Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-7cvxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T20:42:38.042Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Development of food aversions during illness

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 February 2007

Ilene L. Bernstein
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Washington, Seattle, WA 98195, USA
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Symposium on ‘Nutritional aspects of recovery from severe illness and surgery’
Copyright
Copyright © The Nutrition Society 1994

References

Andrykowski, M. A. & Otis, M. L. (1990). Development of learned food aversions in humans: Investigation in a ‘natural laboratory’ of cancer chemotherapy. Appetite 14, 145158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bermudez-Rattoni, F., Forthman, D. L., Sanchez, M. A., Perez, J. L. & Garcia, J. (1988). Odor and taste aversions conditioned in anesthetized rats. Behavioral Neuroscience 102, 726732.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bernstein, I. L. (1978). Learned taste aversions in children receiving chemotherapy. Science 200, 13021303.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bernstein, I. L. & Borson, S. (1986). Learned food aversion: A component of anorexia syndromes. Psychological Review 93, 462472.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bernstein, I. L. & Goehler, L. E. (1983). Chronic lithium chloride infusions: Conditioned suppression of food intake and preference. Behavioral Neuroscience 97, 290298.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bernstein, I. L., Goehler, L. E. & Fenner, D. P. (1984). Learned aversions to proteins in rats on a dietary self-selection regimen. Behavioral Neuroscience 98, 10651072.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bernstein, I. L. & Sigmundi, R. A. (1980). Tumor anorexia: A learned food aversion? Science 209, 416418.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bernstein, I. L., Treneer, C. M., Goehler, L. E. & Murowchick, E. (1985). Tumor growth in rats: Conditioned suppression of food intake and preference. Behavioral Neuroscience 99, 818830.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bernstein, I. L. & Webster, M. M. (1980). Learned taste aversions in humans. Physiology and Behavior 25, 363366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bovbjerg, D. H., Redd, W. H., Jacobsen, P. B., Manne, S. L., Taylor, K. L., Surbone, A., Crown, J. P., Norton, L., Gilewski, T. A., Hudis, C. A., Reichman, B. S., Kaufman, R. J., Currie, V. E. & Hakes, T. B. (1992). An experimental analysis of classically conditioned nausea during cancer chemotherapy. Psychosomatic Medicine 54, 623637.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brot, M. D., Braget, D. J. & Bernstein, I. L. (1987). Flavor, not postingestive, cues contribute to the salience of proteins as targets in aversion conditioning. Behavioral Neuroscience 101, 683689.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gamzu, E., Vincent, G. & Boff, E. (1985). A pharmacological perspective of drugs used in establishing conditioned food aversions. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 443, 231249.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garb, J. L. & Stunkard, A. J. (1974). Taste aversions in man. American Journal of Psychiatry 131, 12041207.Google ScholarPubMed
Garcia, J., Ervin, F. R. & Koelling, R. A. (1966). Learning with prolonged delay of reinforcement. Psychonomic Science 5, 121122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garcia, J., Hankins, W. G. & Rusiniak, K. W. (1974). Behavioral regulation of the milieu interne in man and rat. Science 185, 824831.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garcia, J. & Koelling, R. A. (1966). Relation of cue to consequence in avoidance learning. Psychonomic Science 4, 123124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kalat, J. W. & Rozin, P. (1973). ‘Learned safety’ as a mechanism in long-delay taste-aversion learning in rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 83, 198207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Logue, A. W. (1985). Conditioned food aversion learning in humans. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 443, 316329.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Logue, A. W., Ophir, I. & Strauss, K. E. (1981). The acquisition of taste aversions in humans. Behavioral Research and Therapy 19, 319333.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mattes, R. D., Arnold, C. & Boraas, M. (1987). Learned food aversions among cancer chemotherapy patients: Incidence, nature and clinical implications. Cancer 60, 25762580.3.0.CO;2-5>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Midkiff, E. E. & Bernstein, I. L. (1985). Targets of learned food aversions in humans. Physiology and Behavior 34, 839841.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palmerino, C. C., Rusiniak, K. W. & Garcia, J. (1980). Flavor illness aversions: The peculiar role of odor and taste in memory for poison. Science 208, 753755.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pelchat, M., Grill, H. J., Rozin, P. & Jacobs, J. (1983). Quality of acquired responses to tastes by rattus norvegicus depends on type of associated discomfort. Journal of Comparative Psychology 97, 140153.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pelchat, M. L. & Rozin, P. (1982). The special role of nausea in the acquisition of food dislikes by humans. Appetite 3, 341351.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Revusky, S. H. & Bedarf, E. W. (1967). Association of illness with prior ingestion of novel foods. Science 155, 219220.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roll, D. L. & Smith, J. C. (1972). Conditioned taste aversion in anesthetized rats. In Biological Boundaries of Learning, pp. 98102 [Seligman, M. E. P. and Hager, J. L., editors]. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Sahley, C. L., Gelperin, A. & Rudy, J. (1981). One-trial associative learning modifies food odor preferences of a terrestrial mollusc. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA 78, 640642.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed