Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-7cvxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-18T00:06:05.443Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The effect of acute stress response on conspiracy theory beliefs

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 December 2024

Vojtech Pisl*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine in Pilsen, Charles University, Pilsen, Czech Republic Department of Psychiatry, Institute for Postgraduate Medical Education, Prague, Czech Republic
Turkay Nefes
Affiliation:
Institute for Public Goods and Policies, Spanish National Research Council, Madrid, Spain Siyavuspasa Mah Camlik Cad Cigdem Sokak 7, Apartment 2, TR-34182, Istanbul, Turkey
Benjamin Simsa
Affiliation:
Institute of Social Sciences of the Centre of Social and Psychological Sciences SAS, Košice, Slovakia Department of Psychology, Faculty of Arts, P. J. Šafárik University in Košice, Slovakia
Daniela Kestlerova
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine in Pilsen, Charles University, Pilsen, Czech Republic
Pavel Kubíček
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine in Pilsen, Charles University, Pilsen, Czech Republic
Vojtech Linka
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine in Pilsen, Charles University, Pilsen, Czech Republic
Tatana Martynova
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine in Pilsen, Charles University, Pilsen, Czech Republic
Rachel Sajdlova
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine in Pilsen, Charles University, Pilsen, Czech Republic
David Sejrek
Affiliation:
Grammar School of František Křižík, Czech Republic
Jan Vevera
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine in Pilsen, Charles University, Pilsen, Czech Republic Department of Psychiatry, Institute for Postgraduate Medical Education, Prague, Czech Republic
*
Corresponding author: Vojtech Pisl; Email: pisl@mail.muni.cz

Abstract

The endorsement of conspiracy theories may be increased by subjectively perceived stress. Yet, it is not known whether this correlation is caused by the effects of the acute stress reaction on the brain or other psychological, social, or methodological factors. The effect of an experimentally induced acute stress reaction on conspiracy thinking was tested on a sample (n = 115) of students of medicine. Although the stress procedure caused a substantial increase in salivary cortisol, there was no significant effect on endorsing conspiracy theories or adopting conspiracy interpretations of novel information. The results confirmed no effect of the acute stress reaction on conspiracy thinking, suggesting it may be absent or weaker than expected. The study demonstrated the viability of psychophysiological experimental design in conspiracy research and may inspire further examination of the physiological mechanisms underlying susceptibility to conspiracy theories.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Association for Politics and the Life Sciences

Introduction

Conspiracy theories (CTs) wield significant influence across diverse domains encompassing health, politics, and the workplace (Douglas et al., Reference Douglas, Sutton, Jolley, Wood, Bilewicz, Cichocka and Soral2015). Believing in COVID-19-related CTs is associated with diminished apprehension of the pandemic’s severity, reduced adoption of preventive measures like mask-wearing, and decreased intent to undergo vaccination within the United States (Romer & Jamieson, Reference Romer and Jamieson2020). In their analysis across 17 European countries, Syropoulos and Gkinopoulos (Reference Syropoulos and Gkinopoulos2023) found that lower levels of institutional trust and higher levels of CT beliefs predict vaccine hesitancy even after accounting for demographic variables. Given the profound significance of CTs on public health, social cohesion, and politics, it is crucial to understand all factors that contribute to their prevalence. By uncovering these underlying mechanisms, we can better address the spread of CTs, mitigate their harmful effects, and promote more informed decision-making in society.

The scholarly examination of CTs generally falls into two distinct approaches (Nefes, Reference Nefes2015, Reference Nefes2017). The first approach emphasizes the irrational nature of CTs, viewing them as belief systems that distort reality rather than providing accurate explanations of events. This perspective associates CTs with the dissemination of disinformation by fringe and extremist groups, implying a pathological aspect to these beliefs (e.g.,(Hofstadter, Reference Hofstadter1965; Robins & Post, Reference Robins and Post1997)). Conversely, a more modern perspective regards CTs as rational attempts by individuals to explore alternative explanations and make sense of complex realities (e.g.,(Gray, Reference Gray2010; Knight, Reference Knight2000)). While various studies demonstrate many factors relating to the beliefs in CTs all around the world, there is a gap of research in the scholarship with regards to exploring the physiological mechanisms that may underlie heightened susceptibility to conspiracy theory beliefs.

Our study contributes to fill a critical gap in the literature by investigating, for the first time, the influence of cortisol on individuals’ tendencies to believe in CTs. The paper begins by highlighting the theoretical gaps in our understanding of the physiological mechanisms that may affect conspiracy beliefs. It then details the methods, experimental procedure, and key findings. Finally, the paper concludes with a discussion of the results and their broader implications.

Physiological mechanisms associated with conspiracy theory beliefs

During periods of heightened stress, individuals may exhibit a proclivity for embracing CTs (Douglas et al., Reference Douglas, Uscinski, Sutton, Cichocka, Nefes, Ang and Deravi2019; Lantian et al., Reference Lantian, Muller, Nurra and Douglas2017; J. W. Van Prooijen, Reference Van Prooijen2019). Numerous CTs emerge during times of crisis (Nefes, Reference Nefes2014). Moreover, experiencing elevated perceived stress levels correlated with the belief in CTs among adults in the United States (Swami et al., Reference Swami, Furnham, Smyth, Weis, Lay and Clow2016) and in Italy (Simione et al., Reference Simione, Vagni, Gnagnarella, Bersani and Pajardi2021). The belief in CTs may be influenced by stress, and this effect could arise from the impact of biological stress on the brain. For instance, the influence of cortisol on hippocampal activity could affect memory processes related to conspiracy explanations (Duch, Reference Duch2021; Hermans et al., Reference Hermans, Henckens, Joëls and Fernández2014) or the propensity to less complex learning and reasoning mechanisms under stress might be involved (Moravec et al., Reference Moravec, Minas and Dennis2018; Schwabe, Reference Schwabe2017). Nevertheless, the correlation between perceived psychological stress and conspiracy beliefs does not automatically imply a causal effect of cortisol on conspiracy thinking. First, the relationship between the self-reported life stress and the physiological stress reaction indexed by cortisol levels is weak (Halford et al., Reference Halford, Jonsdottir and Eek2012; Lazarides et al., Reference Lazarides, Ward, Buss, Chen, Voelkle, Gillen, Wadhwa and Entringer2020). Second, Swami et al. (Reference Swami, Furnham, Smyth, Weis, Lay and Clow2016) studied the link between the stress perceived in the last month and the endorsement of conspiracy theories. Yet, the endorsement of popular CTs used in the study may have reflected stable opinions developed months or years before, independently of the stresses of the last month. Thus, factors such as personality traits, sociodemographic status, social groups, and political beliefs could simultaneously affect the propensity to perceive own life as stressful and conspiracy beliefs, inflating the observed correlation between these two constructs. Indeed, conspiracy thinking correlates with trait rather than state anxiety (Krüppel et al., Reference Krüppel, Yoon and Mokros2023), linking endorsement of CTs to individual rather than external factors. Third, the correlation between self-reported stress and CTs might be caused by situational and response factors affecting responses to stress-related as well as conspiracy-related surveys. For example, the momentary emotional status of the respondents or their response styles might simultaneously affect their responses to different survey scales, resulting in inflated or spurious correlations (Podsakoff et al., Reference Podsakoff, MacKenzie, Lee and Podsakoff2003; Tourangeau et al., Reference Tourangeau, Rips and Rasinski2000). Accordingly, the scholarship could benefit from empirical studies that examine the relationship between stress and conspiracy theory beliefs.

Our research addresses this gap in the scholarship by exploring how acute stress responses influence conspiracy beliefs. In our experimental study, we utilize the Maastricht Acute Stress Procedure (MAST) to induce biological stress, characterized by elevated cortisol levels. Aligned with prior academic literature on CTs (Duch, Reference Duch2021; Swami et al., Reference Swami, Furnham, Smyth, Weis, Lay and Clow2016; J.-W. van Prooijen et al., Reference van Prooijen, Douglas, Jan-Willem van Prooijen and Douglas2018), we expect that inducing stress will heighten reported agreement with conspiracy theory statements. Additionally, we aim to differentiate whether acute stress levels impact the expression of agreement with existing CTs or the adoption of conspiracy explanations in response to novel information. This distinction mirrors Swami et al.’s (Reference Swami, Coles, Stieger, Pietschnig, Furnham, Rehim and Voracek2011) categorization of real-world and fictitious CTs, where the former pertains to circulated conspiracy accounts and the latter involves fictional theories created by researchers. For assessing agreement with the former, we present items summarizing existing CTs, such as those related to the Russia-Ukraine War. To examine the adoption of novel conspiracy explanations, we introduce brief fictional narratives describing an event with two alternative explanations, one of which is conspiratorial. In so doing, we investigate whether acute stress is a factor that increases the likelihood of adopting a conspiracy explanation when encountering a new situation (Duch, Reference Duch2021). Our hypotheses are as follows:

  1. 1. Acute stress reactions will amplify individuals’ inclination to express agreement with CTs.

  2. 2. Acute stress reactions will heighten individuals’ inclination to favor a conspiracy explanation when interpreting a new situation.

Methods

Stress induction and cortisol analysis

The MAST is a standardized procedure for experimental initiation of stress reactions in humans, including the experimental and control variants. Experimental MAST combines physical and psychosocial stressors as both are processed differently in the brain and was confirmed to be efficient in eliciting subjective, autonomic, as well as glucocorticoid responses to stress (Smeets et al., Reference Smeets, Cornelisse, Quaedflieg, Meyer, Jelicic and Merckelbach2012). In this procedure, participants face an unpredictable situation, alternating between sinking their hand into cold water (physical stressor) and a counting task requiring them to quickly subtract 17 from 4-digit numbers with harsh, critical supervision and negative feedback while being allegedly recorded on video (social stressors). The control procedure replicates the stress variant without causing stress: lukewarm water is used instead of the cold one, an uncomplicated counting task is carried out without negative feedback, no video recording is simulated, and polite supervision is offered. To validate the MAST procedure, salivary cortisol and blood pressure and pulse will be measured repeatedly before and during the experiment. Salivary samples will be collected using Salivette tubes. Salivary cortisol, a well-established stress biomarker (Hellhammer et al., Reference Hellhammer, Wüst and Kudielka2009; Strahler et al., Reference Strahler, Skoluda, Kappert and Nater2017), will be analyzed using ELISA in a commercial laboratory.

Experimental procedure

Prior to the experiment, participants were instructed to refrain from eating, smoking, and consuming anything except water for at least 2 hours before the study to minimize external influences on cortisol levels. Further, they were asked to avoid caffeine intake, heavy exercise, and substantial stressors on the day of testing. To minimize the effect of circadian fluctuations of cortisol (Hofstra & de Weerd, Reference Hofstra and de Weerd2008), the testing will be done during afternoon hours (1 pm–6 pm).

Upon arrival, the participants were encouraged to drink a glass of water to aid saliva secretion. They received an explanation of the study and an informed consent form, followed by the baseline (t0) cortisol and blood pressure measure. Then, the participants were pseudo-randomly assigned to the experimental or control group and underwent the MAST procedure in one of two separate rooms. Following the MAST procedure and blood pressure measurement (t1), they moved to another room, where cortisol and blood pressure measures were taken (t2). Consequently, during the expected peak of cortisol levels 5–40 minutes after finishing the MAST (Quaedflieg et al., Reference Quaedflieg, Schwabe, Meyer and Smeets2013, Reference Quaedflieg, Meyer, van Ruitenbeek and Smeets2017, Reference Quaedflieg, Stoffregen and Ashton2022; Smeets et al., Reference Smeets, Cornelisse, Quaedflieg, Meyer, Jelicic and Merckelbach2012), the novel conspiracy explanations survey was presented, followed by the conspiracy items survey. After their completion, the last measures of cortisol and blood pressure were taken approximately 30 minutes after the end of the MAST procedure (t3). The participants were debriefed and encouraged to ask questions. The study was approved by the Ethical Committee of University Hospital Pilsen and Faculty of Medicine in Pilsen, Charles University (ref. 49/23).

Sample

In total, 143 general medicine students from Charles University, 20–25 years old, participated in a “psychophysiological study” for their psychology class credit. Data from 7 participants were excluded due to missing or insufficient saliva samples (see Data collection section in Appendix for details). Of the sample with admissible saliva samples (n = 136; 73 in the intervention group), 15 were excluded due to insufficient cortisol response (as dictated by the design, all from the intervention group) and 6 for lack of consistency in their answers (5 in the intervention group). Moreover, 2 participants were excluded from the Conspiracy Survey due to multiple missing items. Hence, the analyses were conducted on a sample of 115 (53 in the intervention group; 75 females) and reduced to n = 113 (51 in the intervention group, 73 females) for the conspiracy survey. No personal details other than sex and those necessary to apply inclusion and exclusion criteria were collected.

The sample size was determined to be 58 per group after applying all exclusion criteria via apriori power analysis with a requirement to detect an effect size corresponding to 7% of the variance in the conspiracy beliefs being explained by stress with 90% statistical power. The statistical power was confirmed by retrospective sensitivity analysis using G*Power 3 (Faul et al., Reference Faul, Erdfelder, Lang and Buchner2007) (see Power Analysis in the Appendix for details). Because the study induces stress experimentally in a relatively homogeneous sample and measures the conspiracy beliefs in a standardized laboratory setting, it magnifies the effects of stress compared to its effects in daily life. Thus, it is plausible to expect that any smaller effect would have negligible impact on interindividual differences in endorsing CTs outside the laboratory.

Exclusion criteria

To prevent medical factors affecting cortisol levels, exclusion criteria inspired by general recommendations and previous studies (Hellhammer et al., Reference Hellhammer, Wüst and Kudielka2009; Kuhlmann & Wolf, Reference Kuhlmann and Wolf2005; Quaedflieg et al., Reference Quaedflieg, Stoffregen and Ashton2022; Strahler et al., Reference Strahler, Skoluda, Kappert and Nater2017) were applied: pregnancy, hormonal contraception, prescription medication or illegal drugs or acute illness in the week before testing, average alcohol intake above 20 glasses per week, regular smoking. Moreover, those substantially departing from the pre-experimental requirements (caffeine intake in the last 6 hrs, sugar intake or heavy exercise in the last 2 hrs etc) were excluded. Additional exclusion criteria were based upon the results of the participants. First, from the experimental group, those who do not show a satisfactory increase in cortisol level following the MAST procedure will be excluded. Specifically, the difference between the baseline (t0) cortisol level and cortisol levels in any of the two later samples (t2 or t3) will need to reach at least 1.5 nmol/l (Miller et al., Reference Miller, Plessow, Kirschbaum and Stalder2013) for the participant to be included in the experimental group. Second, those not passing the attention check in the questionnaires were excluded (see Psychometric tools; missing responses necessary for the attention check were ignored).

Psychometric tools

The conspiracy survey comprises single-sentence conspiracy statements drawn from prior studies (Brotherton et al., Reference Brotherton, French and Pickering2013; Enders et al., Reference Enders, Uscinski, Klofstad, Seelig, Wuchty, Murthi, Premaratne, Funchion and Keefer2021; Stojanov & Halberstadt, Reference Stojanov and Halberstadt2019) and covering various topics excluding medical CTs (see the Appendix I for the list of items). Participants rated these statements on a 6-point Likert scale ranging from “strongly disagree” through “disagree” and “rather disagree” to “strongly agree.” The mean response was used after we established that the in-sample internal consistency was sufficient (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.95).

The novel conspiracy explanations are paragraph-long narratives invented by the authors based on stories used in existing literature (Imhoff & Lamberty, Reference Imhoff and Lamberty2017; Lantian et al., Reference Lantian, Muller, Nurra and Douglas2017). Each story presents an open conspiracy theory (Räikkä, Reference Räikkä2018): a fictional controversy with a mainstream and a conspiracy explanation, satisfying the conflict and the conspiracy criterion. Each story is complemented with three items probing how likely different aspects of the conspiracy explanation are according to the participant, acknowledging some of the nuances of conspiracy beliefs (Raab et al., Reference Raab, Ortlieb, Auer, Guthmann and Carbon2013). A six-point Likert scale will be used again, ranging from “very unlikely” through “unlikely” to “slightly unlikely” to “very likely.” The mean response was used after we established that the in-sample internal consistency was sufficient (Cronbach’s alpha = .92). The stories and items are presented in the Appendix.

The attention check involved rephrasing three conspiracy survey items and presenting them again. Participants with a mean difference larger than 1 point on a 1–6 Likert scale between the original and the repeated items or discrepancies of more than 2 points in any response were excluded.

Statistical analysis

The analysis was conducted in R version 4.3.0, using the tidyverse framework (RStudio Team, 2022; Wickham et al., Reference Wickham, Averick, Bryan, Chang, McGowan, François, Grolemund, Hayes, Henry, Hester, Kuhn, Pedersen, Miller, Bache, Müller, Ooms, Robinson, Seidel, Spinu and Yutani2019). The analytic script and the datafile (see Datafile in the Appendix for details) are available along with the preregistered protocol of the study on https://osf.io/37s6a.

Participants lacking cortisol responses or response consistency were excluded from the analysis. A two-way repeated measures ANOVA (time × group) was employed to confirm the cortisol increase following stress induction. We have used the function anova_test from the rstatix package (Kassambara, Reference Kassambara2020), employing a Type III sum of squares. The main analysis involved a one-tailed Welch t-test (Delacre et al., Reference Delacre, Lakens and Leys2017) for each of the two dependent variables (conspiracy survey and novel conspiracy explanations) to examine anticipated differences in scores between the experimental and control groups for conspiracy items and stories. The threshold for statistical significance was set to p = .05. Considering the difference between these two variables (i.e., tapping responses about existing beliefs a vs formation of novel ones), no correction for multiple comparisons was made. Additionally, a post-hoc exploratory analysis was conducted to estimate the size of the possible effect from our data using the cohens_d function of the rstatix.

A preregistered sensitivity analysis was conducted to assess the robustness of our main findings. Specifically, the main analysis was repeated with the dependent variables converted to binary variables to account for potential bias resulting from the extreme response style. A one-tailed Welch t-test was used on items binarized by setting all “agree” or “likely” responses to 1 and all “disagree” or “unlikely” to 0. As a further sensitivity check, the main analysis was repeated using the Mann–Whitney U test to account for the effects of potential outliers.

Results

The conspiracy survey measures had excellent internal consistency (both alphas > .90) and were positively correlated (r = .68; p < .001). Cortisol has increased in the intervention group (F(214,2) = 38.10; p < .001) and differed before (t(85.02) = 5.15; p < .001) and after (t(83.35 = 8.67; p < .001) filling the surveys, but not on baseline (t(82.99) = 1.50; p = .14; see Figure 1). We found a significant effect of the interaction between the group and time on systolic (F(183,3) = 3.47; p < .05) but not diastolic (F(183,3) = .17; p = .92) blood pressure and on pulse (F(183,3) = 4.10; p < .01). The systolic blood pressure was higher in the intervention group immediately after finishing the stress procedure (t(65.89) = 2.62; p < .05), but not on the baseline (t(109.86) = 0.36; p = .72) or at t2 (t(110) = 1.70; p = .09) or at t3 (t(107.91) = 0.72; p = .48). The pulse was faster in the intervention group immediately after finishing the stress procedure (t(60.92) = 3.08; p < .01) and at t3 (t(93.89) = 2.56; p < .05), but not on the baseline (t(97.06) = 1.40; p = .16) or at t4 (t(90.72) = 1.07; p = .29). The mean time difference from finishing the MAST procedure (t1) was 4.85 minutes to the second cortisol measurement before filling out the surveys (t2) and 28.92 minutes till the final cortisol measurement after the surveys were finished (t3).

Figure 1. Cortisol levels by group and time. The salivary cortisol levels, within the expected range for both groups at the baseline (t0), increased in the intervention group following the MAST procedure (t2) and remain elevated until the surveys were filled by the participants (t3). Error bars display the standard deviation.

We did not reject the null hypotheses for the group differences in conspiracy survey (t(108.82) = 0.54; p = .30) or novel conspiracy explanations (t(111.14) = 1.37; p = .09; see Figure 2). Exploratory post hoc analysis estimates the effect of acute stress on conspiracy items to d = 0.10 (CI = [−0.29; 0.46]) and on novel conspiracy explanations to d = 0.26 (CI = [−0.09; 0.64]). We reached the same conclusion in the sensitivity analysis using binarized outcome variables (t(103.30) = 0.59; p = .28 for conspiracy items and t(110.31) = 0.52; p = .30 for novel conspiracy explanations) and Mann–Whitney test (W = 1510; p = .68 for conspiracy items and W = 1389; p = .15 for novel conspiracy explanations).

Figure 2. Mean answer to the conspiracy theory items (left) and novel conspiracy explanations items (right) by group. The mean responses (coded from 1 for “strongly disagree” to 6 for “strongly agree”) of individual respondents are displayed as dots; boxplots display group interquartile ranges.

Discussion and conclusions

We did not find statistically significant effects of acute stress reactions on agreement with CTs in this study. Despite a substantial increase in salivary cortisol in the stress group, we found no difference in either of the two measures of conspiracy beliefs. The evidence on psychophysiological processes implies that the effects of acute stress on cognition (FeldmanHall et al., Reference FeldmanHall, Raio, Kubota, Seiler and Phelps2015; Margittai et al., Reference Margittai, Nave, Strombach, van Wingerden, Schwabe and Kalenscher2016; Sandi, Reference Sandi2013; Shields et al., Reference Shields, Sazma and Yonelinas2016) are likely to impact the evaluation of CTs (Duch, Reference Duch2021). Yet, our results imply that these effects may be weak or absent, particularly for the endorsement of existing CTs. Even in a homogenous sample intentionally stressed by a standardized experimental procedure in laboratory settings, we were unable to confirm an effect large enough to cause an increase of at least 7% of the interindividual differences in endorsing CTs. These results implicate that the correlation observed between subjectively perceived stress and endorsement of conspiracy items across different subjects by Swami et al. (Reference Swami, Furnham, Smyth, Weis, Lay and Clow2016) is unlikely to be fully mediated by the physiological stress reaction.

Despite not finding a significant relationship between acute stress and beliefs in CTs, the study contributes to the scholarship in two interrelated ways. First, it shows that perceived stress should be investigated separately from the physiological stress reaction, as their effects on beliefs in CTs may differ. In other words, the academic literature on CTs could benefit from a more nuanced examination of stress. Second, the study highlights potential situational and response biases in solely survey-based research on CTs, as momentary emotional states and response styles might influence survey responses. In this regard, measuring acute stress response through hormonal levels opens a new path in political science and sociology, allowing future studies to triangulate their findings and provide a more detailed analysis of the relationship between types of stress and agreement with CTs. Given the rapid contemporary advancements in technologies to measure physiological states, the social scientific literature on and beyond CTs could benefit from these measures.

It should be noted that the generalization of our null results is limited by the setting of the experiment: the nature of the stress procedure, the operationalization of conspiracy beliefs, and the specifics of the sample. With respect to the first, we examined the effects of stress in 30 minutes after applying the stressing procedure and demonstrated that cortisol salivary levels were significantly increased before and after filling the surveys. Yet, the effects of acute stress on the brain and cognition may differ in reaction to different stressors and in timescales ranging from seconds to days or even weeks (Joëls & Baram, Reference Joëls and Baram2009; Quaedflieg & Schwabe, Reference Quaedflieg and Schwabe2018). Therefore, our study cannot rule out potential effects of acute stress on conspiracy beliefs present in stages of the stress response different than those reflected by this study. However, relevant effects of stress on cognition are linked particularly to the cortisol peak about 5–40 minutes after stress. For instance, cognitive control affects conspiracy thinking (Kantorowicz-Reznichenko et al., Reference Kantorowicz-Reznichenko, Folmer and Kantorowicz2022; Pisl et al., Reference Pisl, Volavka, Chvojkova, Cechova, Kavalirova and Vevera2021; Stoica & Umbreș, Reference Stoica and Umbreș2020), while the effects of acute stress on cognitive control are mediated by cortisol (Margittai et al., Reference Margittai, Nave, Strombach, van Wingerden, Schwabe and Kalenscher2016; Yu, Reference Yu2016). Similarly, the memory processes theorized to mediate the effects of stress on conspiracy thinking (Duch, Reference Duch2021) are also linked to cortisol (Quaedflieg et al., Reference Quaedflieg, Schwabe, Meyer and Smeets2013, Reference Quaedflieg, Stoffregen and Ashton2022; Quaedflieg & Schwabe, Reference Quaedflieg and Schwabe2018; Smeets et al., Reference Smeets, van Ruitenbeek, Hartogsveld and Quaedflieg2019). Furthermore, it is plausible to expect that the effects of acute stress response in the first minutes may be too brief and elusive and the long-term effects too weak to cause substantial effects on human propensity to CTs. In sum, there is little evidence suggesting that other effects of acute stress response affect conspiracy thinking more strongly than those measured in this study.

With respect to the conspiracy measures used, we confirmed no effect of acute stress on the endorsement of existing CTs presumably known to the participants or on conspiracy interpretations of novel information. These results imply that acute stress affected neither the social processes influencing the readiness to express agreement with CTs nor the cognitive processes underlying the acceptance of conspiracy explanations when facing novel information. Our collection of diverse items might have failed to capture effects related to specific topics or groups of CTs. For instance, acute stress might have specific effects on CTs related to matters personally affecting the respondents, as stress may orient attention towards threats (McHugh et al., Reference McHugh, Behar, Gutner, Geem and Otto2010; Posner & Petersen, Reference Posner and Petersen1990), perhaps triggering the need for the existential functions of CTs (Douglas et al., Reference Douglas, Uscinski, Sutton, Cichocka, Nefes, Ang and Deravi2019). Furthermore, situational factors could moderate the effects of stress. For example, acute stress might increase inclination to shared conspiracy explanations in social situations when these function to increase belonging to a group (Douglas et al., Reference Douglas, Sutton and Cichocka2017, Reference Douglas, Uscinski, Sutton, Cichocka, Nefes, Ang and Deravi2019). Yet, it is plausible to assume that if the relationship between acute stress and conspiracy thinking was strong, it would have been reflected in our data.

Finally, the sample of students of medicine may have specific cognitive and/or social characteristics affecting which processes and predictors are relevant for their decision whether to endorse a conspiracy explanation. Therefore, our study does not rule out the effect of cortisol on conspiracy thinking when certain moderating factors are present, such as specific cognitive habits (Bensley et al., Reference Bensley, Watkins, Lilienfeld, Masciocchi, Murtagh and Rowan2022; Dagnall et al., Reference Dagnall, Drinkwater, Parker, Denovan and Parton2015) or strategies to cope with stress (Marchlewska et al., Reference Marchlewska, Green, Cichocka, Molenda and Douglas2022). Indeed, higher education, an increased tendency to analytical thinking, or exposure to public campaigns against CTs could have prevented the effects of cortisol on cognition from fully translating into conspiracy thinking. Thus, the effect of acute stress on CTs could be stronger in the general public compared to our sample and perhaps particularly strong in some specific populations.

In our experiment, a standardized stressing procedure provenly eliciting significant physiological stress responses has not manipulated the conspiracy beliefs of a sample with little sociodemographic differences in a measurable manner. Thus, our study demonstrates that the surge of cortisol following exposure to a stressful event does not largely increase neither the acceptance of conspiracy explanations in novel situations nor the general endorsement of popular CTs. Yet, acute stress responses might substantially affect conspiracy beliefs under specific circumstances, in specific populations, or with respect to specific topics. Further studies using similar designs should complement our results, as the present study confirmed the viability of psychophysiological experimenting in conspiracy research.

Data availability statement

The data, coding files and other materials, and pre-registration are publicly available at: https://osf.io/37s6a/.

Financial support

The work was supported by the Association for Politics and Life Sciences, Research Grant Award for data collection, and by the Ministry of the Interior of the Czech Republic, project VJ01010116, and by Charles University: Cooperation, Neuroscience. VP was further supported by the Ministry of education, Youth, and Sports of the Czech Republic (grant SVV 260631). BS was further supported by Charles University, project PRIMUS/24/SSH/017.

Competing interest

Authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.

Footnotes

This article was awarded Open Data, Open Materials and Preregistration badges for transparent practices. See the data availability statement for details.

References

Bensley, D. A., Watkins, C., Lilienfeld, S. O., Masciocchi, C., Murtagh, M. P., & Rowan, K. (2022). Skepticism, cynicism, and cognitive style predictors of the generality of unsubstantiated belief. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 36(1), 8399. https://doi.org/10.1002/ACP.3900CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brotherton, R., French, C. C., & Pickering, A. D. (2013). Measuring belief in conspiracy theories: The generic conspiracist beliefs scale. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 279. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00279CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dagnall, N., Drinkwater, K., Parker, A., Denovan, A., & Parton, M. (2015). Conspiracy theory and cognitive style: A worldview. Frontiers in Psychology, 6(FEB), 128279. https://doi.org/10.3389/FPSYG.2015.00206/BIBTEXCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Delacre, M., Lakens, D., & Leys, C. (2017). Why psychologists should by default use Welch’s T-test instead of student’s T-test. International Review of Social Psychology, 30(1), 92101. https://doi.org/10.5334/IRSP.82CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Douglas, K. M., Sutton, R. M., & Cichocka, A. (2017). The psychology of conspiracy theories. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 26(6), 538542. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721417718261CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Douglas, K. M., Sutton, R. M., Jolley, D., & Wood, M. J. (2015). The social, political, environmental, and health-related consequences of conspiracy theories: Problems and potential solutions. In Bilewicz, M., Cichocka, A., & Soral, W., (Eds.), The psychology of conspiracy. 183200. Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group.Google Scholar
Douglas, K. M., Uscinski, J. E., Sutton, R. M., Cichocka, A., Nefes, T., Ang, C. S., & Deravi, F. (2019). Understanding conspiracy theories. Political Psychology, 40(S1), 335. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12568CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duch, W. (2021). Memetics and neural models of conspiracy theories. Patterns, 2(11), 100353. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.PATTER.2021.100353CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Enders, A. M., Uscinski, J. E., Klofstad, C. A., Seelig, M. I., Wuchty, S., Murthi, M. N., Premaratne, K., Funchion, J. R., & Keefer, L. A. (2021). Do conspiracy beliefs form a belief system? examining the structure and organization of conspiracy beliefs. Journal of Social and Political Psychology, 9(1), 255271. https://doi.org/10.5964/JSPP.5649CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175191. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193146CrossRefGoogle Scholar
FeldmanHall, O., Raio, C. M., Kubota, J. T., Seiler, M. G., & Phelps, E. A. (2015). The effects of social context and acute stress on decision making under uncertainty. Psychological Science, 26(12), 19181926. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615605807/ASSET/IMAGES/LARGE/10.1177_0956797615605807-FIG2.JPEGCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gray, M. (2010). Conspiracy theories in the Arab world. Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halford, C., Jonsdottir, I. H., & Eek, F. (2012). Perceived stress, psychological resources and salivary cortisol. In Margareta Kristenson, Peter Garvin, Ulf Lundberg, The role of saliva cortisol measurement in health and disease (pp. 6786) Bentham Science Publishers. DOI: 10.2174/97816080534211120101Google Scholar
Hellhammer, D. H., Wüst, S., & Kudielka, B. M. (2009). Salivary cortisol as a biomarker in stress research. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34(2), 163171. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.PSYNEUEN.2008.10.026CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hermans, E. J., Henckens, M. J. A. G., Joëls, M., & Fernández, G. (2014). Dynamic adaptation of large-scale brain networks in response to acute stressors. Trends in Neurosciences, 37(6), 304314. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.TINS.2014.03.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hofstadter, R. (1965). The paranoid style in American politics and other essays. Alfred A. Knopf.Google Scholar
Hofstra, W. A., & de Weerd, A. W. (2008). How to assess circadian rhythm in humans: A review of literature. Epilepsy & Behavior, 13(3), 438444. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.YEBEH.2008.06.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Imhoff, R., & Lamberty, P. K. (2017). Too special to be duped: Need for uniqueness motivates conspiracy beliefs. European Journal of Social Psychology, 47(6), 724734. https://doi.org/10.1002/EJSP.2265CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Joëls, M., & Baram, T. Z. (2009). The neuro-symphony of stress. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 10(6), 459. https://doi.org/10.1038/NRN2632CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kantorowicz-Reznichenko, E., Folmer, C. R., & Kantorowicz, J. (2022). Don’t believe it! A global perspective on cognitive reflection and conspiracy theories about COVID-19 pandemic. Personality and Individual Differences, 194, 111666. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.PAID.2022.111666CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kassambara, A. (2020). rstatix: Pipe-friendly framework for basic statistical tests. R Package Version 0.6. 0.Google Scholar
Knight, P. (2000). Conspiracy culture: From the Kennedy assassination to the X-Files. Routledge.Google Scholar
Krüppel, J., Yoon, D., & Mokros, A. (2023). Clarifying the link between anxiety and conspiracy beliefs: A cross-sectional study on the role of coping with stressors. Personality and Individual Differences, 202, 111966. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.PAID.2022.111966CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhlmann, S., & Wolf, O. T. (2005). Cortisol and memory retrieval in women: Influence of menstrual cycle and oral contraceptives. Psychopharmacology, 183(1), 6571. https://doi.org/10.1007/S00213-005-0143-Z/FIGURES/1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lantian, A., Muller, D., Nurra, C., & Douglas, K. M. (2017). I know things they don’t know!. Social Psychology, 48(3), 160173. https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/A000306CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lazarides, C., Ward, E. Ben, Buss, C., Chen, W. P., Voelkle, M. C., Gillen, D. L., Wadhwa, P. D., & Entringer, S. (2020). Psychological stress and cortisol during pregnancy: An ecological momentary assessment (EMA)-based within- and between-person analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 121, 104848. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.PSYNEUEN.2020.104848CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marchlewska, M., Green, R., Cichocka, A., Molenda, Z., & Douglas, K. M. (2022). From bad to worse: Avoidance coping with stress increases conspiracy beliefs. British Journal of Social Psychology, 61(2), 532549. https://doi.org/10.1111/BJSO.12494CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Margittai, Z., Nave, G., Strombach, T., van Wingerden, M., Schwabe, L., & Kalenscher, T. (2016). Exogenous cortisol causes a shift from deliberative to intuitive thinking. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 64, 131135. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.PSYNEUEN.2015.11.018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McHugh, R. K., Behar, E., Gutner, C. A., Geem, D., & Otto, M. W. (2010). Cortisol, stress, and attentional bias toward threat. Anxiety, Stress, & Coping, 23(5), 529545. https://doi.org/10.1080/10615801003596969CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, R., Plessow, F., Kirschbaum, C., & Stalder, T. (2013). Classification criteria for distinguishing cortisol responders from nonresponders to psychosocial stress: Evaluation of salivary cortisol pulse detection in panel designs. Psychosomatic Medicine, 75(9), 832840. https://doi.org/10.1097/PSY.0000000000000002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moravec, P., Minas, R., & Dennis, A. R. (2018). Fake news on social media: People believe what they want to believe when it makes no sense at all. Kelley School of Business Research Paper, 18(87). https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3269541Google Scholar
Nefes, T. S. (2014). Rationale of conspiracy theorizing: Who shot the President Chen Shui-bian? Rationality and Society, 26(3), 373394. https://doi.org/10.1177/1043463113519069CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nefes, T. S. (2015). Scrutinizing impacts of conspiracy theories on readers’ political views: A rational choice perspective on anti-semitic rhetoric in Turkey. The British Journal of Sociology, 66(3), 557575. https://doi.org/10.1111/1468-4446.12137CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nefes, T. S. (2017). The impacts of the Turkish government’s conspiratorial framing of the Gezi Park protests. Social Movement Studies, 16(5), 610622. https://doi.org/10.1080/14742837.2017.1319269CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pisl, V., Volavka, J., Chvojkova, E., Cechova, K., Kavalirova, G., & Vevera, J. (2021). Dissociation, cognitive reflection and health literacy have modest effect on belief in conspiracy theories about COVID-19. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 18(10), 5056. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18105065CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J.-Y., & Podsakoff, N. P. (2003). Common method biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. Lindell & Whit-Ney. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879Google ScholarPubMed
Posner, M. I., & Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, 2542. https://doi.org/10.1146/ANNUREV.NE.13.030190.000325CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quaedflieg, C. W. E. M., Meyer, T., van Ruitenbeek, P., & Smeets, T. (2017). Examining habituation and sensitization across repetitive laboratory stress inductions using the MAST. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 77, 175181. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.PSYNEUEN.2016.12.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quaedflieg, C. W. E. M., & Schwabe, L. (2018). Memory dynamics under stress. Memory, 26(3), 364376. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2017.1338299CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quaedflieg, C. W. E. M., Schwabe, L., Meyer, T., & Smeets, T. (2013). Time dependent effects of stress prior to encoding on event-related potentials and 24 h delayed retrieval. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 38(12), 30573069. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.PSYNEUEN.2013.09.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quaedflieg, C. W. E. M., Stoffregen, H., & Ashton, S. M. (2022). Cortisol reactivity impairs suppression-induced forgetting. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 142, 105774. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.PSYNEUEN.2022.105774CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raab, M. H., Ortlieb, S. A., Auer, N., Guthmann, K., & Carbon, C. C. (2013). Thirty shades of truth: Conspiracy theories as stories of individuation, not of pathological delusion. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(JUL), 53050. https://doi.org/10.3389/FPSYG.2013.00406/BIBTEXCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Räikkä, J. (2018). Conspiracies and conspiracy theories: An introduction. Argumenta, 3(2), 205216. https://doi.org/10.23811/51.arg2017.raiGoogle Scholar
Robins, R., & Post, J. (1997). Political paranoia: The psycho-politics of hatred. Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Romer, D., & Jamieson, K. H. (2020). Conspiracy theories as barriers to controlling the spread of COVID-19 in the U.S. Social Science & Medicine, 263, 113356. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.SOCSCIMED.2020.113356CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
RStudio Team. (2022). RStudio: Integrated development for R. http://www.rstudio.comGoogle Scholar
Sandi, C. (2013). Stress and cognition. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 4(3), 245261. https://doi.org/10.1002/WCS.1222Google ScholarPubMed
Schwabe, L. (2017). Memory under stress: From single systems to network changes. European Journal of Neuroscience, 45(4), 478489. https://doi.org/10.1111/EJN.13478CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shields, G. S., Sazma, M. A., & Yonelinas, A. P. (2016). The effects of acute stress on core executive functions: A meta-analysis and comparison with cortisol. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 68, 651668. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUBIOREV.2016.06.038CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simione, L., Vagni, M., Gnagnarella, C., Bersani, G., & Pajardi, D. (2021). Mistrust and beliefs in conspiracy theories differently mediate the effects of psychological factors on propensity for COVID-19 vaccine. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 2441. https://doi.org/10.3389/FPSYG.2021.683684/BIBTEXCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smeets, T., Cornelisse, S., Quaedflieg, C. W. E. M., Meyer, T., Jelicic, M., & Merckelbach, H. (2012). Introducing the maastricht acute stress test (MAST): A quick and non-invasive approach to elicit robust autonomic and glucocorticoid stress responses. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 37(12), 19982008. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.PSYNEUEN.2012.04.012CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smeets, T., van Ruitenbeek, P., Hartogsveld, B., & Quaedflieg, C. W. E. M. (2019). Stress-induced reliance on habitual behavior is moderated by cortisol reactivity. Brain and Cognition, 133, 6071. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.BANDC.2018.05.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stoica, C. A., & Umbreș, R. (2020). Suspicious minds in times of crisis: Determinants of Romanians’ beliefs in COVID-19 conspiracy theories. European Societies, 23(sup1), S246S261. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616696.2020.1823450CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stojanov, A., & Halberstadt, J. (2019). The conspiracy mentality scale: Distinguishing between irrational and rational suspicion. Social Psychology, 50(4), 215232. https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000381CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strahler, J., Skoluda, N., Kappert, M. B., & Nater, U. M. (2017). Simultaneous measurement of salivary cortisol and alpha-amylase: Application and recommendations. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 83, 657677. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUBIOREV.2017.08.015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swami, V., Coles, R., Stieger, S., Pietschnig, J., Furnham, A., Rehim, S., & Voracek, M. (2011). Conspiracist ideation in Britain and Austria: Evidence of a monological belief system and associations between individual psychological differences and real-world and fictitious conspiracy theories. British Journal of Psychology, 102(3), 443463. https://doi.org/10.1111/J.2044-8295.2010.02004.XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swami, V., Furnham, A., Smyth, N., Weis, L., Lay, A., & Clow, A. (2016). Putting the stress on conspiracy theories: Examining associations between psychological stress, anxiety, and belief in conspiracy theories. Personality and Individual Differences, 99, 7276. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.PAID.2016.04.084CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Syropoulos, S., & Gkinopoulos, T. (2023). Who do we trust? Differences in types of trust and beliefs in conspiracy theories between vaccinated and unvaccinated Europeans across 17 European countries. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 17(9), e12792. https://doi.org/10.1111/SPC3.12792CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tourangeau, R., Rips, L. J., & Rasinski, K. (2000). The psychology of survey response. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Prooijen, J. W. (2019). An existential threat model of conspiracy theories. European Psychologist, 25(1), 1625. https://doi.org/10.1027/1016-9040/A000381CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Prooijen, J.-W., Douglas, K. M., Jan-Willem van Prooijen, C., & Douglas, K. (2018). Belief in conspiracy theories: Basic principles of an emerging research domain. European Journal of Social Psychology, 48(7), 897908. https://doi.org/10.1002/EJSP.2530CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wickham, H., Averick, M., Bryan, J., Chang, W., McGowan, L., François, R., Grolemund, G., Hayes, A., Henry, L., Hester, J., Kuhn, M., Pedersen, T., Miller, E., Bache, S., Müller, K., Ooms, J., Robinson, D., Seidel, D., Spinu, V., … Yutani, H. (2019). Welcome to the tidyverse. Journal of Open Source Software, 4(43), 1686. https://doi.org/10.21105/joss.01686CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yu, R. (2016). Stress potentiates decision biases: A stress induced deliberation-to-intuition (SIDI) model. Neurobiology of Stress, 3, 8395. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.YNSTR.2015.12.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. Cortisol levels by group and time. The salivary cortisol levels, within the expected range for both groups at the baseline (t0), increased in the intervention group following the MAST procedure (t2) and remain elevated until the surveys were filled by the participants (t3). Error bars display the standard deviation.

Figure 1

Figure 2. Mean answer to the conspiracy theory items (left) and novel conspiracy explanations items (right) by group. The mean responses (coded from 1 for “strongly disagree” to 6 for “strongly agree”) of individual respondents are displayed as dots; boxplots display group interquartile ranges.