Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-ndw9j Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-17T19:06:36.183Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Theoretical approaches to grammatical tone

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 December 2023

Florian Lionnet*
Affiliation:
Program in Linguistics, Princeton University, 1-S-19 Green Hall, Princeton, NJ 08544, United States of America
Laura McPherson
Affiliation:
Department of Linguistics, Dartmouth College, 64 College Street, Hanover, NH 03755, United States of America
Nicholas Rolle
Affiliation:
Leibniz-Zentrum Allgemeine Sprachwissenschaft, Schützenstraße 18, Berlin 10117, Germany
*
Corresponding author: Florian Lionnet; Email: flionnet@princeton.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Tone is distinct from other phonological phenomena both qualitatively and quantitatively (Hyman 2011), and has been instrumental in shaping phonological theory in many ways. However, the contributions to current linguistic theory of “grammatical tone’ (GT) – a type of non-concatenative morphology where a morpheme is expressed at least in part by tone and/or tone changes – have been less apparent. In this paper, we take stock of the types of GT patterns attested in the literature and the different theoretical treatments of GT that have been proposed to date. We show that GT is still to a large extent underexplored, and highlight the immense potential of the study of GT for improving our understanding of phonology and its outer limits. This paper serves as an introduction to the high-quality research articles collected in this special issue, which directly address how GT critically informs phonological theory and its current developments.

Type
Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

1. Introduction

Tone is distinct from other phonological phenomena in numerous ways qualitatively and quantitatively, as illustrated by the many examples in Larry M. Hyman’s presidential address at the 92nd Annual Meeting of the Linguistic Society of America (Hyman Reference Hyman2018). In the history of the field, tone has been instrumental in shaping phonological theory, for example, establishing phonological representations as multi-tiered under Autosegmental Theory (Goldsmith Reference Goldsmith1976, Reference Goldsmith1990) and establishing the existence of tone circles in certain Chinese tone sandhi patterns (Chen Reference Chen1987), to mention but two examples. However, the contributions to current linguistic theory from ‘grammatical tone’ have been less apparent. This thematic issue is a collection of papers addressing the topic of grammatical tone, specifically aiming to unite various empirical and theoretical perspectives.

As a starting point, we can broadly define grammatical tone as a tonological pattern (e.g. involving tone addition, deletion, replacement, shifting, assimilation, dissimilation, etc.), which is restricted to a specific morpheme or construction or a natural class of morphemes or constructions, and not attributable to the general tonal phonology. A straightforward example is given in (1) from the Nigerian language Kalabari (ijn; Harry Reference Harry2004). Here, the imperative is expressed by adding a high-low (HL) grammatical tone sequence to the end of the verb, with no additional marking. Notice that the grammatical tones co-occur with the lexical tones rather than replacing them, resulting in otherwise marked contour tones and vowel lengthening. Other data in the language reveal that this HL sequence cannot be interpreted simply as intonational boundary tones (see e.g. (3) below).

In examples like Kalabari, grammatical tone is straightforwardly analysed as ‘floating tones’ which concatenate to the edge of the word akin to segmental affixes. While such patterns are extremely common and perhaps prototypical of grammatical tone patterns, they represent the starting point of grammatical tone typology rather than its end point. Consider next the Bantu language Makonde (kde; Kraal Reference Kraal2005: 255–257), in (2). The left-hand columns illustrate nouns in isolation, which show six basic lexical tone melodies whose surface forms are dictated by various general tonal operations. In the context of the modifier ntwaani ‘what kind of’, however, all nouns are neutralised to a basic LH pattern. This kind of tone alternation is not a general rule of the language but rather one triggered by the modifier itself, thus meeting our definition of grammatical tone. But unlike in the Kalabari example in (1), simple concatenation of floating tone alone appears insufficient to derive the pattern.

In the last two and a half decades, there has been a surge of in-depth descriptive and analytic work on such patterns of grammatical tone (hereafter GT). In Africa, a sample of families in which GT is pervasive include Dogon (Heath Reference Heath2008; McPherson Reference McPherson2014; McPherson & Heath Reference McPherson and Heath2016), Mabia/Gur (Hyman & Olawsky Reference Hyman, Olawsky, Githiora, Littlefield and Manfredi2003; Roberts Reference Roberts2016), Mande (McPherson Reference McPherson2017, Reference McPherson2019; Green Reference Green2018; Konoshenko Reference Konoshenko2018), Ijoid (Efere Reference Efere2001; Harry Reference Harry2004; Harry & Hyman Reference Harry and Hyman2014; Rolle Reference Rolle2021), Bantu (Odden & Bickmore Reference Odden and Bickmore2014) and Nilotic (Andersen Reference Andersen1995; Trommer Reference Trommer2011), among others. Extensive work on GT has been conducted outside of Africa, as well. In the Americas, perhaps the most famous cases of robust GT are found in the Otomanguean family in Mexico, which includes Zapotec, Mixtec and Chatino sub-groups (Campbell Reference Campbell2014; McIntosh Reference McIntosh2015; Sullivant Reference Sullivant2015; Villard Reference Villard2015; Palancar & Léonard Reference Palancar and Léonard2016). Further, in Asia GT is found widely in the Japonic family (Kawahara & Wolf Reference Kawahara and Wolf2010; Kubozono & Giriko Reference Kubozono and Giriko2018; Matsumori Reference Matsumori2019), and to a lesser degree in other Asian languages which are otherwise ‘GT-poor’, such as Cantonese (yue; e.g. ‘changed tone’, Alderete et al. Reference Alderete, Chan and Tanaka2022), Burmese (mya; incipient GT for possessor marking, Allott Reference Allott1967; Tian Reference Tian2018) and Dzongkha (dzo; incipient GT for ergative marking, Watters Reference Watters2018: 127).

From these collective efforts, we now know that GT demonstrates a unique configuration of properties, above and beyond the already well-known remarkability of tone generally. Templatic effects are found in great regularity across African GT systems, surpassing in regularity and scope their more famous segmental template counterparts à la Semitic and Yokuts. One example comes from Kalabari, a language introduced already. Here, the lexical tone of a noun is replaced with one of four tonal templates (L, HL, LH or HH) depending on the modifier it co-occurs with. The core patterns are shown in (3) (Harry & Hyman Reference Harry and Hyman2014: 651).

Such patterns of ‘tonal overwriting’ in (3) – as well as in (2) from Makonde – are widespread in GT systems, and are often not bound to the word from which they originate. A particularly extreme case comes from the Orungu dialect of Myene (mye; Bantu, Maniacky & Ambouroue Reference Maniacky and Ambouroue2014: 252, 257), shown in (4). Most inflectional contexts have a quite limited GT effect (if any), for example, the negative past in (4a), whose prefixes sponsor no GT, and the floating tone of the root (conventionally represented as ) does not venture beyond its own word boundaries. In contrast, in the superficially similar negative present in (4b), the floating GT replaces the tones of all words to its right, up to the clause boundary. Similarly, contexts such as the negative subjunctive in (4c) sponsor a floating GT, which equally spreads in an unbounded manner to the right. Here, tonal overwriting is clause-bound, rather than word- or even phrase-bound.

Furthermore, one recent case of grammatical tone circulated widely in the literature comes from another Bantu language Kuria (kuj; Odden Reference Odden and Odden1987; Cammenga Reference Cammenga1994, Reference Cammenga2004; Mwita Reference Mwita2008; Marlo et al. Reference Marlo, Mwita and Paster2014, Reference Marlo, Mwita and Paster2015; Rolle & Lionnet Reference Rolle, Lionnet, Baek, Takahashi and Yeung2020; Sande et al. Reference Sande, Jenks and Inkelas2020; Trommer Reference Trommerin press), and has caused phonologists to reassess the received wisdom that ‘grammars don’t count’ (on counting effects: McCarthy & Prince Reference McCarthy and Prince1986; Kenstowicz Reference Kenstowicz1994: 372; Hayes Reference Hayes1995: 307; Smith & Tsimpli Reference Smith and Tsimpli1995: 312; Isac & Reiss Reference Isac and Reiss2008: 65; Graf Reference Graf2017; Paster Reference Paster2019; Kawahara & Kumagai Reference Kawahara and Kumagai2023; see these last two works for extensive references). In Kuria, a high GT is assigned to the first, second, third or fourth mora of the prosodic stem depending on the grammatical context. (5) summarises the basic patterns, with page references to Mwita (Reference Mwita2008). (Note that various language-general spreading rules are not reflected in these forms, as they are orthogonal to the grammatical tone patterns.)

The clause-level tonal overwriting from Orungu and the counting effects in Kuria are excellent examples of tone going above and beyond what is expected of segmental counterparts. In comparison, we know of no clear and convincing case of a language where vowel harmony is triggered by a prefix but spreads unbounded to the opposite clause edge (for related commentary, see Kaisse Reference Kaisse, Zhang and Qian2019, and for a reported case of sentence-level harmony in Isaaq Somali [som], see Andrzejewski Reference Andrzejewski1955). We sketch such a pattern in (6), where we call it ‘Faux Orungu’. Likewise, we know of no language in which inflectionally restricted umlaut idiosyncratically targets a numerically defined position in the word, as imagined in (7) in ‘Faux Kuria’ using a hypothetical stem /turuŋana/.

Other notable properties making grammatical tone distinct are non-local assignment of GT on the tonal tier (Tommo So [dto]; McPherson & Heath Reference McPherson and Heath2016: 623), GT demonstrating affix faithfulness as commonly as root faithfulness (Rolle Reference Rolle2018; cf. McCarthy & Prince Reference McCarthy and Prince1995; Beckman Reference Beckman1998; Urbanczyk Reference Urbanczyk, van Oostendorp, Ewen, Hume and Rice2011), across-the-board paradigmatic tone polarity (Kipsigis [sgc]; Kouneli & Nie Reference Kouneli and Nie2021) and scalar GT changes in a five-height tone system (Guébie [gie]; Sande Reference Sande2019).

The empirical richness of GT has engendered a rich literature of explicit theoretical proposals. Grammatical tones have been analysed as qualitatively distinct contrastive units (Kimenyi Reference Kimenyi1978 on Kinyarwanda [kin]), and more recently as quantitatively distinct using gradient strength (a scale from 0.0 to 1.0; Smolensky & Goldrick Reference Smolensky and Goldrick2016; Zimmermann Reference Zimmermann, Gallagher, Gouskova and Yin2018; Kushnir Reference Kushnir2022). Various approaches have also used special configurations of floating tones to produce unique grammatical tone effects, for example, ‘circumfixal’ floating tones in Trommer (Reference Trommer2011). Other approaches postulate no special ‘tones’ in GT patterns, but rather involve some modification of the grammar and rule/constraint set. Examples include the use of Cophonology Theory plus markedness constraints (Inkelas Reference Inkelas, Booij and Marle1998; Sande et al. Reference Sande, Jenks and Inkelas2020), construction constraints with direct reference to syntactic c-command (McPherson Reference McPherson2014) and various forms of transderivational correspondence (e.g. antifaithfulness in Alderete Reference Alderete2001a, Reference Alderete2001b). Still others appeal only to prosodic domains and markedness to account for GT patterns (Rolle & Kari Reference Rolle and Kari2022), adopt a version of MaxEnt involving special operations ‘regularisation’ and ‘scaling’ (Gouskova & Linzen Reference Gouskova and Linzen2015), or simply assume omnipresent suppletive allomorphy (Archangeli & Pulleyblank Reference Archangeli and Pulleyblank2022). Hovering over all GT accounts is the perennial question of why tone (and tone alone) shows its unique set of properties. Certainly, the autosegmental nature of tone is partially responsible (where tones often interact ‘at a distance’), but what precisely can preclude patterns akin to the hypothetical (6) and (7) (inter alia) is not settled.

Despite both our empirical progress and subsequent theoretical sophistication, the above theories are rarely directly compared nor are the underlyingly GT patterns. Part of the motivation for this special issue is that there still remains a wide disconnect between linguistic theory and this grammatical tone literature, especially with respect to interface theories which probe the relationship of phonology to separate syntactic/morphological module(s). This is true across the phonologically oriented literature, for example, Stratal Optimality Theory (Bermúdez-Otero Reference Bermúdez-Otero and Trommer2012, Reference Bermúdez-Otero, Hannahs and Bosch2018; Kiparsky Reference Kiparsky, Hsiao and Wee2015), Constraint Indexation (Pater Reference Pater2000; Jurgec & Bjorkman Reference Jurgec and Bjorkman2018), Match Theory (Selkirk Reference Selkirk2009, Reference Selkirk, Goldsmith, Riggle and Yu2011; Selkirk & Lee Reference Selkirk and Lee2015) and other architectural treatises (e.g. Scheer Reference Scheer2011, Reference Scheer2012). Equally, GT remains under-incorporated in morphosyntactically oriented interface work, for example, Paradigm Function Morphology (Stump Reference Stump2001), Distributed Morphology (Halle & Marantz Reference Halle, Marantz, Hale and Keyser1993), Construction Morphology (Booij Reference Booij2010) and Parallel Architecture (Jackendoff Reference Jackendoff1997).

2. Summary of papers

The seeds of this issue were planted at the 2021 Princeton Phonology Forum (PɸF), whose focus was ‘tone and phonological theory’. Many of the talks at this workshop involved GT, and given the importance of this topic as a whole (as discussed in §1), this naturally led to the present issue. While some of the participants of that workshop have papers here, the two were formally separate and submissions were open to any and all interested parties.

Our issue is inevitably skewed towards Africa, in line with the characterisation that certain tonal regions of the world – in particular Asia – use tone primarily lexically rather than grammatically. Still, this issue involves a wealth of exemplification of GT from several languages families such as Mabia/Gur, Benue-Congo (especially its sub-branch Bantu), Kru and Nilotic in Africa, as well as Otomanguean in Mexico.

The first paper, by Hannah Sande, asks the general question ‘Is grammatical tone item-based or process-based?’ Morphology as item-based vs. process-based has been a long-standing debate in the morphological literature (Hockett Reference Hockett1954; Bermúdez-Otero Reference Bermúdez-Otero and Trommer2012), with certain frameworks, like the aforementioned Distributed Morphology, treating morphological operations as primarily item-based while others, such as Anderson’s (Reference Anderson1992) A-morphous Morphology, argue that morphology is better analysed as process-based.

In her article, Sande draws on case studies from a number of languages and develops a set of diagnostics to determine whether the GT alternations are best analysed as item-based, process-based or suppletive. These diagnostics include whether the GT has a consistent realisation across a paradigm, whether it is phonologically derivable, and whether it follows the general rules of the language’s phonology. If a case of GT meets all three criteria, it is best analysed as item-based; if it fails to meet any of the criteria, it is best analysed as suppletive; and finally, if it is phonologically derivable but is inconsistent in its realisation and outside of the regular bounds of the phonological grammar, then it is best analysed as process-based.

Sande goes on to show that item- and process-based morphology can in fact be unified in a single framework, Cophonologies by Phase (Sande Reference Sande2019; Sande et al. Reference Sande, Jenks and Inkelas2020). However, the distinction between items and processes remains relevant for how this framework captures GT. Item-based GT involves the addition of an underlying phonological form in the vocabulary item, process-based GT involves morpheme-specific constraint ranking and suppletive GT – such as the Chinese tone replacement case discussed in the article – requires lexical listing of allomorphs. While many excellent papers have analysed individual cases of GT, this paper highlights what GT can reveal about the architecture of the morphophonological grammar when diverse case studies are considered together.

Next are four papers focusing on GT patterns in particular languages. Most of the cases of GT involve tonological operations that express a morphosyntactic feature, but tone can interact with morphology in broader ways as well. Arto Anttila and Adams Bodomo’s paper, titled ‘Tone and morphological level ordering in Dagaare’ explores these latter interactions. In Dagaare (dga; Mabia/Gur, Ghana and Burkina Faso), tonal processes such as dissimilation, spreading and tonal absorption are sensitive to morphological level (stem level, word level, postlexical level) as well as syntactic category, and as such fit our working definition of GT as a grammatically restricted tonological pattern.

To expand, cross-linguistically nouns and verbs are subject to different constraint rankings and hence different tonal behaviour (Smith Reference Smith, van Oostendorp, Ewen, Hume and Rice2011). This situation is found in Dagaare. For instance, at the stem level, toneless syllables are filled in by H tone spreading in nouns, but by default L insertion in verbs, e.g. /tuu-ró/ $\to$ [túú-ró] ‘follow-ag’ (noun), but /tuu-ró/ $\to$ [tùù-ró] ‘follow-ipfv’ (verb). The key piece of evidence for level ordering in Dagaare comes from whether and how a sequence of two H tones is repaired. At the stem level, Meeussen’s Rule applies, dissimilating the second H tone to L. In contrast, at the word level, such as in compound formation, a downstep is inserted between the two H tones, and at the postlexical level, HH sequences are left unrepaired.

Anttila and Bodomo’s paper provides an example in which GT requires cyclicity in word formation: at most one downstep is permitted per word, and downstep derived at the stem level by contour tone simplification blocks the derivation of downstep at the word level, a familiar ‘inside-out’ effect. They couch their analysis in Stratal OT (Kiparsky Reference Kiparsky2000), where constraint rankings may differ depending upon stratum but also depending upon syntactic category, as in Cophonology Theory (Anttila Reference Anttila2002; Inkelas & Zoll Reference Inkelas and Zoll2007). In short, the authors demonstrate that ‘once morphology is properly understood phonology turns out to be simple’. GT thus proves to be an important tool in understanding morphology, phonology and the interface between the two components of grammar.

Next is Nadine Grimm’s paper ‘Exponence and the functional load of grammatical tone in Gyeli’. This examines the division of labour between tonal and segmental co-exponents of morphological categories, focusing on the intricate tone system of Gyeli (gyi; Bantu, Cameroon). GT is used almost exclusively in the marking of tense/aspect/mood and polarity on the verb and preverbal markers in Gyeli.

Grimm’s paper makes two main points. First, the more segmental material there is in a morpheme, the weaker the functional load of the tonal co-exponent of that morpheme. In other words, morphemes with both segmental and tonal co-exponents are characterised by one-sided redundancy: the segmental co-exponent would in most cases suffice to distinguish the morpheme (and the tonal information can be seen as redundant), whereas the tonal co-exponent on its own would not be sufficient (i.e. the segmental information cannot be considered redundant). Tone is thus morphologically informative and non-ambiguous only when it is the sole exponent of a morphological category.

The second point of the paper has to do with the relation between grammatical and lexical tones. Gyeli is a language where GT is ‘dominant’, following Rolle’s (Reference Rolle2018) usage of the term – that is, when confronted with a choice between the realisation of grammatical and lexical tones, the language always realises the former at the expense of the latter, which is overwritten. Interestingly, however, when there is room for both in a verb form (i.e. when there are enough tone-bearing units), both are realised. In other words, Gyeli strives to maintain both lexical and grammatical tones, and sacrifices lexical tone only when one of the two must go. GT in Gyeli is thus dominant only ‘under duress’, that is, as a last resort. This case exemplifies GT winning out over a root’s underlying lexical tone when they compete, a common situation in tone systems and one which contradicts the general finding with segmental patterns, in which a root’s phonological features are preserved over those of an affix (Urbanczyk Reference Urbanczyk, van Oostendorp, Ewen, Hume and Rice2011).

Bantu is one of the few families where there are both a critical mass of tone researchers and a substantial existing tone literature. It is therefore not surprising to find another Bantu-focused paper: Larry Hyman and Hildah Kemunto Nyamwaro’s paper on ‘Grammatical tone mapping in Ekegusii’. Indeed, their study here picks up where Bickmore’s (Reference Bickmore1997, Reference Bickmore1999) and Cammenga’s (Reference Cammenga2002) earlier studies on Ekegusii (guz) left off. Hyman & Kemunto Nyamwaro’s paper provides an extremely detailed empirical study of verbal GT, including a state-of-the-art description that works out the many conditioning factors governing the shape of individual GT patterns.

Their paper brings up several important theoretical issues. One is that in order to derive the Ekegusii tone mappings, global access to word-level morphological and phonological information is required. Thus, while the GT patterns themselves target the smaller prosodic stem domain, the conditioning factors need not be contained solely within this constituent. This finding is striking when put alongside other papers in this issue (in particular Yuni Kim’s; see below), which involve a more constrained cyclic model which lacks the power of such global calculations. Rather than viewing this simply as a contradiction, we see it as calling for a more nuanced theory which finely demarcates where we expect globality effects and where cyclicity effects.

Perhaps the most remarkable aspect of Ekegusii is yet another instance of what Rolle & Bickmore (Reference Rolle and Bickmore2022) descriptively refer to as ‘first–last tone harmony’, found infrequently but persistently across Bantu (Meeussen Reference Meeussen1967, Reference Meeussen1971; Nsuka Nkutsi Reference Nsuka Nkutsi1982; Schadeberg Reference Schadeberg1989; Roberts-Kohno Reference Roberts-Kohno2000, Reference Roberts-Kohno2014; Bickmore Reference Bickmore2007, Reference Bickmore2014; Hyman Reference Hyman2012; Jones & Freyer Reference Jones, Freyer, Clem, Jenks and Sande2019). Hyman & Kemunto Nyamwaro show that certain grammatical contexts in Ekegusii show ‘tonal agreement’ between the subject prefix, which appears at the left edge of the word, and the final inflectional vowel at the right edge. For example, in the ‘subject relative clause today past negative (Past1)’, if the subject prefix is lexically high-toned, then a grammatical high tone falls on the last suffix (e.g. ábanto -tá-á-súgum-á ‘people who didn’t push’). In contrast, if this subject prefix happens to be toneless, then the final suffix too must remain toneless and cannot host the grammatical tone (e.g. … o-tá-á-súgúm-a ‘…who didn’t push’). Rolle & Bickmore (Reference Rolle and Bickmore2022) interpret such globally sensitive patterns as suppletive allomorphy, and articulate the ramifications of this for linguistic theory with respect to morphological locality and directionality. Regardless of interpretation, non-local dependencies like this should be of major interest to linguists of several stripes (including the computationally minded, for whom first–last phonological harmony is said to be outside of what is possible in phonology; see Heinz & Idsardi Reference Heinz and Idsardi2013; Lai Reference Lai2015; Jardine Reference Jardine2020).

Lastly, Yuni Kim’s paper ‘Grammatical and lexical sources of allomorphy in Amuzgo inflectional tone’ takes us outside of Africa to examine the complex GT patterns in the Mexican language Amuzgo (azg). Amuzgo is part of the Otomanguean family, known for having some of the most complicated systems of inflectional tone (Palancar & Léonard Reference Palancar and Léonard2016), made especially challenging by the often large number of inflection classes whose membership appears to be mostly unpredictable. Amuzgo is no exception. Kim shows that transitive verbs in Amuzgo belong to up to 21 arbitrary inflection classes, defined by the distribution of replacive GT patterns in 1sg and 2sg forms. However, this irregularity levels out in derived causatives, where inflectional tone patterns become predictable based on the lexical tone of the verb root.

Kim accounts for this discrepancy in behaviour by appealing to cyclic spell-out: morphosyntax spells out in chunks when a phase head is reached, and morpho-lexical features are checked off in the process, so that they are no longer active or visible at later cycles (Bobaljik Reference Bobaljik2000). Causatives contain a Voice head, which triggers spell-out, and so by the time person inflection is reached, the verb’s inflection class features are no longer active; thus, allomorphy can be sensitive to phonological form alone. Non-causatives, on the other hand, contain no such phase head, and so tone marking person can be sensitive to inflection class. Kim’s work thus adds to the growing number of case studies in which GT provides crucial evidence for a cyclic model of morphosyntax (see also McPherson & Heath Reference McPherson and Heath2016; McPherson Reference McPherson2019; Sande Reference Sande2019; Sande et al. Reference Sande, Jenks and Inkelas2020).

Kim analyses the replacive nature of Amuzgo GT with a combination of allomorph listing and cophonologies. Essentially, every input contains both the root’s and any affix’s lexical tones, along with a list of candidate allomorphs with each of Amuzgo’s five possible inflectional tones. These allomorphs are listed in a preferred order, with a constraint Priority assigning an increasing number of violations the farther down the priority list a particular allomorph is. Slightly different constraint sets for 1sg and 2sg inflection account for tonal differences between these two forms. Viewed in terms of Sande’s diagnostics, Amuzgo GT appears to be suppletive. It is not phonologically consistent across the paradigm; it is not easily phonologically derivable (as Kim shows in her §4.2); and it does not follow the general phonology of the language. Nevertheless, the choice between listed suppletive allomorphs can be phonologically conditioned. Thus, Kim’s analysis of Amuzgo GT elucidates not only the larger architecture of morphosyntax but also the details of how phonology and morphology interact to decide upon surface allomorphs.

3. Conclusion

We sought to produce an issue dedicated to the importance of grammatical tone for wider linguistic theory, and phonological inquiry in particular. In this sense, the papers collected here succeed by bearing on a number of ongoing theory-driven debates in the literature, such as the role of storage vs. computation in deriving surface patterns, local vs. global access to neighbouring phonological content and the fine timing of morpho-phonological processes in a stratal organisation.

At the same time, we must acknowledge that at least half the world’s languages are tonal (Yip Reference Yip2002), with a huge number of them in the least-documented areas of the world (Hammarström Reference Hammarström2010, Reference Hammarström2014). Although Africa (particularly Bantu languages) and Meso-America are relatively well-represented in the literature on grammatical tone (and, unsurprisingly, in the present collection), tonal languages from other areas or language families where GT has been identified (e.g. in New Guinea, North and South America, Pakistan or New Caledonia) ought to be included in discussions of grammatical tone as well. The task of bringing together grammatical tone and linguistic theory is enormous, and requires broader coverage, both empirical and theoretical, of the many tonal systems that are still awaiting analysis and often even basic description. In the end, we firmly believe that linguistic theory – and especially the interrelations of phonology, morphology and syntax – still has a huge amount to gain by specifically engaging with grammatical tone. This special issue, however limited in scope, is one step in this direction.

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank all the reviewers of this thematic issue who made our jobs so much easier. Special thanks are also due to the editors Colin Ewen, Daniel Currie Hall, Ellen Kaisse and Yoonjung Kang, who provided invaluable guidance and assistance along the way, and graciously accommodated our ever-changing timeline. All errors in this introduction remain that of the guest editors.

Author contributions

The three author names are listed in alphabetical order.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

References

Alderete, John (2001a). Dominance effects as transderivational anti-faithfulness. Phonology 18. 201253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alderete, John (2001b). Morphologically governed accent in Optimality Theory. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Alderete, John, Chan, Queenie & Tanaka, Shin-ichi (2022). The morphology of Cantonese ‘changed tone’: extensions and limitations. Gengo Kenkyu 161. 139169.Google Scholar
Allott, Anna J. (1967). Grammatical tone in modern spoken Burmese. Wissenschaftliche Zeitschrift der Karl-Marx-Universität Leipzig: Gesellschafts- und Sprachwissenschaftliche Reihe 16. 157161.Google Scholar
Andersen, Torben (1995). Morphological stratification in Dinka: on the alternations of voice quality, vowel length, and tone in the morphology of transitive verbal roots in a monosyllabic language. Studies in African Linguistics 23. 163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, Stephen R. (1992). A-morphous morphology, number 62 in Cambridge Studies in Linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andrzejewski, Bogumił W. (1955). The problem of vowel representation in the Isaaq dialect of Somali. Bulletin of the School of Oriental and African Studies 17. 568580.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anttila, Arto (2002). Morphologically conditioned phonological alternations. NLLT 20. 142.Google Scholar
Archangeli, Diana & Pulleyblank, Douglas (2022). Emergent phonology, number 7 in Conceptual Foundations of Language Science. Berlin: Language Science Press.Google Scholar
Beckman, Jill N. (1998). Positional faithfulness. PhD dissertation, University of Massachusetts Amherst.Google Scholar
Bermúdez-Otero, Ricardo (2012). The architecture of grammar and the division of labour in exponence. In Trommer, Jochen (ed.) The morphology and phonology of exponence. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 883.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bermúdez-Otero, Ricardo (2018). Stratal phonology. In Hannahs, S. J. & Bosch, Anna R. K. (eds.) The Routledge handbook of phonological theory. London: Routledge. 100134.Google Scholar
Bickmore, Lee (1997). Problems in constraining high tone spread in Ekegusii. Lingua 102. 265290.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bickmore, Lee (1999). High tone spread in Ekegusii revisited: an Optimality Theoretic account. Lingua 109. 109153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bickmore, Lee (2007). Cilungu phonology. Stanford, CA: CSLI.Google Scholar
Bickmore, Lee (2014). Cilungu tone melodies: a descriptive and comparative study. Africana Linguistica 20. 3962.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bobaljik, Jonathan (2000). The ins and outs of contextual allomorphy. University of Maryland Working Papers in Linguistics 10. 3571.Google Scholar
Booij, Geert (2010). Construction morphology. Language and Linguistics Compass 4. 543555.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cammenga, Jelle (2002). Phonology and morphology of Ekegusii: a Bantu language of Kenya. Cologne: Rüdiger Köppe.Google Scholar
Cammenga, Jelle (2004). Igikuria phonology and morphology: a Bantu language of South-West Kenya and North-West Tanzania. Cologne: Rüdiger Köppe.Google Scholar
Cammenga, Jillert (1994). Kuria phonology and morphology. PhD dissertation, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam.Google Scholar
Campbell, Eric William (2014). Aspects of the phonology and morphology of Zenzontepec Chatino, a Zapotecan language of Oaxaca, Mexico. PhD dissertation, University of Texas at Austin.Google Scholar
Chen, Matthew Y. (1987). The syntax of Xiamen tone sandhi. Phonology Yearbook 4. 109149.Google Scholar
Efere, Emmanuel Efereala (2001). The pitch system of the Bumo dialect of Izon. UBC Working Papers in Linguistics 4. 115259.Google Scholar
Goldsmith, John (1976). Autosegmental phonology. PhD dissertation, Massachusetts Institute of Technology.Google Scholar
Goldsmith, John A. (1990). Autosegmental and metrical phonology. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Gouskova, Maria & Linzen, Tal (2015). Morphological conditioning of phonological regularization. The Linguistic Review 32. 427473.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graf, Thomas (2017). The power of locality domains in phonology. Phonology 34. 385405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, Christopher R. (2018). A survey of word-level replacive tonal patterns in Western Mande. Mandenkan 59. 67108.Google Scholar
Halle, Morris & Marantz, Alec (1993). Distributed morphology and the pieces of inflection. In Hale, Kenneth & Keyser, Samuel Jay (eds.) The view from Building 20: essays in linguistics in honor of Sylvain Bromberger. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. 111176.Google Scholar
Hammarström, Harald (2010). The status of the least documented language families in the world. Language Documentation & Conservation 4. 177212.Google Scholar
Hammarström, Harald (2014). The status of description of the world’s languages. Ms, Radboud Universiteit Nijmegen.Google Scholar
Harry, Otelemate G. (2004). Aspects of the tonal system of Kalaḅarị-Ịjọ. Stanford, CA: CSLI.Google Scholar
Harry, Otelemate G. & Hyman, Larry M. (2014). Phrasal construction tonology: the case of Kalabari. Studies in Language 38. 649689.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes, Bruce (1995). Metrical stress theory: principles and case studies. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Heath, Jeffrey (2008). A grammar of Jamsay. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heinz, Jeffrey & Idsardi, William (2013). What complexity differences reveal about domains in language. Topics in Cognitive Science 5. 111131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hockett, Charles F. (1954). Two models of grammatical description. Word 10. 210234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hyman, Larry M. (2012). Post-verbal subject in the Nzadi relative clause. Journal of African Languages and Linguistics 33. 97117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hyman, Larry M. (2018). What tone teaches us about language. Lg 94. 698709.Google Scholar
Hyman, Larry M. & Olawsky, Knut J. (2003). Dagbani verb tonology. In Githiora, Chege, Littlefield, Heather & Manfredi, Victor (eds.) Kinyĩra njĩra! Step firmly on the pathway! trends in African linguistics 5. Lawrenceville, NJ: Africa World Press. 97108.Google Scholar
Inkelas, Sharon (1998). The theoretical status of morphologically conditioned phonology: a case study from dominance. In Booij, Geert & Van Marle, Jaap (eds.) Yearbook of morphology 1997. Amsterdam: Springer. 121155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Inkelas, Sharon & Zoll, Cheryl (2007). Is grammar dependence real? A comparison between cophonological and indexed constraint approaches to morphologically conditioned phonology. Linguistics 45. 133171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Isac, Daniela & Reiss, Charles (2008). I-language: an introduction to linguistics as cognitive science. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, Ray (1997). The architecture of the language faculty, number 26 in Linguistic Inquiry Monographs. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Jardine, Adam (2020). Melody learning and long-distance phonotactics in tone. NLLT 38. 11451195.Google Scholar
Jones, Patrick & Freyer, Jake (2019). Reconsidering tone and melodies in Kikamba. In Clem, Emily, Jenks, Peter & Sande, Hannah (eds.) Theory and description in African linguistics: selected papers from the 47th annual Conference on African Linguistics. Berlin: Language Science Press. 177198.Google Scholar
Jurgec, Peter & Bjorkman, Bronwyn M. (2018). Indexation to stems and words. Phonology 35. 577615.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaisse, Ellen M. (2019). What kinds of processes are postlexical? And how powerful are they? In Zhang, Hongming & Qian, Youyong (eds.) Prosodic studies: challenges and prospects. London: Routledge. 227251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kawahara, Shigeto & Kumagai, Gakuji (2023). Lyman’s Law can count only up to two. Laboratory Phonology 14. 127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kawahara, Shigeto & Wolf, Matthew (2010). On the existence of root-initial-accenting suffixes: an elicitation study of Japanese [-zu]. Linguistics 48. 837864.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kenstowicz, Michael J. (1994). Phonology in generative grammar. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Kimenyi, Alexandre (1978). Grammatical tone neutralization in Kinyarwanda. Studies in African Linguistics 9. 303317.Google Scholar
Kiparsky, Paul (2000). Opacity and cyclicity. The Linguistic Review 17. 351366.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kiparsky, Paul (2015). Stratal OT: a synopsis and FAQs. In Hsiao, Yuchau E. & Wee, Lian-Hee (eds.) Capturing phonological shades within and across languages. Newcastle: Cambridge Scholars Publishing. 244.Google Scholar
Konoshenko, Maria (2018). Word-level replacive tonal patterns in Mande nominal constructions: on Christopher Green’s binary typology. Mandenkan 59. 109120.Google Scholar
Kouneli, Maria & Nie, Yining (2021). Across-the-board tonal polarity in Kipsigis: implications for the morphology–phonology interface. Lg 97. e111e138.Google Scholar
Kraal, Pieter J. (2005). A grammar of Makonde (Chinnima, Tanzania). PhD dissertation, Universiteit Leiden.Google Scholar
Kubozono, Haruo & Giriko, Mikio (eds.) (2018). Tonal change and neutralization, number 27 in Phonology and Phonetics. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kushnir, Yuriy (2022). Accent strength in Lithuanian: evidence from the Saussurean accent shift. Glossa 7. 47 pp.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lai, Regine (2015). Learnable vs. unlearnable harmony patterns. LI 46. 425451.Google Scholar
Maniacky, Jacky & Ambouroue, Odette (2014). Melodic tones in Orungu (Bantu B11b). Africana Linguistica 20. 243261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marlo, Michael R., Mwita, Leonard Chacha & Paster, Mary (2014). Kuria tone melodies. Africana Linguistica 20. 277294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marlo, Michael R., Mwita, Leonard Chacha & Paster, Mary (2015). Problems in Kuria H tone assignment. NLLT 33. 251265.Google Scholar
Matsumori, Akiko (2019). Prosodic unit, recursive structure, and nature of accent in Miyako Ryukyuan. The Linguistic Review 36. 5183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCarthy, John J. & Prince, Alan (1995). Faithfulness and reduplicative identity. University of Massachusetts Occasional Papers in Linguistics 18. 249384.Google Scholar
McCarthy, John J. & Prince, Alan S. (1986). Prosodic morphology. Ms, University of Massachusetts and Rutgers University. Available online at https://ruccs.rutgers.edu/images/tech_rpt/tr-32/pm86all.pdf.Google Scholar
McIntosh, Justin Daniel (2015). Aspects of phonology and morphology of Teotepec Eastern Chatino. PhD dissertation, University of Texas at Austin.Google Scholar
McPherson, Laura (2014). Replacive grammatical tone in the Dogon languages. PhD dissertation, University of California, Los Angeles.Google Scholar
McPherson, Laura (2017). Multiple feature affixation in Seenku plural formation. Morphology 27. 217252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McPherson, Laura (2019). Seenku argument-head tone Sandhi: allomorph selection in a cyclic grammar. Glossa 4. 41 pp.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McPherson, Laura & Heath, Jeffrey (2016). Phrasal grammatical tone in the Dogon languages. NLLT 32. 593639.Google Scholar
Meeussen, A.E. (1967). Bantu grammatical reconstructions. Africana Linguistica 3. 79121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meeussen, A.E. (1971). Relative clauses in Bantu. Studies in African Linguistics Supplement 2. 310.Google Scholar
Mwita, Leonard Chacha (2008). Verbal tone in Kuria. PhD dissertation, University of California, Los Angeles.Google Scholar
Nsuka Nkutsi, F. (1982). Les structures fondamentales du relatif dans les langues bantoues. Tervuren: Musée Royal de l’Afrique Centrale.Google Scholar
Odden, David (1987). Predicting tone in Kikuria. In Odden, David (ed.) Current approaches to African linguistics, volume 4. Dordrecht: Foris. 311326.Google Scholar
Odden, David & Bickmore, Lee (2014). Melodic tone in Bantu: overview. Africana Linguistica 20. 314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Palancar, Enrique L. & Léonard, Jean Léo (eds.) (2016). Tone and inflection: new facts and new perspectives. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paster, Mary (2019). Phonology counts. Radical: A Journal of Phonology 1. 161.Google Scholar
Pater, Joe (2000). Non-uniformity in English secondary stress: the role of ranked and lexically specific constraints. Phonology 17. 237274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, David (2016). Word tone and tonal overwriting in Kabiye nouns. Journal of African Languages and Linguistics 37. 131160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts-Kohno, Rosalind Ruth (2000). Kikamba phonology and morphology. PhD dissertation, Ohio State University.Google Scholar
Roberts-Kohno, Ruth (2014). Melodic tones in Kikamba. Africana Linguistica 20. 367384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rolle, Nicholas (2018). Grammatical tone: typology and theory. PhD dissertation, University of California, Berkeley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rolle, Nicholas (2021). Lexical tone contrast in Izon as ubiquitous floating tone. Phonological Data and Analysis 3. 140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rolle, Nicholas & Bickmore, Lee (2022). Outward-sensitive phonologically conditioned suppletive allomorphy vs. first–last tone harmony in Cilungu. Morphology 32. 197247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rolle, Nicholas & Kari, Ethelbert Emmanuel (2022). Tone and prosodic recursion in Degema nouns and noun phrases. Journal of African Languages and Linguistics 43. 249283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rolle, Nicholas & Lionnet, Florian (2020). Phantom structure: a representational account of floating tone association. In Baek, Hyunah, Takahashi, Chikako & Yeung, Alex Hong-Lun (eds.) Proceedings of the 2019 Annual Meeting on Phonology. Washington, DC: Linguistic Society of America. 10 pp.Google Scholar
Sande, Hannah (2019). A unified account of conditioned phonological alternations: evidence from Guébie. Lg 95. 456497.Google Scholar
Sande, Hannah, Jenks, Peter & Inkelas, Sharon (2020). Cophonologies by ph(r)ase. NLLT 38. 11211261.Google Scholar
Schadeberg, Thilo C. (1989). Tone and history of Nyamwezi verb forms with complex final tones. Afrika und Übersee 72. 3342.Google Scholar
Scheer, Tobias (2011). A guide to morphosyntax–phonology interface theories: how extra-phonological information is treated in phonology since Trubetzkoy’s Grenzsignale. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Scheer, Tobias (2012). Direct interface and one-channel translation: a non-diacritic theory of the morphosyntax–phonology interface. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Selkirk, Elisabeth (2009). On clause and intonational phrase in Japanese: the syntactic grounding of prosodic constituent structure. Gengo Kenkyu 136. 3573.Google Scholar
Selkirk, Elisabeth (2011). The syntax–phonology interface. In Goldsmith, John, Riggle, Jason & Yu, Alan C. L. (eds.) The handbook of phonological theory. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell. 435483.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Selkirk, Elisabeth & Lee, Seunghun J. (2015). Constituency in sentence phonology: an introduction. Phonology 32. 118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, Jennifer L. (2011). Category-specific effects. In van Oostendorp, Marc, Ewen, Colin J., Hume, Elizabeth & Rice, Keren (eds.) The Blackwell companion to phonology, volume 4. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell. 24392463.Google Scholar
Smith, Neil & Tsimpli, Ianthi-Maria (1995). The mind of a savant: language learning and modularity. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Smolensky, Paul & Goldrick, Matthew (2016). Gradient symbolic representations in grammar: the case of French liaison. Ms, Rutgers University. ROA #1286.Google Scholar
Stump, Gregory T. (2001). Inflectional morphology: a theory of paradigm structure. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sullivant, John Ryan (2015). The phonology and inflectional morphology of Cháknyá, Tataltepec de Valdés Chatino, a Zapotecan language. PhD dissertation, University of Texas at Austin.Google Scholar
Tian, Mimi (2018). Anatomy of a grammatical tone: the case of ‘induced creaky tone’ in Burmese. Linguistics of the Tibeto-Burman Area 41. 192218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trommer, Jochen (2011). Phonological aspects of Western Nilotic mutation morphology. PhD dissertation, Universität Leipzig.Google Scholar
Trommer, Jochen (in press). The stratal structure of Kuria morphological tone. To appear in Phonology 39.Google Scholar
Urbanczyk, Suzanne (2011). Root–affix asymmetries. In van Oostendorp, Marc, Ewen, Colin J., Hume, Elizabeth & Rice, Keren (eds.) The Blackwell companion to phonology, volume 4. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell. 24902515.Google Scholar
Villard, Stéphanie (2015). The phonology and morphology of Zacatepec Eastern Chatino. PhD dissertation, University of Texas at Austin.Google Scholar
Watters, Stephen A. (2018). A grammar of Dzongkha (dzo): phonology, words, and simple clauses. PhD dissertation, Rice University.Google Scholar
Yip, Moira J. (2002). Tone. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zimmermann, Eva (2018). Being exceptional is being weak: tonal exceptions in San Miguel el Grande Mixtec. In Gallagher, Gillian, Gouskova, Maria & Yin, Sora Heng (eds.) Proceedings of the 2017 Annual Meeting on Phonology. Washington, DC: Linguistic Society of America. 12 pp.Google Scholar