Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-dh8gc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-18T06:18:33.993Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Mind, Society, and the Growth of Knowledge

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 April 2022

Paul Thagard*
Affiliation:
Philosophy Department, University of Waterloo
*
Send reprint requests to the author, Philosophy Department, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario, N2L 3G1, Canada.

Abstract

Explanations of the growth of scientific knowledge can be characterized in terms of logical, cognitive, and social schemas. But cognitive and social schemas are complementary rather than competitive, and purely social explanations of scientific change are as inadequate as purely cognitive explanations. For example, cognitive explanations of the chemical revolution must be supplemented by and combined with social explanations, and social explanations of the rise of the mechanical worldview must be supplemented by and combined with cognitive explanations. Rational appraisal of cognitive and social strategies for improving knowledge should appreciate the interdependence of mind and society.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Philosophy of Science Association 1994

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

Thanks to Paul Rusnock, Cameron Shelley, Miriam Solomon, and Jim van Evra for comments on an earlier draft, and to Kathleen Gorman for research assistance. This work is supported by a grant from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada.

References

Barnes, B. (1985), About Science. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Bloor, D. (1991), Knowledge and Social Imagery. 2d ed. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Brown, J. R. (ed.), (1984), Scientific Rationality: The Sociological Turn. Dordrecht: Reidel.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, J. R. (ed.), (1989), The Rational and the Social. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Churchland, P. (1989), A Neurocomputational Perspective. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Collins, H. (1985), Changing Order: Replication and Induction in Scientific Practice. London: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Conant, J. (1964), Harvard Case Histories in Experimental Science. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Darden, L. (1991), Theory Change in Science: Strategies from Mendelian Genetics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1958), The Autobiography of Charles Darwin and Selected Letters. New York: Dover.Google Scholar
Donovan, A. (1988), “The Chemical Revolution: Essays in Reinterpretation”, Osiris 4.Google Scholar
Downes, S. M. (1993), “Socializing Naturalized Philosophy of Science”, Philosophy of Science 60: 452468.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Falkenhainer, B.; Forbus, K. D.; and Gentner, D. (1989), “The Structure-Mapping Engine: Algorithms and Examples”, Artificial Intelligence 41: 163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gärdenfors, P. (1988), Knowledge in Flux. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gentner, D. (1983), “Structure-Mapping: A Theoretical Framework for Analogy”, Cognitive Science 7: 155170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giere, R. (1988), Explaining Science: A Cognitive Approach. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.10.7208/chicago/9780226292038.001.0001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giere, R. (1992), Cognitive Models of Science. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Goldman, A. (1986), Epistemology and Cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Goldman, A. (1992), Liaisons: Philosophy Meets the Cognitive and Social Sciences. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Guerlac, H. (1961), Lavoisier—The Crucial Year. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Guyton de Morveau, L. B.; Lavoisier, A. L.; Berthollet, C. L.; and de Fourcroy, A. F. (1787), Méthode de Nomenclature Chimique. Paris: Cuchet.Google Scholar
Hadden, R. W. (1988), “Social Relations and the Content of Early Modern Science”, British Journal of Sociology 39: 255280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harman, G. (1986), Change in View: Principles of Reasoning. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Holland, J. H.; Holyoak, K. J.; Nisbett, R. E.; and Thagard, P. R. (1986), Induction: Processes of Inference, Learning, and Discovery. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Holmes, F. (1985), Lavoisier and the Chemistry of Life. Madison, WI: University of Wisconsin Press.Google Scholar
Holyoak, K. J. and Thagard, P. (1989), “Analogical Mapping by Constraint Satisfaction”, Cognitive Science 13: 295355.10.1207/s15516709cog1303_1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holyoak, K. J. and Thagard, P. (forthcoming), Mental Leaps: Analogy in Creative Thought. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howson, C. and Urbach, P. (1989), Scientific Reasoning: The Bayesian Tradition. La Salle, IL: Open Court.Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N. and Byrne, R. M. (1991), Deduction. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kitcher, P. (1981), “Explanatory Unification”, Philosophy of Science 48: 507531.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kitcher, P. (1993), The Advancement of Science. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kunda, Z. (1990), “The Case for Motivated Inference”, Psychological Bulletin 108: 480498.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langley, P.; Simon, H.; Bradshaw, G.; and Zytkow, J. (1987), Scientific Discovery. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Latour, B. and Woolgar, S. (1986), Laboratory Life: The Construction of Scientific Facts. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Leake, D. B. (1992), Evaluating Explanations: A Content Theory. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Levi, I. (1991), The Fixation of Belief and Its Undoing. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levin, A. (1984), “Venel, Lavoisier, Fourcroy, Cabanis, and the Idea of Scientific Revolution”, History of Science 22: 303320.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Longino, H. (1990), Science as Social Knowledge: Values and Objectivity in Scientific Inquiry. Princeton: Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCann, H. (1978), Chemistry Transformed: The Paradigmatic Shift from Phlogiston to Oxygen. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
McEvoy, J. G. (1988), “Continuity and Discontinuity in the Chemical Revolution”, Osiris 4: 195213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perrin, C. E. (1987), “Revolution or Reform: The Chemical Revolution and Eighteenth Century Concepts of Scientific Change”, History of Science 25: 395423.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perrin, C. E. (1988), “The Chemical Revolution: Shifts in Guiding Assumptions”, in Donovan, A., Laudan, L., and Laudan, R., (eds.), A. Donovan, L. Laudan, and R. Laudan, Dordrecht: Kluwer, pp. 105124.Google Scholar
Read, S. and Marcus-Newhall, A. (1993), “The Role of Explanatory Coherence in the Construction of Social Explanations”, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 65: 429447.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schank, P. and Ranney, M. (1992), “Assessing Explanatory Coherence: A New Method for Integrating Verbal Data with Models of On-Line Belief Revision”, in Proceedings of the Fourteenth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 599604.Google Scholar
Schank, R. C. (1986), Explanation Patterns: Understanding Mechanically and Creatively. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Solomon, M. (1992), “Scientific Rationality and Human Reasoning”, Philosophy of Science 59: 439455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Solomon, M. (1994), “Social Empiricism”, Nous 28: 325342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thagard, P. (1988), Computational Philosophy of Science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thagard, P. (1989), “Explanatory Coherence”, Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12: 435467.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thagard, P. (1990), “The Conceptual Structure of the Chemical Revolution”, Philosophy of Science 57: 183209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thagard, P. (1992), Conceptual Revolutions. Princeton: Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thagard, P. (1993), “Societies of Minds: Science as Distributed Computing”, Studies in History and Philosophy of Science 24: 4967.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thagard, P.; Holyoak, K.; Nelson, G.; and Gochfeld, D. (1990), “Analog Retrieval by Constraint Satisfaction”, Artificial Intelligence 46: 259310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomson, T. (1813), “Biographical Account of M. Lavoisier”, Annals of Philosophy 2: 8192.Google Scholar
Woolgar, S. (1988), Science: The Very Idea. Chichester, Sussex: Ellis Horwood.Google Scholar