Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-mlc7c Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-17T21:52:44.018Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Creating an acute energy deficit without stimulating compensatory increases in appetite: is there an optimal exercise protocol?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 April 2014

Kevin Deighton*
Affiliation:
School of Sport, Leeds Metropolitan University, Leeds LS6 3QS, UK School of Sport, Exercise and Health Sciences, Loughborough University, Leicestershire LE11 3TU, UK
David J. Stensel
Affiliation:
School of Sport, Exercise and Health Sciences, Loughborough University, Leicestershire LE11 3TU, UK
*
*Corresponding author: K. Deighton, email K.Deighton@leedsmet.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Recent years have witnessed significant interest from both the scientific community and the media regarding the influence of exercise on subsequent appetite and energy intake responses. This review demonstrates a consensus among the majority of scientific investigations that an acute bout of land-based endurance exercise does not stimulate any compensatory increases in appetite and energy intake on the day of exercise. Alternatively, preliminary evidence suggests that low volume, supramaximal exercise may stimulate an increase in appetite perceptions during the subsequent hours. In accordance with the apparent insensitivity of energy intake to exercise in the short term, the daily energy balance response to exercise appears to be primarily determined by the energy cost of exercise. This finding supports the conclusions of recent training studies that the energy expenditure of exercise is the strongest predictor of fat loss during an exercise programme.

Type
Conference on ‘Nutrition and healthy ageing’
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2014 

Abbreviations:
HIIE

high-intensity intermittent exercise

VO2 max

maximum oxygen uptake

Overweight and obesity are defined by a BMI of 25–29·9 and 30 kg/m2 or greater, respectively, and characterised by an excess accumulation of body fat. Recent decades have witnessed a global increase in BMI to the extent that 22·5 % of adults worldwide were estimated to be overweight in 2008, with an additional 11·8 % qualifying as obese( Reference Finucane, Stevens and Cowan 1 ). These conditions are associated with an increased prevalence of several chronic diseases( Reference Bray 2 ), which has resulted in the classification of overweight and obesity as one of the top five global risk factors for mortality and one of the top ten risk factors for morbidity( 3 ). However, weight loss as little as 3 % has been associated with favourable changes in chronic disease risk factors and therefore represents a major public health priority( Reference Donnelly, Blair and Jakicic 4 ).

For weight loss to occur, a sustained negative energy balance is required and is typically achieved by decreasing energy intake (i.e. dieting) and/or increasing energy expenditure (i.e. exercising). Interest in the appetite and energy intake responses to exercise stems from the acknowledgement that physical activity may enhance weight loss via an increase in energy expenditure( Reference Donnelly, Blair and Jakicic 4 ) but is dependent upon subsequent food intake, as an increase in energy consumption may negate the energy deficit of exercise. Furthermore, any compensatory increases in appetite after exercise are likely to enhance the difficulty of maintaining a negative energy balance and increase psychological discomfort within participants.

The importance of this issue has recently been highlighted in two prominent media articles, which suggested that exercise stimulates compensatory increases in appetite and food intake that prevent weight loss( Reference Cloud 5 ) and actually increase body fat( Reference Gray 6 ). However, the conclusions of these articles oppose findings from the scientific literature, which indicate that appetite and energy intake remain largely unchanged in the hours after an acute bout of exercise( Reference Blundell and King 7 Reference Martins, Morgan and Truby 9 ). Although this relationship cannot continue indefinitely, the insensitivity of the appetite-regulating system to exercise-induced energy deficits is in stark contrast with the powerful homoeostatic responses to food restriction. In this regard, the current evidence suggests that food restriction elicits rapid compensatory increases in appetite and food intake, which does not occur in response to an equivalent exercise-induced energy deficit( Reference Hubert, King and Blundell 10 , Reference King, Wasse and Ewens 11 ).

The present paper aims to provide a comprehensive review of the scientific literature relating to the acute effects of exercise on appetite and energy intake responses. The final sections of this review subsequently address the question of whether there is an optimal exercise protocol for minimising compensatory increases in appetite after exercise.

Exercise-induced anorexia

Strenuous exercise (≥60 % of maximum oxygen uptake (VO2 max)) has consistently been found to acutely supress appetite during and shortly after the exercise bout. This is known as exercise-induced anorexia and has been demonstrated during a variety of exercise modes including running( Reference Broom, Stensel and Bishop 12 Reference King, Miyashita and Wasse 14 ), cycling( Reference Becker, Macedo and Cunha 15 Reference Ueda, Yoshikawa and Katsura 21 ), swimming( Reference King, Wasse and Stensel 22 ) and resistance exercise( Reference Broom, Batterham and King 23 ). Although significant changes in appetite have been reported in these studies, values tend to return to control values within 30 min of the cessation of exercise. Such a transient effect is unlikely to influence energy intake in the hours after exercise but may delay the initiation of feeding when food is provided immediately after exercise( Reference King, Wasse and Stensel 24 ). It therefore remains most important to understand the influence of exercise on resting appetite and energy intake responses in the hours after exercise.

Appetite responses in the immediate post-exercise period (0–120 min)

The majority of studies that have investigated the appetite response to exercise have employed an observation period lasting up to 2 h after the exercise bout. The consensus among these studies is that, after the recovery from exercise-induced anorexia, appetite during the post-exercise period does not differ from a resting control trial( Reference Burns, Broom and Miyashita 13 , Reference Becker, Macedo and Cunha 15 , Reference Evero, Hackett and Clark 17 Reference Martins, Morgan and Bloom 19 , Reference Erdmann, Tahbaz and Lippl 25 Reference Ueda, Yoshikawa and Katsura 34 ). However, although appetite perceptions do not appear to decrease during the post-exercise period, it must be acknowledged that some investigations have reported elevated appetite perceptions after exercise compared with a control trial( Reference Jokisch, Coletta and Raynor 35 Reference Verger, Lanteaume and Louis-Sylvestre 37 ).

The majority of the studies listed earlier have investigated the appetite response to exercise in healthy non-obese men and demonstrated no differences in appetite between exercise and control trials during the 2 h after a range of exercise modes including walking( Reference Farah, Brunstrom and Gill 26 ), cycling( Reference Becker, Macedo and Cunha 15 , Reference Evero, Hackett and Clark 17 Reference Martins, Morgan and Bloom 19 , Reference Jokisch, Coletta and Raynor 35 ) and running( Reference Burns, Broom and Miyashita 13 , Reference Gonzalez, Veasey and Rumbold 28 , Reference Kelly, Guelfi and Wallman 30 ). The use of male participants in the majority of investigations has led some authors to postulate that sex-based differences may occur in the appetite response to exercise due to the critical relationship between energy balance and reproductive function in females( Reference Wade and Jones 38 ). However, the present research suggests that females do not exhibit increased appetite perceptions during the 2 h period after exercise( Reference Laan, Leidy and Lim 18 , Reference Martins, Morgan and Bloom 19 , Reference Hagobian, Yamashiro and Hinkel-Lipsker 29 , Reference Larson-Meyer, Palm and Bansal 31 Reference Unick, Otto and Goodpaster 33 ). Furthermore, a recent study by Hagobian et al.( Reference Hagobian, Yamashiro and Hinkel-Lipsker 29 ) directly compared the appetite response to exercise in male and female participants and concluded that 80 min cycling at 70 % VO2 max did not stimulate increases in appetite in either sex during the 40 min after exercise.

As obesity is the result of a chronic excess of energy intake over energy expenditure, it is also logical to consider that appetite regulation in response to exercise may differ between lean and obese individuals. However, the present research suggests that exercise also fails to stimulate immediate compensatory increases in appetite in overweight and obese populations. In this regard, Unick et al.( Reference Unick, Otto and Goodpaster 33 ) reported that walking at 70–75 % of the maximum heart rate to expend 13 kJ/kg body mass did not stimulate any compensatory increases in appetite during the 60 min after exercise in overweight and obese women. Similarly, Tsofliou et al.( Reference Tsofliou, Pitsiladis and Malkova 32 ) did not observe any increases in appetite during the 60 min after 20 min brisk walking in ten obese healthy women. Furthermore, in a direct comparison between obese and normal weight young men, Ueda et al.( Reference Ueda, Yoshikawa and Katsura 34 ) demonstrated that 60 min cycling at 50 % VO2 max did not stimulate any increases in appetite during the 60 min after exercise in either group.

Energy intake responses in the immediate post-exercise period (0–120 min)

The majority of studies detailed in the previous section also assessed the food intake response to exercise by providing participants with an ad libitum meal ≤2 h after the exercise bout. In accordance with the appetite responses detailed in the previous section, the majority of these studies demonstrated that energy intake was unaffected by exercise( Reference George and Morganstein 27 Reference Kelly, Guelfi and Wallman 30 , Reference Tsofliou, Pitsiladis and Malkova 32 , Reference Unick, Otto and Goodpaster 33 , Reference Balaguera-Cortes, Wallman and Fairchild 39 Reference Maraki, Tsofliou and Pitsiladis 41 ). However, some studies have demonstrated increases( Reference Laan, Leidy and Lim 18 , Reference Martins, Morgan and Bloom 19 , Reference Verger, Lanteaume and Louis-Sylvestre 37 , Reference Shorten, Wallman and Guelfi 42 ) or decreases( Reference Ueda, Yoshikawa and Katsura 34 , Reference Almada, Cataldo and Smalley 43 , Reference Westerterp-Plantenga, Verwegen and Ijedema 44 ) in energy intake after exercise.

An early study to investigate the effect of exercise on subsequent food intake responses was performed by King et al. in 1994( Reference King, Burley and Blundell 40 ). In this study, cycling at 30 or 70 % VO2 max to expend ∼1460 kJ did not affect energy intake at an ad libitum buffet meal 15 min after exercise. Furthermore, in a second group of healthy young men, energy intake was unaffected by either 26 or 52 min cycling at 75 % VO2 max. Many of these findings were replicated by Erdmann et al.( Reference Erdmann, Tahbaz and Lippl 25 ) in a combined sample of normal-weight men and women, as energy intake was unaffected by 30 min cycling at either 50 or 100 W. However, although energy intake was unchanged in response to 60 min cycling at a fixed work rate of 50 W, increasing the duration to 120 min stimulated an increase in energy intake. Although these findings suggest that the energy intake response to exercise is dependent upon exercise duration, all trials commenced after a 12 h overnight fast with the ad libitum meal provided 15 min after exercise. Therefore the observed increases in energy intake may have been a result of the extended overnight fast rather than exercise duration per se.

Although these studies may be criticised for the provision of a buffet meal so close to the cessation of exercise, other authors have also demonstrated no change in energy intake after exercise when the ad libitum meal was provided up to 1 h after exercise. This relationship has been demonstrated in young men and women( Reference Gonzalez, Veasey and Rumbold 28 , Reference Kelly, Guelfi and Wallman 30 , Reference Balaguera-Cortes, Wallman and Fairchild 39 , Reference Maraki, Tsofliou and Pitsiladis 41 ), overweight and obese women( Reference Tsofliou, Pitsiladis and Malkova 32 , Reference Unick, Otto and Goodpaster 33 ) and in response to a variety of exercise modes. Furthermore, Hagobian et al.( Reference Hagobian, Yamashiro and Hinkel-Lipsker 29 ) reported no change in energy intake in response to exercise in men and women matched for age and VO2 max.

The potential influence of exercise mode on the energy intake response to exercise has been demonstrated by Larson-Meyer et al.( Reference Larson-Meyer, Palm and Bansal 31 ). In this study, energy intake from an ad libitum meal that was provided 2 h after exercise was not affected by 60 min running at 70 % VO2 max but increased in response to 60 min walking exercise at the same relative intensity in a separate group of participants. Although this study suggests that the exercise mode may influence energy intake responses, it is plausible that the higher percentage body fat and lower VO2 max in the walking group may have confounded the results.

In this regard, Finlayson et al.( Reference Finlayson, Bryant and Blundell 45 ) demonstrated that body fat and physical activity levels may influence the energy intake response to exercise in females. This novel study analysed the energy intake response to 50 min cycling exercise and separated the participants into two groups: compensators and non-compensators. Compensators were defined as the participants that increased energy intake beyond the energy cost of exercise, whereas non-compensators consumed less energy than that expended during exercise. Analysis of between-group differences revealed a significantly higher BMI and percentage body fat and a lower habitual exercise frequency in the compensators. Alternatively, recent evidence suggests that this relationship may not occur in men as Jokisch et al.( Reference Jokisch, Coletta and Raynor 35 ) demonstrated that 45 min cycling at 65–75 % of the maximum heart rate decreased energy intake in a sample of inactive normal weight young men but not active normal weight young men.

Other studies have also demonstrated a decrease in energy intake but are confounded by the provision of the ad libitum meal within 10 min of exercise completion( Reference Almada, Cataldo and Smalley 43 , Reference Westerterp-Plantenga, Verwegen and Ijedema 44 ), which may have prevented the recovery from exercise-induced anorexia prior to feeding. However, in support of these findings, Ueda et al.( Reference Ueda, Yoshikawa and Katsura 34 ) observed a decrease in energy intake at an ad libitum meal that was provided 60 min after a 60 min bout of cycling at 50 % VO2 max. Furthermore, this reduction in energy intake occurred in both lean and obese male participants. Other authors have also reported similar energy intake responses in normal weight and overweight participants( Reference George and Morganstein 27 ) but this is not a universal finding( Reference Kissileff, Pi-Sunyer and Segal 46 ).

Some authors have demonstrated an increase in energy intake during the immediate post-exercise period( Reference Laan, Leidy and Lim 18 , Reference Martins, Morgan and Bloom 19 , Reference Verger, Lanteaume and Louis-Sylvestre 37 , Reference Shorten, Wallman and Guelfi 42 ). The reasons for such variations in the energy intake response to exercise are unclear but may be influenced by a variety of factors including participant differences, the composition and timing of the ad libitum meal, variations in exercise mode and time-of-day effects. Although measures of absolute energy intake provide important information regarding feeding behaviour, King et al.( Reference King, Burley and Blundell 40 ) suggested that it may be more relevant to express the energy intake response to exercise as ‘relative energy intake’ after deducting the net energy cost of exercise. Subsequently, all increases in absolute energy intake reported thus far in this review are negated after accounting for the energy expenditure of exercise. Although this approach provides an overview of energy balance, the short monitoring period of the studies described thus far bias the results towards a lower relative energy intake during exercise trials as food intake is unlikely to be up-regulated sufficiently at a single feeding episode to overturn substantial energy deficits. Therefore investigations into the energy intake response to multiple ad libitum meals are important and are discussed later.

Appetite responses beyond a single test meal (2–9 h)

Although some authors have suggested that exercise may stimulate appetite in response to a standardised meal in the post-exercise period( Reference Malkova, McLaughlin and Manthou 47 ), the majority of studies demonstrate that appetite does not increase above control values during the 2–9 h after exercise. This includes a variety of exercise modes including running( Reference King, Miyashita and Wasse 14 , Reference Broom, Batterham and King 23 , Reference King, Lluch and Stubbs 48 Reference Wasse, Sunderland and King 50 ), cycling( Reference Wasse, Sunderland and King 50 ), walking( Reference Borer, Wuorinen and Chao 51 Reference Pomerleau, Imbeault and Parker 54 ) and resistance exercise( Reference Broom, Batterham and King 23 ) in both male and female populations.

However, exceptions have been observed within the literature as King et al.( Reference King, Wasse and Stensel 22 ) demonstrated that 60 min intermittent swimming exercise stimulated an increase in appetite compared with a resting control trial from 1·5 to 6 h after exercise. This contrasts with previous findings from the same author as 60 min brisk walking exercise( Reference King, Wasse and Broom 53 ) and 90 min running exercise( Reference King, Miyashita and Wasse 14 ) did not influence resting appetite perceptions during the 7 and 8·5 h post-exercise period, respectively. These differences occurred despite utilising a similar experimental protocol and participant population of physically active young men, which suggests a potential influence of exercise mode on subsequent appetite responses. However, subtle changes in meal timing and the recruitment of different participants confound any inferences regarding exercise mode.

The potential confounding influence of these factors is demonstrated by the findings of two studies from Broom et al.( Reference Broom, Stensel and Bishop 12 , Reference Broom, Batterham and King 23 ), as 60 min running at 70 % VO2 max in young physically active males stimulated an increase in hunger from 2 to 8 h after exercise in the former but not the latter of the two studies. Such discrepancies highlight the need for within-measures study designs when comparing the appetite response to different exercise protocols.

Energy intake responses beyond a single test meal (2–9 h)

Few studies have monitored food intake in a laboratory setting for more than 2 h after an acute exercise bout. However, the available research suggests that exercise does not stimulate any changes in energy intake during the subsequent 22·5 h( Reference King, Miyashita and Wasse 14 ). This finding is also supported by studies that have used self-report measures of food intake, which have failed to discover any changes in energy intake during the 24 h( Reference Hanlon, Larson and Bailey 55 ), 48 h( Reference King, Lluch and Stubbs 48 ) and 72 h( Reference Pomerleau, Imbeault and Parker 54 ) after exercise.

Studies that have successfully performed prolonged monitoring of food intake in a laboratory setting have typically provided participants with one or two buffet meals during the 3–7·5 h after exercise. Studies from our laboratory have reported that energy intake is unchanged by exercise under these laboratory conditions at any of the provided feeding opportunities( Reference King, Miyashita and Wasse 14 , Reference King, Wasse and Stensel 22 , Reference Wasse, Sunderland and King 49 , Reference King, Wasse and Broom 53 ). Furthermore, the provision of an overnight food bag upon leaving the laboratory demonstrated that energy intake continued to remain unchanged for 22·5 h after exercise( Reference King, Miyashita and Wasse 14 ).

The importance of such prolonged monitoring of energy intake is demonstrated by Pomerleau et al.( Reference Pomerleau, Imbeault and Parker 54 ). In this study, energy intake at an ad libitum buffet meal was significantly higher 1 h after walking at 70 % VO2 max compared with a resting control. However, after the provision of an additional ad libitum meal 6·5 h after exercise and an overnight snack bag, energy intake did not differ significantly between the trials.

One weakness of the earlier studies is that meals were provided to participants at pre-defined time points during the trials, which constrains the opportunities for food intake and may hinder the detection of differences in energy intake. This issue was recently addressed by King et al.( Reference King, Wasse and Stensel 24 ) who allowed participants unlimited access to a buffet meal during the 6 h after 60 min of running at 70 % VO2 max. In support of previous findings, energy intake remained unchanged after exercise compared with a resting control trial and resulted in a substantially lower relative energy intake after exercise.

Manipulating exercise protocols to produce the most beneficial responses

This review has demonstrated a consensus among the majority of the scientific literature that an acute bout of land-based exercise does not stimulate compensatory increases in appetite and energy intake during the following hours. Subsequently, recent efforts have focussed on manipulating exercise protocols to most effectively reduce appetite and energy balance in the hours after exercise.

In this regard, Cheng et al.( Reference Cheng, Bushnell and Cannon 16 ) recently reported that moderate intensity cycling exercise induced a more prolonged suppression of hunger when performed 2 h after a high-fat breakfast compared with after a 12 h overnight fast. However, a subsequent investigation from our research group at Loughborough University suggested that this effect is limited to the period of exercise-induced anorexia, as appetite and energy intake did not differ from a resting control trial during the 5–9 h period after fasted or postprandial exercise despite greater appetite suppression during the postprandial exercise bout( Reference Deighton, Zahra and Stensel 56 ).

More commonly, investigators have manipulated the intensity of exercise when comparing exercise protocols but initial studies yielded equivocal findings. In this regard, Thompson et al.( Reference Thompson, Wolfe and Eikelboom 20 ) demonstrated greater appetite suppression during cycling exercise at 68 % VO2 max compared with 35 % VO2 max but found no differences in energy intake after exercise. Conversely, Imbeault et al.( Reference Imbeault, Saint-Pierre and Alméras 57 ) found no differences in appetite but reported a decrease in energy intake after running at 75 % VO2 max compared with energy-matched walking at 35 % VO2 max. Alternatively, Ueda et al.( Reference Ueda, Yoshikawa and Katsura 21 ) and King et al.( Reference King, Burley and Blundell 40 ) found no differences in the appetite or energy intake responses to cycling at 50 v. 75 % VO2 max and 30 v. 70 % VO2 max, respectively.

Interest in the influence of exercise intensity on appetite and energy intake responses has been renewed in recent years due to the popularity of high-intensity intermittent exercise (HIIE). In this regard, several authors within both the scientific literature and the media have suggested that HIIE may elicit greater weight loss than traditional endurance exercise due to greater reductions in appetite during the post-exercise period( Reference Boutcher 58 Reference Bendoris 63 ). However, despite these postulations, research from our laboratory has demonstrated that a recently popularised sprint interval exercise protocol, consisting of six 30 s maximal sprints on a cycle ergometer, stimulated compensatory increases in appetite during the 5 h monitoring period after exercise that did not occur in response to 60 min continuous cycling at 60 % VO2 max. Although these differences in appetite did not influence ad libitum food intake on the day of exercise, similar absolute food intake between trials resulted in a substantial negative energy balance after endurance exercise compared with sprint exercise and a resting control trial due to a significantly higher energy expenditure during the endurance exercise than sprint exercise (mean 2640 (sd 418) v. 594 (sd 50) kJ; P<0·0005)( Reference Deighton, Barry and Connon 64 ).

In order to further investigate this issue, a high-volume, submaximal HIIE protocol was investigated in a subsequent study and it was determined that compensatory increases in appetite and energy intake did not occur during the 5 h monitoring period after ten 4 min cycling intervals at 85–90 % VO2 max or 60 min continuous cycling at 60 % VO2 max( Reference Deighton, Karra and Batterham 65 ). The absence of any compensatory increases in appetite in this study suggests that previously observed increases after supramaximal interval exercise are likely to be a result of the extreme intensity rather than the intermittent nature of exercise. It remains plausible that a threshold exercise intensity may exist for the stimulation of appetite during the post-exercise period but this requires further investigation. It is also important to note that these studies employed exercise protocols that represented both extremes of the HIIE spectrum (i.e. very low-volume supramaximal interval exercise and high-volume, submaximal interval exercise) and therefore provides convincing evidence that HIIE does not elicit lower appetite perceptions during the hours after exercise than traditional endurance exercise.

Conclusions

Contrary to recent media articles, this review demonstrates a consensus among the majority of scientific investigations that an acute bout of land-based endurance exercise does not stimulate compensatory increases in appetite and energy intake during the hours after exercise. The  present  research also disputes the postulations of recent authors that HIIE elicits lower appetite and energy intake responses than traditional endurance exercise. Alternatively, initial investigations suggest that supramaximal exercise may stimulate increases in appetite that do not occur in response to larger energy deficits induced by moderate intensity exercise. Furthermore, in accordance with the consensus that energy intake remains unresponsive to exercise in the short term, the daily energy balance response to exercise appears to be primarily determined by the energy cost of exercise. These findings support the concept that appetite and energy intake are primarily determined via oro-gastric, rather than metabolic mechanisms( Reference Borer, Wuorinen and Chao 51 ), and also support the conclusions of recent training studies that the energy expenditure of exercise is the strongest predictor of fat loss during an exercise programme( Reference Barwell, Malkova and Leggate 66 , Reference Jakicic, Marcus and Lang 67 ).

Financial support

None.

Conflicts of interest

None.

Authorship

K. D. conceived and wrote the manuscript. D. J. S. read and approved the manuscript.

References

1. Finucane, MM, Stevens, GA, Cowan, MJ et al. (2011) National, regional, and global trends in body-mass index since 1980: systematic analysis of health examination surveys and epidemiological studies with 960 country-years and 9·1 million participants. Lancet 377, 557567.Google Scholar
2. Bray, GA (2004) Medical consequences of obesity. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 89, 25832589.Google Scholar
3. World Health Organisation (2009) Global health risks. Available at http://www.who.int/healthinfo/global_burden_disease/GlobalHealthRisks_report_full.pdf Google Scholar
4. Donnelly, JE, Blair, SN, Jakicic, JM et al. (2009) American college of sports medicine position stand. Appropriate physical activity intervention strategies for weight loss and prevention of weight regain for adults. Med Sci Sports Exerc 41, 459471.Google Scholar
5. Cloud, J (2009) Why exercise won't make you thin. Time Magazine. Available at http://www.time.com/time/health/article/0,8599,1914857,00.html Google Scholar
6. Gray, R (2009) Health warning: exercise makes you fat. The Daily Telegraph. Available at http://www.telegraph.co.uk/science/6083234/Health-warning-exercise-makes-you-fat.html Google Scholar
7. Blundell, JE & King, NA (1999) Physical activity and regulation of food intake: current evidence. Med Sci Sports Exerc 31, S573S583.Google Scholar
8. Blundell, JE, Stubbs, RJ, Hughes, DA et al. (2003) Cross talk between physical activity and appetite control: does physical activity stimulate appetite? Proc Nutr Soc 62, 651661.Google Scholar
9. Martins, C, Morgan, L & Truby, H (2008) A review of the effects of exercise on appetite regulation: an obesity perspective. Int J Obes 32, 13371347.Google Scholar
10. Hubert, P, King, NA & Blundell, JE (1998) Uncoupling the effects of energy expenditure and energy intake: appetite response to short-term energy deficit induced by meal omission and physical activity. Appetite 31, 919.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
11. King, JA, Wasse, LK, Ewens, J et al. (2011) Differential acylated ghrelin, peptide YY3-36, appetite, and food intake responses to equivalent energy deficits created by exercise and food restriction. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 96, 11141121.Google Scholar
12. Broom, DR, Stensel, DJ, Bishop, NC et al. (2007) Exercise-induced suppression of acylated ghrelin in humans. J Appl Physiol 102, 21652171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
13. Burns, SF, Broom, DR, Miyashita, M et al. (2007) A single session of treadmill running has no effect on plasma total ghrelin concentrations. J Sports Sci 25, 635642.Google Scholar
14. King, JA, Miyashita, M, Wasse, LK et al. (2010) Influence of prolonged treadmill running on appetite, energy intake and circulating concentrations of acylated ghrelin. Appetite 54, 492498.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
15. Becker, GF, Macedo, RCO, Cunha, GDS et al. (2012) Combined effects of aerobic exercise and high-carbohydrate meal on plasma acylated ghrelin and levels of hunger. Appl Physiol Nutr Metab 37, 184192.Google Scholar
16. Cheng, MH-Y, Bushnell, D, Cannon, DT et al. (2009) Appetite regulation via exercise prior or subsequent to high-fat meal consumption. Appetite 52, 193198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
17. Evero, N, Hackett, LC, Clark, RD et al. (2012) Aerobic exercise reduces neuronal responses in food reward brain regions. J Appl Physiol 112, 16121619.Google Scholar
18. Laan, DJ, Leidy, HJ, Lim, E et al. (2010) Effects and reproducibility of aerobic and resistance exercise on appetite and energy intake in young, physically active adults. Appl Physiol Nutr Metab 35, 842847.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
19. Martins, C, Morgan, LM, Bloom, SR et al. (2007) Effects of exercise on gut peptides, energy intake and appetite. J Endocrinol 193, 251258.Google Scholar
20. Thompson, DA, Wolfe, LA & Eikelboom, R (1988) Acute effects of exercise intensity on appetite in young men. Med Sci Sports Exerc 20, 222227.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
21. Ueda, S, Yoshikawa, T, Katsura, Y et al. (2009) Comparable effects of moderate intensity exercise on changes in anorectic gut hormone levels and energy intake to high intensity exercise. J Endocrinol 203, 357364.Google Scholar
22. King, JA, Wasse, LK & Stensel, DJ (2011) The acute effects of swimming on appetite, food intake, and plasma acylated ghrelin. J Obes 351628.Google Scholar
23. Broom, DR, Batterham, RL, King, JA et al. (2009) Influence of resistance and aerobic exercise on hunger, circulating levels of acylated ghrelin, and peptide YY in healthy males. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 296, R29R35.Google Scholar
24. King, JA, Wasse, LK & Stensel, DJ (2013) Acute exercise increases feeding latency in healthy normal weight young males but does not alter energy intake. Appetite 61, 4551.Google Scholar
25. Erdmann, J, Tahbaz, R, Lippl, F et al. (2007) Plasma ghrelin levels during exercise – effects of intensity and duration. Regul Pept 143, 127135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
26. Farah, NMF, Brunstrom, JM & Gill, JMR (2012) Using a novel computer-based approach to assess the acute effects of exercise on appetite-related measures. Appetite 58, 196204.Google Scholar
27. George, VA & Morganstein, A (2003) Effect of moderate intensity exercise on acute energy intake in normal and overweight females. Appetite 40, 4346.Google Scholar
28. Gonzalez, JT, Veasey, RC, Rumbold, PLS et al. (2013) Breakfast and exercise contingently affect postprandial metabolism and energy balance in physically active males. Br J Nutr 110, 721732.Google Scholar
29. Hagobian, TA, Yamashiro, M, Hinkel-Lipsker, J et al. (2013) Effects of acute exercise on appetite hormones and ad libitum energy intake in men and women. Appl Physiol Nutr Metab 38, 6672.Google Scholar
30. Kelly, PJ, Guelfi, KJ, Wallman, KE et al. (2012) Mild dehydration does not reduce postexercise appetite or energy intake. Med Sci Sports Exerc 44, 516524.Google Scholar
31. Larson-Meyer, DE, Palm, S, Bansal, A et al. (2012) Influence of running and walking on hormonal regulators of appetite in women. J Obes 730409.Google Scholar
32. Tsofliou, F, Pitsiladis, YP, Malkova, D et al. (2003) Moderate physical activity permits acute coupling between serum leptin and appetite-satiety measures in obese women. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 27, 13321339.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
33. Unick, JL, Otto, AD, Goodpaster, BH et al. (2010) Acute effect of walking on energy intake in overweight/obese women. Appetite 55, 413419.Google Scholar
34. Ueda, S, Yoshikawa, T, Katsura, Y et al. (2009) Changes in gut hormone levels and negative energy balance during aerobic exercise in obese young males. J Endocrinol 201, 151159.Google Scholar
35. Jokisch, E, Coletta, A & Raynor, HA (2012) Acute energy compensation and macronutrient intake following exercise in active and inactive males who are normal weight. Appetite 58, 722729.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
36. Kawano, H, Mineta, M, Asaka, M et al. (2013) Effects of different modes of exercise on appetite and appetite-regulating hormones. Appetite 66, 2633.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
37. Verger, P, Lanteaume, MT & Louis-Sylvestre, J (1994) Free food choice after acute exercise in men. Appetite 22, 159164.Google Scholar
38. Wade, GN & Jones, JE (2004) Neuroendocrinology of nutritional infertility. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 287, R1277R1296.Google Scholar
39. Balaguera-Cortes, L, Wallman, KE, Fairchild, TJ et al. (2011) Energy intake and appetite-related hormones following acute aerobic and resistance exercise. Appl Physiol Nutr Metab 36, 958966.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
40. King, NA, Burley, VJ & Blundell, JE (1994) Exercise-induced suppression of appetite: effects on food intake and implications for energy balance. Eur J Clin Nutr 48, 715724.Google Scholar
41. Maraki, M, Tsofliou, F, Pitsiladis, YP et al. (2005) Acute effects of a single exercise class on appetite, energy intake and mood. Is there a time of day effect? Appetite 45, 272278.Google Scholar
42. Shorten, AL, Wallman, KE & Guelfi, KJ (2009) Acute effect of environmental temperature during exercise on subsequent energy intake in active men. Am J Clin Nutr 90, 12151221.Google Scholar
43. Almada, C, Cataldo, LR, Smalley, SV et al. (2013) Plasma levels of interleukin-6 and interleukin-18 after an acute physical exercise: relation with post-exercise energy intake in twins. J Physiol Biochem 69, 8595.Google Scholar
44. Westerterp-Plantenga, MS, Verwegen, CR, Ijedema, MJ et al. (1997) Acute effects of exercise or sauna on appetite in obese and nonobese men. Physiol Behav 62, 13451354.Google Scholar
45. Finlayson, G, Bryant, E, Blundell, JE et al. (2009) Acute compensatory eating following exercise is associated with implicit hedonic wanting for food. Physiol Behav 97, 6267.Google Scholar
46. Kissileff, HR, Pi-Sunyer, FX, Segal, K et al. (1990) Acute effects of exercise on food intake in obese and nonobese women. Am J Clin Nutr 52, 240245.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
47. Malkova, D, McLaughlin, R, Manthou, E et al. (2008) Effect of moderate-intensity exercise session on preprandial and postprandial responses of circulating ghrelin and appetite. Horm Metab Res 40, 410415.Google Scholar
48. King, NA, Lluch, A, Stubbs, RJ et al. (1997) High dose exercise does not increase hunger or energy intake in free living males. Eur J Clin Nutr 51, 478483.Google Scholar
49. Wasse, LK, Sunderland, C, King, JA et al. (2012) Influence of rest and exercise at a simulated altitude of 4000 m on appetite, energy intake, and plasma concentrations of acylated ghrelin and peptide YY. J Appl Physiol 112, 552559.Google Scholar
50. Wasse, LK, Sunderland, C, King, JA et al. (2013) The influence of vigorous running and cycling exercise on hunger perceptions and plasma acylated ghrelin concentrations in lean young men. Appl Physiol Nutr Metab 38, 16.Google Scholar
51. Borer, KT, Wuorinen, E, Chao, C et al. (2005) Exercise energy expenditure is not consciously detected due to oro-gastric, not metabolic, basis of hunger sensation. Appetite 45, 177181.Google Scholar
52. Borer, KT, Wuorinen, E, Ku, K et al. (2009) Appetite responds to changes in meal content, whereas ghrelin, leptin, and insulin track changes in energy availability. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 94, 22902298.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
53. King, JA, Wasse, LK, Broom, DR et al. (2010) Influence of brisk walking on appetite, energy intake, and plasma acylated ghrelin. Med Sci Sports Exerc 42, 485492.Google Scholar
54. Pomerleau, M, Imbeault, P, Parker, T et al. (2004) Effects of exercise intensity on food intake and appetite in women. Am J Clin Nutr 80, 12301236.Google Scholar
55. Hanlon, B, Larson, MJ, Bailey, BW et al. (2012) Neural response to pictures of food after exercise in normal-weight and obese women. Med Sci Sports Exerc 44, 18641870.Google Scholar
56. Deighton, K, Zahra, JC & Stensel, DJ (2012) Appetite, energy intake and resting metabolic responses to 60 min treadmill running performed in a fasted versus a postprandial state. Appetite 58, 946954.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
57. Imbeault, P, Saint-Pierre, S, Alméras, N et al. (1997) Acute effects of exercise on energy intake and feeding behaviour. Br J Nutr 77, 511521.Google Scholar
58. Boutcher, SH (2011) High-intensity intermittent exercise and fat loss. J Obes 868305.Google Scholar
59. Heydari, M, Freund, J & Boutcher, SH (2012) The effect of high-intensity intermittent exercise on body composition of overweight young males. J Obes 480467.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
60. Trapp, EG, Chisholm, DJ, Freund, J et al. (2008) The effects of high-intensity intermittent exercise training on fat loss and fasting insulin levels of young women. Int J Obes 32, 684691.Google Scholar
61. Tremblay, A, Simoneau, JA & Bouchard, C (1994) Impact of exercise intensity on body fatness and skeletal muscle metabolism. Metabolism 43, 814818.Google Scholar
62. Naish, J (2013) Is three minutes a week of exercise all you need to get fit? Scientists say ideal fitness regime involves intense bursts of activity. The Daily Mail. Available at http://www.dailymail.co.uk/health/article-2255867/Is-minutes-week-exercise-need-fit-Scientists-say-ideal-fitness-regime-involves-intense-bursts-activity.html Google Scholar
63. Bendoris, M (2012) The two-minute wonder workout. The Scottish Sun. Available at http://www.thescottishsun.co.uk/scotsol/homepage/scotlandfeatures/4159373/The-two-minute-wonder-workout.html Google Scholar
64. Deighton, K, Barry, R, Connon, CE et al. (2013) Appetite, gut hormone and energy intake responses to low volume sprint interval and traditional endurance exercise. Eur J Appl Physiol 113, 11471156.Google Scholar
65. Deighton, K, Karra, E, Batterham, RL et al. (2013) Appetite, energy intake, and PYY3-36 responses to energy-matched continuous exercise and submaximal high-intensity exercise. Appl Physiol Nutr Metab 38, 947952.Google Scholar
66. Barwell, ND, Malkova, D, Leggate, M et al. (2009) Individual responsiveness to exercise-induced fat loss is associated with change in resting substrate utilization. Metabolism 58, 13201328.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
67. Jakicic, JM, Marcus, BH, Lang, W et al. (2008) Effect of exercise on 24-month weight loss maintenance in overweight women. Arch Intern Med 168, 15501559.Google Scholar