of learning, performance, and behavior modification. Psychological Review, 81, 199213. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0036330 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bizot, J. C., Chenault, N., Houze, B., Herpin, A., David, S., Pothion, S., & Trovero, F. (2007). Methylphenidate reduces impulsive behaviour in juvenile Wistar rats, but not in adult Wistar, SHR and WKY rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 193, 215223. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-007-0781-4 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Black, J., Belluzzi, J. D., & Stein, L. (1985). Reinforcement delay of one second severely impairs acquisition of brain self-stimulation. Brain Research, 359, 113119. https://doi.org/10.1016/0006-8993(85)91418-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blain, S. D., Sassenberg, T. A., Xi, M., Zhao, D., & DeYoung, C. G. (2021). Extraversion but not depression predicts reward sensitivity: Revisiting the measurement of anhedonic phenotypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 121, e1e18. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000371 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boakes, R. A. (1977). Performance on learning to associate a stimulus with positive reinforcement. In David, H. & Hurwitz, H. M. B. (Eds.), Operant-Pavlovian interactions (pp. 6797). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. https://doi.org/9781003150404 Google Scholar
Bobova, L., Finn, P. R., Rickert, M. E., & Lucas, J. (2009). Disinhibitory psychopathology and delay discounting in alcohol dependence: Personality and cognitive correlates. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 17, 5161. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014503 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bozhilova, N. S., Michelini, G., Kuntsi, J., & Asherson, P. (2018). Mind wandering perspective on attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuroscience Biobehavioral Reviews, 92, 464476. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.07.010 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calabresi, P., Picconi, B., Tozzi, A., & Di Filippo, M. (2007). Dopamine-mediated regulation of corticostriatal synaptic plasticity. Trends in Neuroscience, 30, 211219. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2007.03.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carbajal, M. S., Bounmy, A. J. C., Harrison, O. B., Nolen, H. G., Regan, S. L., Williams, M. T., … Sable, H. J. K. (2023). Impulsive choice in two different rat models of ADHD-Spontaneously hypertensive and Lphn3 knockout rats. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 17, 1094218. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2023.1094218 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cardinal, R. N., Winstanley, C. A., Robbins, T. W., & Everitt, B. J. (2004). Limbic corticostriatal systems and delayed reinforcement. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1021, 3350. https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1308.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carragher, N., Krueger, R. F., Eaton, N. R., Markon, K. E., Keyes, K. M., Blanco, C., … Hasin, D. S. (2014). ADHD and the externalizing spectrum: Direct comparison of categorical, continuous, and hybrid models of liability in a nationally representative sample. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 49, 13071317. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-013-0770-3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cassenaer, S., & Laurent, G. (2012). Conditional modulation of spike-timing-dependent plasticity for olfactory learning. Nature, 482, 4752. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature10776 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Catania, A. C. (1971). Reinforcement schedules: The role of responses preceding the one that produces the reinforcer. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 15, 271287. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1971.15-271 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Catania, A. C. (2005). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): Delay-of-reinforcement gradients and other behavioral mechanisms. Precommentary on Sagvolden et al, 2005. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 419425. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X05220071 Google Scholar
Catania, A. C., Sagvolden, J., & Keller, K. J. (1988). Reinforcement schedules: Retroactive and proactive effects of reinforcers inserted into fixed-interval performance. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 49, 4973. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1988.49-49 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Centonze, D., Picconi, B., Gubellini, P., Bernardi, G., & Calabresi, P. (2001). Dopaminergic control of synaptic plasticity in the dorsal striatum. European Journal of Neuroscience, 13, 10711077. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01485.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corbett, D., & Wise, R. A. (1980). Intracranial self-stimulation in relation to the ascending dopaminergic systems of the midbrain: A moveable electrode mapping study. Brain Research, 185, 115. https://doi.org/10.1016/0006-8993(80)90666-6 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Critchfield, T. S., & Lattal, K. A. (1993). Acquisition of a spatially defined operant with delayed reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 59, 373387. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1993.59-373 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Darvas, M., Wunsch, A. M., Gibbs, J. T., & Palmiter, R. D. (2014). Dopamine dependency for acquisition and performance of Pavlovian conditioned response. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences U S A, 111, 27642769. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1400332111 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davey, G. C., & Cleland, G. G. (1982). Topography of signal-centered behavior in the rat: Effects of deprivation state and reinforcer type. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 38, 291304. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1982.38-291 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Day, J. J., Roitman, M. F., Wightman, R. M., & Carelli, R. M. (2007). Associative learning mediates dynamic shifts in dopamine signaling in the nucleus accumbens. Nature Neuroscience, 10, 10201028. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1923 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Pauw, S. S., & Mervielde, I. (2011). The role of temperament and personality in problem behaviors of children with ADHD. Journal of Abnormal Child Psycholology, 39, 277291. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-010-9459-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Depue, R. A., & Collins, P. F. (1999). Neurobiology of the structure of personality: Dopamine, facilitation of incentive motivation, and extraversion. Behavioral Brain Science, 22, 491517; discussion 518–569. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x99002046 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dickinson, A., Watt, A., & Griffiths, W. (1992). Free-operant acquisition with delayed reinforcement. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section B Comparative Physiology and Psychology, 45, 241258. https://doi.org/10.1080/14640749208401019 Google Scholar
Ferster, C. B. (1953). Sustained behaviour under delayed reinforcement. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 45, 218224. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0062158 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Firestone, P., & Douglas, V. (1975). The effects of reward and punishment on reaction times and autonomic activity in hyperactive and normal children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 3, 201216. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00916751 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flagel, S. B., Clark, J. J., Robinson, T. E., Mayo, L., Czuj, A., Willuhn, I., … Akil, H. (2012). A selective role for dopamine in stimulus-reward learning. Nature, 469, 5357. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature09588 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fobbs, W. C., & Mizumori, S. J. (2017). A framework for understanding and advancing intertemporal choice research using rodent models. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 139, 8997. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2017.01.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fox, A. T., Hand, D. J., & Reilly, M. P. (2008). Impulsive choice in a rodent model of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Behavioural Brain Research, 187, 146152. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2007.09.008 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fujita, S., Okutsu, H., Yamaguchi, H., Nakamura, S., Adachi, K., Saigusa, T., & Koshikawa, N. (2003). Altered pre- and postsynaptic dopamine receptor functions in spontaneously hypertensive rat: An animal model of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Oral Science, 45, 7583. https://doi.org/10.2334/josnusd.45.75 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallistel, C. R., Stellar, J. R., & Bubis, E. (1974). Parametric analysis of brain stimulation reward in the rat: I. The transient process and the memory-containing process. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 87, 848859. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0037220 Google ScholarPubMed
Garcia, A., & Kirkpatrick, K. (2013). Impulsive choice behavior in four strains of rats: Evaluation of possible models of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Behavioural Brain Research, 238, 1022. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2012.10.017 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garrud, P., Goodall, G., & Mackintosh, N. (1981). Overshadowing of a stimulus-reinforcer association by an instrumental response. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior, 33, 123135. https://doi.org/10.1080/14640748108400817 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gomez, R., Chen, W., & Houghton, S. (2023). Differences between DSM-5-TR and ICD-11 revisions of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A commentary on implications and opportunities. World Journal of Psychiatry, 13, 139144. https://doi.org/10.5498/wjp.v13.i5.138 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gomez, R., & Corr, P. J. (2014). ADHD and personality: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 34, 376388. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2014.05.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gomez, R., Stavropoulos, V., Watson, S., Brown, T., & Chen, W. (2022). Inter-relationships between ADHD, ODD and impulsivity dimensions in emerging adults revealed by network analysis: Extending the ‘trait impulsivity hypothesis’. Heliyon, 8, e10712. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.heliyon.2022.e10712 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gonzalez-Barriga, F., & Orduna, V. (2022). Spontaneously hypertensive rats show higher impulsive action, but equal impulsive choice with both positive and aversive consequences. Behavioural Brain Research, 427, 113858. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2022.113858 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grice, G. R. (1948). The relation of secondary reinforcement to delayed reward in visual discrimination learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, 116. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0061016 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hand, D. J., Fox, A. T., & Reilly, M. P. (2006). Response acquisition with delayed reinforcement in a rodent model of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Behavioural Brain Research, 175, 337342. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2006.09.001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayden, B. Y. (2016). Time discounting and time preference in animals: A critical review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23, 3953. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-015-0879-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
He, K., Huertas, M., Hong, S. Z., Tie, X., Hell, J. W., Shouval, H., & Kirkwood, A. (2015). Distinct eligibility traces for LTP and LTD in cortical synapses. Neuron, 88, 528538. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.037 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hendley, E. D., Atwater, D. G., Myers, M. M., & Whitehorn, D. (1983). Dissociation of genetic hyperactivity and hypertension in SHR. Hypertension, 5, 211217. https://doi.org/10.1161/01.hyp.5.2.211 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hill, J. C., Herbst, K., & Sanabria, F. (2012). Characterizing operant hyperactivity in the spontaneously hypertensive rat. Behavioral Brain Functions, 8, 5. https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-8-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hirsh, J. B., Morisano, D., & Peterson, J. B. (2008). Delay discounting: Interactions between personality and cognitive ability. Journal of Research in Personality, 42, 16461650. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2008.07.005 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoerger, M. L., & Mace, F. C. (2006). A computerized test of self-control predicts classroom behavior. Journal of Applied Behaviour Analysis, 39, 147159. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2006.171-04 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hulsbosch, A. K., Beckers, T., De Meyer, H., Danckaerts, M., Van Liefferinge, D., Tripp, G., & Van der Oord, S. (2023). Instrumental learning and behavioral persistence in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity-disorder: Does reinforcement frequency matter? The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 64, 16311640. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.13805 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iaboni, F., Douglas, V. I., & Baker, A. G. (1995). Effects of reward and response costs on inhibition in ADHD children. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104, 232240. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.104.1.232 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jackson, J. N., & MacKillop, J. (2016). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and monetary delay discounting: A meta-analysis of case-control studies. Biolological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 1, 316325. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2016.01.007 Google ScholarPubMed
Jacobsson, P., Hopwood, C. J., Soderpalm, B., & Nilsson, T. (2021). Adult ADHD and emerging models of maladaptive personality: A meta-analytic review. BMC Psychiatry, 21, 282. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-021-03284-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Janis, I. B., & Nock, M. K. (2009). Are self-injurers impulsive?: Results from two behavioral laboratory studies. Psychiatry Research, 169, 261267. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2008.06.041 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johansen, E. B., Killeen, P. R., Russell, V. A., Tripp, G., Wickens, J. R., Tannock, R., … Sagvolden, T. (2009). Origins of altered reinforcement effects in ADHD. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 5, 7. https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-5-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johansen, E. B., Killeen, P. R., & Sagvolden, T. (2007). Behavioral variability, elimination of responses, and delay-of-reinforcement gradients in SHR and WKY rats. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 3, 60. https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-3-60 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johansen, E. B., Sagvolden, T., & Kvande, G. (2005). Effects of delayed reinforcers on the behavior of an animal model of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Behavioural Brain Research, 162, 4761. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2005.02.034 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Killeen, P. R. (2011). Models of trace decay, eligibility for reinforcement, and delay of reinforcement gradients, from exponential to hyperboloid. Behavioural Processes, 87, 5763. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2010.12.016 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Killeen, P. R., & Sitomer, M. T. (2003). MPR. Behavioural Processes, 62, 4964. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0376-6357(03)00017-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, K. M., Baratta, M. V., Yang, A., Lee, D., Boyden, E. S., & Fiorillo, C. D. (2012). Optogenetic mimicry of the transient activation of dopamine neurons by natural reward is sufficient for operant reinforcement. PLoS One, 7, e33612. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0033612 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lattal, K. A., & Gleeson, S. (1990). Response acquisition with delayed reinforcement. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 16, 2739. https://doi.org/10.1037/0097-7403.16.1.27 Google ScholarPubMed
Lempert, K., Steinglass, J., Pinto, A., Kable, J., & Simpson, H. (2019). Can delay discounting deliver on the promise of RDoC? Psychological Medicine, 49, 190199. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291718001770.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levy, F. (1991). The dopamine theory of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 25, 277283. https://doi.org/10.3109/00048679109077746 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levy, F., Hay, D. A., McStephen, M., Wood, C., & Waldman, I. (1997). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A category or a continuum? Genetic analysis of a large-scale twin study. Journal of American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 737744. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199706000-00009 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, Y.-T., Huang, Y.-L., Chen, J.-J., Hyland, B. I., & Wickens, J. R. (Submitted). Phasic dopamine response to reward in spontaneously hypertensive and Sprague-Dawley rats: Effects of low-dose methylphenidate.Google Scholar
Ljungberg, T., Apicella, P., & Schultz, W. (1992). Responses of monkey dopamine neurons during learning of behavioral reactions. Journal of Neurophysiology, 67, 145163. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.1992.67.1.145 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lynch, M. R., & Wise, R. A. (1985). Relative effectiveness of pimozide, haloperidol and trifluoperazine on self-stimulation rate-intensity functions. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 23, 777780. https://doi.org/10.1016/0091-3057(85)90071-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacKillop, J., Weafer, J., Gray, J. C., Oshri, A., Palmer, A., & de Wit, H. (2016). The latent structure of impulsivity: Impulsive choice, impulsive action, and impulsive personality traits. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 233, 33613370. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-016-4372-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mahalingam, V., Stillwell, D., Kosinski, M., Rust, J., & Kogan, A. (2014). Who can wait for the future? A personality perspective. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 5, 573583. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550613515007 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manning, J., Hedden, T., Wickens, N., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., Prelec, D., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2014). Personality influences temporal discounting preferences: Behavioral and brain evidence. Neuroimage, 98, 4249. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.04.066 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marx, I., Hacker, T., Yu, X., Cortese, S., & Sonuga-Barke, E. (2021). ADHD and the choice of small immediate over larger delayed rewards: A comparative meta-analysis of performance on simple choice-delay and temporal discounting paradigms. Journal of Attention Disorders, 25, 171187. https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054718772138 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCarty, R., & Kopin, I. J. (1979). Patterns of behavioral development in spontaneously hypertensive rats and Wistar-Kyoto normotensive controls. Developmental Psychobiology, 12, 239243. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.420120307 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, E. M., Pomerleau, F., Huettl, P., Russell, V. A., Gerhardt, G. A., & Glaser, P. E. (2012). The spontaneously hypertensive and Wistar Kyoto rat models of ADHD exhibit sub-regional differences in dopamine release and uptake in the striatum and nucleus accumbens. Neuropharmacology, 63, 13271334. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropharm.2012.08.020 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mullins-Sweatt, S. N., Bornovalova, M. A., Carragher, N., Clark, L. A., Corona Espinosa, A., Jonas, K., … Watts, A. L. (2022). HiTOP assessment of externalizing antagonism and disinhibition. Assessment, 29, 3445. https://doi.org/10.1177/10731911211033900 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Myers, N. A. (1960). Extinction following partial and continuous primary and secondary reinforcement. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 172179. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0040187 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nigg, J. T., John, O. P., Blaskey, L. G., Huang-Pollock, C. L., Willcutt, E. G., Hinshaw, S. P., & Pennington, B. (2002). Big five dimensions and ADHD symptoms: Links between personality traits and clinical symptoms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 451469. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.83.2.451 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nigg, J. T., Karalunas, S. L., Feczko, E., & Fair, D. A. (2020). Toward a revised nosology for Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder heterogeneity. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 5, 726737. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2020.02.005 Google Scholar
Odum, A. L. (2011a). Delay discounting: I’m a k, you’re a k. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 96, 427439. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2011.96-423 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Odum, A. L. (2011b). Delay discounting: Trait variable? Behavioural Processes, 87, 19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2011.02.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Okamoto, K., & Aoki, K. (1963). Development of a strain of spontaneously hypertensive rats. Japanese Circulation Journal, 27, 282293. https://doi.org/10.1253/jcj.27.282 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Okouchi, H., & Lattal, K. A. (2006). An analysis of reinforcement history effects. Journal of the Experimental Analydid of Behavior, 86, 3142. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2006.75-05 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Orduna, V. (2015). Impulsivity and sensitivity to amount and delay of reinforcement in an animal model of ADHD. Behavioural Brain Research, 294, 6271. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2015.07.046 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ostaszewski, P. (1996). The relation between temperament and rate of remporal discounting. European Journal of Personality, 10, 161172. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-0984 3.0.CO;2-R>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pan, W. X., Schmidt, R., Wickens, J. R., & Hyland, B. I. (2005). Dopamine cells respond to predicted events during classical conditioning: Evidence for eligibility traces in the reward-learning network. Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 62356242. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1478-05.2005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patros, C. H., Alderson, R. M., Kasper, L. J., Tarle, S. J., Lea, S. E., & Hudec, K. L. (2016). Choice-impulsivity in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 43, 162174. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2015.11.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pawlak, V., & Kerr, J. N. (2008). Dopamine receptor activation is required for corticostriatal spike-timing-dependent plasticity. Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 24352446. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4402-07.2008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perin, C. T. (1943). A quantitative investigation of the delay-of-reinforcement gradient. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 3751. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0056738 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Phillips, R. D., Walsh, E. C., Zurcher, N. R., Lalush, D. S., Kinard, J. L., Tseng, C. E., … Dichter, G. S. (2023). Striatal dopamine in anhedonia: A simultaneous [(11)C]raclopride positron emission tomography and functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 333, 111660. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2023.111660 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ramos, S., Lopez-Tolsa, G. E., Sjoberg, E. A., & Pellon, R. (2019). Effect of schedule-induced behavior on responses of spontaneously hypertensive and Wistar-Kyoto rats in a delay-discounting task: A preliminary report. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 13, 255. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00255 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Renner, E. K. (1964). Delay of reinforcement: A historical review. Psychological Bulletin, 61, 341361. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0048335 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reynolds, J. N. J., Hyland, B. I., & Wickens, J. R. (2001). A cellular mechanism of reward-related learning. Nature, 413, 6770. https://doi.org/10.1038/35092560 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roessner, V., Sagvolden, T., Dasbanerjee, T., Middleton, F. A., Faraone, S. V., Walaas, S. I., … Bock, N. (2010). Methylphenidate normalizes elevated dopamine transporter densities in an animal model of the attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder combined type, but not to the same extent in one of the attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder inattentive type. Neuroscience, 167, 11831191. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.02.073 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sagvolden, T. (2000). Behavioral validation of the spontaneously hypertensive rat (SHR) as an animal model of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (AD/HD). Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 24, 3139. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0149-7634(99)00058-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sagvolden, T., Aase, H., Zeiner, P., & Berger, D. (1998). Altered reinforcement mechanisms in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Behavioural Brain Research, 94, 6171. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4328(97)00170-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sagvolden, T., Hendley, E. D., & Knardahl, S. (1992). Behavior of hypertensive and hyperactive rat strains: Hyperactivity is not unitarily determined. Physiology & Behavior, 52, 4957. https://doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(92)90432-2 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sagvolden, T., Johansen, E. B., Aase, H., & Russell, V. A. (2005). A dynamic developmental theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) predominantly hyperactive/impulsive and combined subtypes. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 397419; discussion 419–368. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X05000075 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sagvolden, T., Metzger, M. A., Schiorbeck, H. K., Rugland, A. L., Spinnangr, I., & Sagvolden, G. (1992). The spontaneously hypertensive rat (SHR) as an animal model of childhood hyperactivity (ADHD): Changed reactivity to reinforcers and to psychomotor stimulants. Behavioral & Neural Biology, 58, 103112. https://doi.org/10.1016/0163-1047(92)90315-u CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sagvolden, T., Pettersen, M. B., & Larsen, M. C. (1993). Spontaneously hypertensive rats (SHR) as a putative animal model of childhood hyperkinesis: SHR behavior compared to four other rat strains. Physiology & Behavior, 54, 10471055. https://doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(93)90323-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sagvolden, T., Russell, V. A., Aase, H., Johansen, E. B., & Farshbaf, M. (2005). Rodent models of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biolological Psychiatry, 57, 12391247. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.02.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sanabria, F., & Killeen, P. R. (2008). Evidence for impulsivity in the spontaneously hypertensive rat drawn from complementary response-withholding tasks. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 4, 7. https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-4-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scheres, A., Dijkstra, M., Ainslie, E., Balkan, J., Reynolds, B., Sonuga-Barke, E., & Castellanos, F. X. (2006). Temporal and probabilistic discounting of rewards in children and adolescents: Effects of age and ADHD symptoms. Neuropsychologia, 44, 20922103. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.10.012 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schultz, W. (2002). Getting formal with dopamine and reward. Neuron, 36, 241263. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00967-4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schultz, W., Apicella, P., & Ljungberg, T. (1993). Responses of monkey dopamine neurons to reward and conditioned stimuli during successive steps of learning a delayed response task. Journal of Neuroscience, 13, 900913. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.13-03-00900.1993 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schultz, W., Dayan, P., & Montague, P. R. (1997). A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science, 275, 15931599. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.275.5306.159 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shen, W., Flajolet, M., Greengard, P., & Surmeier, D. J. (2008). Dichotomous dopaminergic control of striatal synaptic plasticity. Science, 321, 848851. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1160575 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shindou, T., Shindou, M., Watanabe, S., & Wickens, J. (2019). A silent eligibility trace enables dopamine-dependent synaptic plasticity for reinforcement learning in the mouse striatum. European Journal of Neuroscience, 49, 726736. https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.13921 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silic, B., Aggarwal, M., Liyanagama, K., Tripp, G., & Wickens, J. R. (2023). Conditioned approach behavior of SHR and SD rats during Pavlovian conditioning. Behavioural Brain Research, 443, 114348. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2023.114348 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simpson, F. O., Phelan, E. L., Clark, D. W., Jones, D. R., Gresson, C. R., Lee, D. R., & Bird, D. L. (1973). Studies on the New Zealand strain of genetically hypertensive rats. Clinical Science (Mol Med Suppl.), 45(Suppl 1), 15s21s. https://doi.org/10.1042/cs045015s Google ScholarPubMed
Smillie, L. D., Cooper, A. J., & Pickering, A. D. (2011). Individual differences in reward-prediction-error: Extraversion and feedback-related negativity. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 6, 646652. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsq078 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smirk, F. H., & Hall, W. H. (1958). Inherited hypertension in rats. Nature, 182, 727728. https://doi.org/10.1038/182727a0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, T. R., Southern, R., & Kirkpatrick, K. (2023). Mechanisms of impulsive choice: Experiments to explore and models to map the empirical terrain. Learning & Behavior. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-023-00577-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, Y., Bennett, B. D., Bolam, J. P., Parent, A., & Sadikot, A. F. (1994). Synaptic relationships between dopaminergic afferents and cortical or thalamic input in the sensorimotor territory of the striatum in monkey. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 344, 119. https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.903440102 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sonuga-Barke, E. J., Sergeant, J. A., Nigg, J., & Willcutt, E. (2008). Executive dysfunction and delay aversion in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Nosologic and diagnostic implications. Child and Adolescent Psychiatrics Clinics of North America, 17, 367384. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chc.2007.11.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sonuga-Barke, E. J., Taylor, E., Sembi, S., & Smith, J. (1992). Hyperactivity and delay aversion--I. The effect of delay on choice. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 387398. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1992.tb00874.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stein, D. J., Lund, C., & Nesse, R. M. (2013). Classification systems in psychiatry: Diagnosis and global mental health in the era of DSM-5 and ICD-11. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 26, 493497. https://doi.org/10.1097/YCO.0b013e3283642dfd CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sutherland, K. R., Alsop, B., McNaughton, N., Hyland, B. I., Tripp, G., & Wickens, J. R. (2009). Sensitivity to delay of reinforcement in two animal models of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Behavioural Brain Research, 205, 372376. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2009.07.011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, L. W. (1982). The projections of the ventral tegmental area and adjacent regions: A combined fluorescent retrograde tracer and immunofluorescence study in the rat. Brain Research Bulletin, 9, 321353. https://doi.org/10.1016/0361-9230(82)90145-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tarpy, R., & Sawabini, F. L. (1974). Reinforcement delay: A selective review of the last decade. Psychological Bulletin, 81, 984997. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0037428 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tripp, G., & Alsop, B. (1999). Sensitivity to reward frequency in boys with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 28, 366375. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15374424jccp280309 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tripp, G., & Alsop, B. (2001). Sensitivity to reward delay in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 691698. Retrieved from https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11464973 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tripp, G., & Wickens, J. R. (2008). Research review: Dopamine transfer deficit: A neurobiological theory of altered reinforcement mechanisms in ADHD. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, 691704. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01851.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tripp, G., & Wickens, J. R. (2009). Neurobiology of ADHD. Neuropharmacology, 57, 579589. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropharm.2009.07.026 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watanabe, S., Fujita, M., Ito, Y., Okada, T., Kusuoka, T., & Nishimura, T. (1997). Brain dopamine transporter in spontaneously hypertensive rats. Journal of Nuclear Medicine, 38, 470474.Google ScholarPubMed
Wearden, J. H., & Lejeune, H. (2006). “The stone which the builders rejected…”: Delay of reinforcement and response rate on fixed-interval and related schedules. Behavioural Processes, 71, 7787. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2005.08.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whiteside, S. P., & Lynam, D. R. (2003). Understanding the role of impulsivity and externalizing psychopathology in alcohol abuse: Application of the UPPS impulsive behavior scale. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 11, 210217. https://doi.org/10.1037/1064-1297.11.3.210 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wickens, J. R. (2008). Synaptic plasticity in the basal ganglia. Behavioual Brain Research. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2008.10.030 Google ScholarPubMed
Wickens, J. R., Begg, A. J., & Arbuthnott, G. W. (1996). Dopamine reverses the depression of rat corticostriatal synapses which normally follows high-frequency stimulation of cortex in vitro. Neuroscience, 70, 15. https://doi.org/0306-4522(95)00436-M CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wickens, J. R., Hyland, B. I., & Tripp, G. (2011). Animal models to guide clinical drug development in ADHD: Lost in translation? British Journal of Pharmacology, 164, 11071128. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1476-5381.2011.01412.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wickens, J. R., Macfarlane, J., Booker, C., & McNaughton, N. (2004). Dissociation of hypertension and fixed interval responding in two separate strains of genetically hypertensive rat. Behavioural Brain Research, 152, 393401. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2003.10.023 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wickens, J. R., & Tripp, E. G. (1998). A biological theory of ADHD: Dopamine timing is off. International Journal of Neuroscience, 97, 252.Google Scholar
Williams, J., & Dayan, P. (2005). Dopamine, learning, and impulsivity: A biological account of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 15, 160179. https://doi.org/10.1089/cap.2005.15.160 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willner, P., Hale, A. S., & Argyropoulos, S. (2005). Dopaminergic mechanism of antidepressant action in depressed patients. Journal of Affective Disorders, 86, 3745. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2004.12.010 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Winstanley, C. A., Theobald, D. E., Cardinal, R. N., & Robbins, T. W. (2004). Contrasting roles of basolateral amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex in impulsive choice. Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 47184722. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5606-03.2004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wise, R. A. (2004). Dopamine, learning and motivation. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 483494. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1406 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wise, R. A. (2006). Role of brain dopamine in food reward and reinforcement. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society London B Biological Science, 361, 11491158. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2006.1854 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wise, R. A., & Bozarth, M. A. (1984). Brain reward circuitry: Four circuit elements “wired” in apparent series. Brain Research Bulletin, 297, 265273. https://doi.org/10.1016/0361-9230(84)90190-4 Google Scholar
Wultz, B., & Sagvolden, T. (1992). The hyperactive spontaneously hypertensive rat learns to sit still, but not to stop bursts of responses with short interresponse times. Behavior Genetics, 22, 415433. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01066613 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yagishita, S., Hayashi-Takagi, A., Ellis-Davies, G. C., Urakubo, H., Ishii, S., & Kasai, H. (2014). A critical time window for dopamine actions on the structural plasticity of dendritic spines. Science, 345, 16161620. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1255514 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yeh, Y. H., Myerson, J., & Green, L. (2021). Delay discounting, cognitive ability, and personality: What matters? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 28, 686694. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-020-01777-w CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. Transfer of dopamine response from actual reward to cues predicts behavioral characteristics. Traces show idealized dopamine signal in normal (a) and hypothesized dopamine transfer deficit (b). In both cases, unexpected primary reward causes a dopamine response. Normally, after repeated pairing of cue and reward, the dopamine response transfers to the cue. When there is a dopamine transfer deficit the cue response fails to develop as strongly as normal, and the response to the actual reward persists. Compared to normal rat strains, the SHR shows a dopamine transfer deficit. This is associated with characteristic of immediate over-delayed reward. In humans, a dopamine transfer deficit may give rise to symptoms of ADHD. These can be viewed as extremes of normal variations in individual personality traits.

Figure 1

Figure 2. Delay of reinforcement gradient interpretation of fixed-interval responding. (a) Classic representation of the control exerted by delayed reinforcers. Habit strength is plotted as a function of time of reinforcement, based on the formula ${1 \over L} = {0.22^{ - 0.215t}} - 0.0188t + 0.32 $ fitted to experimentally measured latency or responses, L, for reinforcement at different delay times, t, (Perin, 1943). (b) Example of FI responding showing the increase in response rate as the time of reinforcer delivery approaches for four different strains of rat. GH, Genetically Hypertensive, SHR, Spontaneously Hypertensive, WKY, Wistar Kiyoto, WI, Wistar. Redrawn from Wickens et al. (2004).