Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-4rdpn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-20T02:02:46.968Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The relationship between obesity and cognitive health and decline

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 September 2017

Louise Dye
Affiliation:
Leeds Nutrition and Behaviour Group, School of Psychology, University of Leeds, Leeds LS2 9JT, UK
Neil Bernard Boyle*
Affiliation:
Leeds Nutrition and Behaviour Group, School of Psychology, University of Leeds, Leeds LS2 9JT, UK
Claire Champ
Affiliation:
Leeds Nutrition and Behaviour Group, School of Psychology, University of Leeds, Leeds LS2 9JT, UK
Clare Lawton
Affiliation:
Leeds Nutrition and Behaviour Group, School of Psychology, University of Leeds, Leeds LS2 9JT, UK
*
*Corresponding author: N. B. Boyle, fax +44 (0)113 3435749, email n.b.boyle@leeds.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The relationship between obesity and cognitive impairment is important given the globally ageing population in whom cognitive decline and neurodegenerative disorders will carry grave individual, societal and financial burdens. This review examines the evidence for the link between obesity and cognitive function in terms of both the immediate effects on cognitive performance, and effects on the trajectory of cognitive ageing and likelihood of dementia. In mid-life, there is a strong association between obesity and impaired cognitive function. Anthropometric measures of obesity are also associated with reduced neural integrity (e.g. grey and white matter atrophy). Increasing age coupled with the negative metabolic consequences of obesity (e.g. type 2 diabetes mellitus) are likely to significantly contribute to cognitive decline and incidence of dementia. Stress is identified as a potential risk factor promoting abdominal obesity and contributing to impaired cognitive function. However, the potentially protective effects of obesity against cognitive decline in older age require further examination. Finally, surgical and whole diet interventions, which address obesity may improve cognitive capacity and confer some protection against later cognitive decline. In conclusion, obesity and its comorbidities are associated with impaired cognitive performance, accelerated cognitive decline and neurodegenerative pathologies such as dementia in later life. Interventions targeting mid-life obesity may prove beneficial in reducing the cognitive risks associated with obesity.

Type
Conference on ‘Diet, nutrition and mental health and wellbeing’
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2017 

Ageing and cognitive decline

In 2011, 17·5 % of the European population was aged 65 years or older and this is expected to rise to 30 % by 2060( 1 ). The increasing life expectancy across the world necessitates urgent public health action aimed at preserving the physical and mental health status and autonomy of the elderly via optimal control of chronic diseases and a focus on the various dimensions of quality of life (physical, psychological, social). Ageing is accompanied, for a significant proportion of the population, by cognitive decline which is the primary risk factor for the development of neurodegenerative disorders, including Alzheimer's disease (AD)( Reference Yankner, Lu and Loerch 2 ). The 2015 World Alzheimer Report estimated 46·8 million cases of dementia in 2015 and projects that this number will double every 20 years( 3 ), underscoring the magnitude of this problem in terms of social and economic aspects, including towering costs of disease management, caregiver burden, loss of income and loss of productivity for the patient and the caregiver, and palliative and terminal care. Therefore, preserving normal cognitive capacities for as long as possible, along with improving knowledge about the preclinical phase in order to identify and target at-risk asymptomatic individuals, are urgent public health challenges( Reference Mortimer, Borenstein and Gosche 4 , Reference Amieva, Jacqmin-Gadda and Orgogozo 5 ).

There is a genetic component to dementia( Reference Karch, Cruchaga and Goate 6 ). ApoE is the strongest risk factor for late onset AD. ApoE encodes three common alleles (ε2, ε3, ε4). ApoEε4 is associated with increased AD risk in a cumulative fashion such that one ApoEε4 allele increases AD risk 3-fold, and two ApoEε4 alleles increase AD risk 12-fold. The presence of the ApoEε4 allele is associated with decrease in age at AD onset, whereas ApoEε2 is associated with decreased risk for AD and later age at onset. Other factors may also contribute to the development of dementia and factors which are modifiable require examination so that we can make health and lifestyle interventions to reduce the burden of dementia in the face of a globally ageing population.

Curative treatments or therapies aiming to at least decelerate cognitive decline are rather inefficient. At present, neuroscientists working in the field of AD indicate strong doubts that a cure will be found soon( Reference de la Torre 7 ). When significant cognitive loss has already taken place, the respective neuronal networks are likely irreversibly compromised and their replacement, if it were to become technically feasible, would not restore the individual's intellectual identity. Thus, prevention of cognitive decline is the only sensible approach to meet the challenge of an ageing and dementing population( Reference de la Torre 7 Reference Middleton and Yaffe 9 ). The onset of cognitive decline is evident from as early as 45 years of age with a 3·6 % decline in mental reasoning demonstrated in men and women aged 45–49( Reference Singh-Manoux, Kivimaki and Glymour 10 ). Evidence of such early decline highlights the importance of promoting healthy lifestyles in middle age, at the latest, in order to slow the trajectory of age related cognitive decline.

A confounding factor is the rising prevalence of obesity in Europe( 11 ), which is associated with impaired glucose tolerance (IGT), subsequent type 2 diabetes mellitus (T2DM) and low grade inflammation, culminating in metabolic syndrome, all of which are associated with cognitive impairment in the short- and long-term( Reference Gunstad, Lhotsky and Wendell 12 Reference Yaffe, Kanaya and Lindquist 15 ). Therefore, obesity and its comorbidities are risk factors associated with impaired cognitive performance and cognitive decline.

Obesity and cognitive function

The relationship between obesity and an increased risk of mortality and somatic morbidity is well documented( Reference Flegal, Kit and Orpana 16 ). An association between obesity and cognitive health is receiving increasing recognition. Mid-life obesity is a significant risk factor for developing AD and vascular dementia in later life( Reference Xu, Atti and Gatz 17 , Reference Whitmer, Gunderson and Barrett-Connor 18 ). In addition to an increased risk of an accelerated trajectory of cognitive ageing (discussed later), evidence suggests early to mid-adulthood obesity may have an immediate detrimental impact on cognitive functioning.

A negative association between anthropometric measures of obesity (e.g. BMI, waist circumference) and a number of cognitive domains has been reported. For example, obesity is related to impaired performance on tasks of episodic memory. Verbal learning, indexed by delayed recall and recognition of words, is impaired in those with higher v. lower BMI( Reference Cournot, Marquie and Ansiau 19 , Reference Gunstad, Paul and Cohen 20 ). Similar deficits are demonstrated for visual modality episodic memory tasks( Reference Cheke, Bonnici and Clayton 21 ). Impaired working memory performance has also been demonstrated in overweight and obese young adults compared with healthy weight controls( Reference Coppin, Nolan-Poupart and Jones-Gotman 22 ). However, some studies report no difference in memory performance between obese and non-obese individuals( Reference Conforto and Gershman 23 ).

Performance detriments are additionally evident in non-memory related cognitive domains. For example, impaired psychomotor( Reference Cournot, Marquie and Ansiau 19 ) and selective attention( Reference Fergenbaum, Bruce and Lou 24 ) performance have been reported in obese cohorts; although not consistently( Reference Boeka and Lokken 25 , Reference Gonzales, Tarumi and Miles 26 ). Performance decrements in the executive functions of concept formation and set-shifting, measured by performance on the Wisconsin card sorting test, are also shown in obese cohorts relative to normal weight comparison groups( Reference Fagundo, de la Torre and Jiménez-Murcia 27 , Reference Lokken, Boeka and Yellumahanthi 28 ).

Decision-making impairments exist across disordered eating populations; for example, anorexia nervosa( Reference Cavedini, Bassi and Ubbiali 29 ) and bulimia( Reference Boeka and Lokkenz 30 ). A number of studies have reported that decision-making performance is also altered in obese populations. Performance on the Iowa gambling task suggests morbidly obese individuals have a reduced capacity to maximise an immediate reward or programme a delayed reward( Reference Brogan, Hevey and O'Callaghan 31 ). Obese individuals also show impaired performance on additional tasks that require the delay of gratification (e.g. Delayed discounting task( Reference Davis, Patte and Curtis 32 )). Impaired processing of inter-temporal choices may contribute to poor diet choices in obese individuals (e.g. an increased sensitivity to the immediate reward of consuming highly palatable, energy dense foods while discounting the negative health and metabolic consequences in the long term). Such suboptimal decision-making processing can be considered a significant risk factor in an obesogenic environment in which effortful control of energy intake is critical for the maintenance of a healthy body weight.

A negative relationship between obesity and cognitive performance has not been consistently found, both within and across cognitive domains. Inconsistency in the evidence may be due to the potential moderating influence of a number of obesity-associated comorbidities known to adversely impact upon cognitive performance. This includes T2DM, hypertension, hypercholesterolaemia and insulin resistance, which may co-occur in individuals. Two recent systematic reviews( Reference Fitzpatrick, Gilbert and Serpell 33 , Reference Prickett, Brennan and Stolwyk 34 ) of the effects of obesity on cognitive performance in adults (aged 18–65 years) concluded that while there is evidence of impaired cognitive function in obese populations, there is insufficient evidence to date to confirm that these impairments are independent of obesity-related comorbidities and demographic variables (e.g. age, education). This lack of clarity is largely due to many studies failing to adequately control for potential confounding comorbidities

Effects of obesity on the brain

Brain imaging studies suggest that obesity is associated with neural atrophy. Structural alterations in the neural architecture of obese individuals have been reported. For example, elevated BMI is linked to decreased brain volume( Reference Ward, Carlsson and Trivedi 35 ), independent of age and morbidity( Reference Gunstad, Paul and Cohen 36 ). Increased BMI is also associated with grey matter atrophy in the temporal, frontal and occipital cortices, hippocampus, thalamus and midbrain( Reference Shefer, Marcus and Stern 37 ), and reduced integrity of white matter throughout the brain( Reference Verstynen, Weinstein and Schneider 38 ). It is important to note that it is not always possible to disentangle the contribution of obesity to these structural impairments from the effects of ageing and obesity-related comorbidities (e.g. hypertension, metabolic oversupply/dysregulation). However, obesity can be considered a considerable risk factor for reduced neural integrity.

In addition to structural and morphological alterations in the obese brain, neural imaging studies show altered functional activity. This includes decreased regional blood flow to the prefrontal cortex in healthy adults with elevated BMI( Reference Willeumier, Taylor and Amen 39 ). Significantly reduced functional activity in cortical areas associated with episodic memory (hippocampus, angular gyrus and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex) is associated with both obesity and insulin resistance( Reference Cheke, Bonnici and Clayton 21 ). Obese individuals also demonstrate lower working memory task related activation in the right parietal cortex( Reference Gonzales, Tarumi and Miles 26 ).

Obesity has been associated with an increase in brain age, in respect of cerebral white matter atrophy, where the greatest degree of atrophy has been identified in middle-age, equating to an estimated increase in brain age of 10 years. Importantly, middle-age may represent a critical period for brain ageing, where vulnerability to obesity is particularly acute compared with later life( Reference Ronan, Alexander-Bloch and Wagstyl 40 ). Indeed, white matter atrophy has been identified as starting in middle-age( Reference Fotenos, Snyder and Girton 41 ). The action of proinflammatory cytokines is a possible mechanism for these changes in white matter volume( Reference Ronan, Alexander-Bloch and Wagstyl 40 ). Adipose tissue secretes adipokines( Reference Kershaw and Flier 42 ), manifesting either pro- or anti-inflammatory properties( Reference Makki, Froguel and Wolowczuk 43 ). Importantly, in obesity, there is a reduction in adiponectin( Reference Kaser, Tatarczyk and Stadlmayr 44 ), which is known to protect against inflammation, cell proliferation and supports energy metabolism( Reference Nigro, Scudiero and Monaco 45 ), and upregulation of other adipokines, leading to a chronic inflammatory state and metabolic disease( Reference Ouchi, Parker and Lugus 46 ). Consequently, a host of microphysiological changes occur, which facilitate white matter abnormalities( Reference Bolzenius, Laidlaw and Cabeen 47 ).

Age, obesity and risk of dementia

The relationship between obesity and later cognitive decline is far from clear. There is increasing evidence that higher adiposity, both in respect of BMI and central obesity, in mid-life is a risk factor for subsequent impairment in cognitive functioning( Reference Whitmer, Gunderson and Barrett-Connor 18 , Reference Beydoun, Beydoun and Wang 48 , Reference Whitmer, Gustafson and Barrett-Connor 49 ), and has been identified as a modifiable risk factor for cognitive decline and impairment( Reference Baumgarta, Snyderb and Carrillob 50 ). However, obesity in later-life seems to offer a level of protection against cognitive decline, with some studies demonstrating slower decline in the obese compared with those of a normal weight( Reference Kim, Kim and Park 51 , Reference Hughes, Borenstein and Schofield 52 ).

Associations between age and risk level for later dementia show that obese individuals admitted to hospital between ages 30 and 39 years showed a 3·5 increased relative risk ratio for AD and vascular dementia in later life. This relative risk reduced in a stepped fashion up to age 70 years in those obese on admission but was still associated with a greater risk of subsequent dementia compared with non-obese controls. The risk of later vascular dementia was reduced in those obese from age 80 years onwards( Reference Wotton and Goldacre 53 ). It has been estimated that being obese at ages 40–45 years increases risk of later dementia by 74 % relative to those of a normal weight( Reference Whitmer, Gunderson and Barrett-Connor 18 ). Therefore, mid-life obesity increases risk of dementia in later-life while greater BMI at increased age may attenuate this risk. Thus, classification of obesity in septuagenarians appears to confer protection from dementia( Reference Keage, Gupta and Brayne 54 ). Obesity has been associated with the lowest odds ratio for dementia relative to diabetes, hypertension and dyslipidaemia in mid-(45–65 years) and late-life (above 65 years). However, the odds ratio is higher for mid-life relative to later-life obesity (2·0 and 0·8, respectively)( Reference Kloppenborg, van den Berg and Kappelle 55 ). In a recent systematic review and meta-regression analysis of longitudinal studies examining BMI in mid-life and risk of dementia in late life, being obese but not overweight in mid-life was found to lead to an increased risk of subsequent dementia( Reference Albanese, Launer and Egger 56 ). Furthermore, a review of empirical research found that the relationship between obesity and risk of dementia was most consistent when obesity was assessed during mid-life and cognition was assessed in later-life, and in studies where there were longer follow-up periods( Reference Kloppenborg, van den Berg and Kappelle 55 ).

Importantly, the association between obesity and dementia has been shown to vary as a function of adiposity measure, the outcome of interest and the age at which an individual is classed as obese( Reference Wotton and Goldacre 53 , Reference Luchsinger, Patel and Tang 57 , Reference Luchsinger and Gustafson 58 ). Indeed, using BMI as a measure of adiposity is problematic given that BMI fails to distinguish muscle from adipose tissue and does not indicate distribution of adiposity( Reference Stevens, McClain and Truesdale 59 ). Longitudinal studies have the benefit of following cohorts of individuals over prolonged periods of time, making them an ideal approach for observing disease development( Reference Caruana, Roman and Hernández-Sánchez 60 ) and identifying patterns, correlates and possible causes of changes that occur with age( Reference Zelinski and Burnight 61 ). In one such study, individuals aged 65 years and over were followed for 5 years to assess the prospective association between adiposity, weight change and dementia, as a function of age. Importantly, findings were adjusted for sample demographics and ApoE ε4. BMI between 26·3 and 29·6 kg/m2 was related to a lower risk of dementia including AD compared with BMI <23·4 kg/m2. Waist circumference >97 cm was associated with an increased risk of dementia associated with stroke. However, age mediated the effects of BMI and waist circumference. In individuals below age 76 years BMI between 23·4 and 29·6 kg/m2 was associated with lower risk of dementia, with a higher risk for those with BMI outside this range. In individuals over age 76 years, risk of dementia decreased with increasing BMI. Waist circumference >97 cm was associated with a higher risk of dementia and AD in those below age 76 years, but this relationship disappeared for those aged over 76 years. Weight loss was related to increased dementia risk relative to those who remained weight stable. However, those that gained weight had a higher risk of dementia associated with stroke( Reference Luchsinger, Patel and Tang 57 ).

Ageing, obesity and impairment in specific cognitive domains

Impaired verbal fluency, memory and performance on global screening measures have also been related to obesity; demonstrating an independent relationship between cognitive performance and obesity. Individuals aged 19–93 years were assessed on average 3·1 times, every 2–3 years. On measures of global cognitive function, higher BMI, waist circumference and waist-to-hip ratio (WHR) were associated with poorer performance; poorer performance was seen with increasing age in cases of larger waist circumference and WHR. Executive function was assessed by trail making tests A and B( Reference Gunstad, Lhotsky and Wendell 12 ). Both tests measure mental flexibility, speed of processing and visual search( Reference Tombaugh 62 ). With increasing age, higher BMI and waist circumference were related to quicker performance on trail making tests A( Reference Gunstad, Lhotsky and Wendell 12 ), where twenty-five numbered circles are connected sequentially( Reference Tombaugh 62 ). However, higher WHR related to slower performance with age on trail making tests B( Reference Gunstad, Lhotsky and Wendell 12 ), where numbers and letters are connected in order in an alternate fashion (e.g. 1-A, 2-B, etc.)( Reference Tombaugh 62 ). Similarly, increasing obesity as identified on all three obesity measures and ageing were associated with declining performance in visual memory. The relationship between verbal functioning and obesity varied depending upon the measure used but did not vary with age. Specifically, poorer performance for letter and category fluency was related to BMI, waist circumference was related to poorer performance for letter fluency and WHR was related to poorer performance for category fluency. Only WHR was associated with visuospatial skills with increasing age, whereby a higher WHR was related to slower decline( Reference Gunstad, Lhotsky and Wendell 12 ). Similar relationships have been found between weight change and cognitive performance. In a second prospective study, BMI was calculated across the adult lifetime (early adulthood, and early and late mid-life). Executive function, memory and performance on the mini-mental state examination were examined in late mid-life. Adjusting for age, sex and education, cumulative obesity was related to poorer performance on the mini-mental state examination and in inductive reasoning and verbal fluency (phonemic and semantic) compared with normal weight. The finding for mini-mental state examination performance remained following further adjustment for health behaviours and health measures assessed in late mid-life. Conversely, cognition was also associated with the cumulative effects of being underweight (BMI < 20). Being underweight on two or three occasions was related to lower inductive reasoning, verbal fluency and mini-mental state examination performance also( Reference Sabia, Kivimaki and Shipley 63 ). Being underweight and a reduction in weight may reflect the onset of pre-clinical dementia, which has consequences of lowered food intake and changes in lifestyle( Reference Besser, Gill and Monsell 64 , Reference Gu, Scarmeas and Cosentino 65 ). This notion of cognitive dysfunction promoting weight loss prior to clinical significance has been supported in a review of nineteen cohort studies( Reference Albanese, Launer and Egger 56 ).

Metabolic consequences of obesity

Adiposity is a risk factor for a range of health conditions including vascular comorbidities, which in themselves increase the risk of dementia. Chronic, low-grade inflammation associated with obesity has been linked to insulin resistance( Reference Felice and Ferreira 66 ). Proinflammatory cytokines have been suggested to cause insulin resistance in liver and adipose tissue by interfering with insulin signalling. Specifically, both autocrine/paracrine cytokine signalling and endocrine cytokine signalling have been implicated in localised and systemic insulin resistance( Reference de Luca and Olefsky 67 ). Insulin resistance is central to the metabolic syndrome, which has been associated with an increased risk of cognitive decline and dementia( Reference Yaffe, Kanaya and Lindquist 15 , Reference Kim and Feldman 68 Reference Felice, Lourenco and Ferreira 70 ). A number of consequences of insulin resistance have been posited including, but not restricted to, lipid metabolism and mitochondrial dysfunction, white matter atrophy, and synaptic loss and neuro-inflammation( Reference de la Monte 71 , Reference Mullins, Mustapic and Goetzl 72 ). Further, the administration of insulin for the treatment of insulin resistance has produced promising results in alleviating cognitive impairment in cognitively healthy and unhealthy samples; however, treatment effects have been found to be modified by dose and ApoE genotype( Reference Shemesh, Rudich and Harman-Boehm 73 ). For example, improvements have been found in areas such as visuospatial and verbal working memory( Reference Claxton, Baker and Hanson 74 , Reference Reger, Watson and Green 75 ).

Type 2 diabetes and cognitive function

Insulin resistance is predictive of subsequent development of T2DM( Reference Taylor 76 ). This may reflect part of a sequential relationship, which features adiposity and hyperinsulinaemia as appearing in a linear fashion prior to the onset of glucose intolerance and T2DM( Reference Luchsinger and Gustafson 77 ).

IGT occurs prior to the development of diabetes and may contribute to cognitive impairments (see( Reference Lamport, Lawton and Mansfield 78 ) for a review). Recent studies have shown that the performance of ostensibly healthy middle-aged women with IGT was impaired in cognitive tasks, which predominantly engage the hippocampus( Reference Lamport, Chadwick and Dye 79 ). IGT in later life may confer a greater risk for AD than T2DM( Reference Xu, Atti and Gatz 17 ) because IGT is likely to be untreated and result in longer exposure to glucose excursions. Indeed, higher glucose levels may increase the risk of cognitive dysfunction in non-diabetic individuals( Reference Crane, Walker and Hubbard 80 , Reference Anstey, Sargent-Cox and Eramudugolla 81 ).

T2DM not only increases the risk of subsequent cognitive dysfunction( Reference Vagelatos and Eslick 82 , Reference Cheng, Huang and Deng 83 ), but may also lead to an accelerated rate of cognitive ageing( Reference Cukierman, Gerstein and Williamson 84 , Reference Kanaya, Barrett-Connor and Gildengorin 85 ). T2DM has been associated with reduced performance in a number of cognitive domains including verbal memory, processing speed( Reference Awad, Gagnon and Messier 86 , Reference Messier 87 ), attention( Reference Fontbonne, Berr and Ducimetière 88 ), spatial working memory( Reference Huang, Jia and Xie 89 ), verbal fluency( Reference Kanaya, Barrett-Connor and Gildengorin 85 ) and executive function( Reference Abbatecola, Paolisso and Lamponi 90 ). However, glycaemic control may play a role in determining the extent to which individuals experience cognitive impairment( Reference Munshi, Capelson and Grande 91 , Reference Ravona-Springer, Heymann and Schmeidler 92 ).

T2DM is an independent risk factor for the development of CVD( Reference Martín-Timón, Sevillano-Collantes and Segura-Galindo 93 ). Similarly, hyperinsulinaemia and insulin resistance may lead to an increased risk of CVD( Reference Mitsuhashi, Hibi and Kosuge 94 , Reference Patel, Rawal and Bagchi 95 ). Cardiovascular risk factors present in mid-life increase the risk of subsequent dementia( Reference Whitmer, Sidney and Selby 96 ). Hypoperfusion and microemboli, both a consequence of cardiac disease, have been implicated in the aetiology of dementia( Reference de la Torre 97 ). Additionally, multiple lacunar infarctions are common in individuals with diabetes and these have been associated with cognitive decline( Reference Umemura, Kawamura and Hotta 98 ). Further, T2DM has been associated with global brain atrophy( Reference Wisse, de Bresser and Geerlings 99 ), with the rate of loss being greater than that found in normal ageing( Reference Kooistra, Geerlings and Mali 100 ), supporting the notion of accelerated cognitive ageing. Moreover, the abnormalities present in insulin resistance in T2DM have also been observed in those with AD( Reference Moloney, Griffin and Timmons 101 , Reference Talbot, Wang and Kazi 102 ). As insulin action has been implicated in neuronal and synaptic formation, development, repair and neuroprotection( Reference Cohen, Tong and Wands 103 , Reference Apostolatos, Song and Acosta 104 ), this has important clinical consequences.

Since obesity and subsequent T2DM increases the risk of AD by 65 % (relative risk in T2DM is 1·46( Reference Caruana, Roman and Hernández-Sánchez 60 )) and about 80 % of AD patients have problems with glycaemic control, AD has been referred to as type-3 diabetes( Reference Zelinski and Burnight 61 ). It has been proposed that AD is a metabolic disease, mediated by impairments in brain insulin responsiveness, glucose utilisation and energy metabolism, which leads to increased oxidative stress, and inflammation, which worsens insulin resistance( Reference de la Monte 105 ). Advanced glycation end-products are also elevated in both T2DM and AD. The relative risk of vascular dementia in those with T2DM is 2·49( Reference Cheng, Huang and Deng 83 ) and its development relates to a history of hypertension and disturbances in cerebral blood flow.

Further evidence for the potential role of insulin resistance in dementia comes from evidence that an increase in enzymes responsible for the generation of β-amyloid, as well as increased levels of β-amyloid in the brain, have been identified following induced insulin resistance in animal studies( Reference Vandal, White and Tremblay 106 , Reference Son, Song and Byun 107 ). Additionally, impairments in insulin signalling contribute to impairments and dysfunction in mitochondrial structure and function due to energy deficiency, having consequences for increased reactive oxygen species production( Reference Butterfield, Di Domenico and Barone 108 ) and neuropathology in AD( Reference Felice and Ferreira 66 ).

The obesity paradox

It should also be noted that being underweight in middle and old age has recently been associated with an increased dementia risk. The incidence of dementia decreased as a function of increasing BMI. Paradoxically, morbid obesity in adult life was associated with a 29 % lower dementia risk compared to healthy weight( Reference Qizilbash, Gregson and Johnson 109 ). In older adults, current obesity levels have been inversely associated with dementia( Reference Fitzpatrick, Kuller and Lopez 110 ). This could represent an ‘obesity paradox’ in which late life weight loss may precede dementia( Reference Hughes, Borenstein and Schofield 52 ) and occur before any presentation of cognitive impairment. A recent retrospective cohort study of almost two million individuals aged over 40 years in the UK, reported that being underweight in middle age and old age carries an increased risk of dementia( Reference Qizilbash, Gregson and Johnson 109 ). This assertion is controversial and, in contrast to the evidence of an association between obesity and dementia, may reflect the tendency to underdiagnose dementia by general practitioners at the time the data were collected, and over or under adjustment for a number of factors such as competing risk of mortality as well as selection bias, and bias in the diagnosis of dementia in those with lower BMI/age( Reference Harrison and Shenkin 111 ).

Collectively, these findings may go some way to explain the relationship between mid-life obesity and late-life dementia. There is less evidence available to explain the unexpected finding that obesity in late life confers protection against dementia. However, possible mechanisms include larger leg lean mass promoting glucose metabolism( Reference Snijder, Dekker and Visser 112 ), which could avoid the pathogenic consequences of increased glucose availability via glucose uptake into muscle.

Stress as contributing risk factor

Stress is experienced when an individual perceives a mismatch between the demands of a stressor and their ability to cope. In homeostatic terms: demand exceeds the regulatory capacity of the organism( Reference Chrousos 113 ). The stress response is primarily mediated by two neuroendocrine systems: the sympathetic adrenal–medullary system and the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis. Activation of these systems ultimately results in the release of corticosteroids (via the sympathetic adrenal-medullary system) and glucocorticoids (via the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis) which instigate adaptive survival responses to meet the demands of the stressor. While stress responses are adaptive, prolonged, excessive, or repeated response activation can result in a cumulative toll on the organism which accelerates wear and tear on bodily systems( Reference McEwen, McCann, Lipton, Sternberg, Chrousos, Gold and Smith 114 ). Chronic psychosocial stress drives physiological dysregulation that has been associated with multiple and profound negative effects on human health and well-being, ultimately affecting quality and longevity of life( Reference McEwen, Yehuda and McEwen 115 , Reference McEwen 116 ).

Stress, energy homeostasis and metabolic outcomes

Exposure to stress is associated with both metabolic dysfunction( Reference Kyrou, Chrousos, Tsigos, Chrousos and Tsigos 117 ) and impairment of cognitive performance( Reference Lupien, Fiocco and Wan 118 ). Therefore, stress is a potentially important risk factor contributing to the relationship between obesity and cognitive function. There is considerable neurobiological overlap between stress and energy homeostasis systems. The hypothalamus is sensitive to the negative feedback action of glucocorticoids, and also to energy balance and appetite hormones (e.g. insulin, ghrelin, leptin)( Reference Sinha and Jastreboff 119 ). Furthermore, the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis is sensitive to most central and peripheral neuropeptides involved in energy homeostasis and appetite (e.g. orexigenic neuropeptide Y( Reference Rohleder and Kirschbaum 120 )). Chronic stress also modifies peripheral metabolic and adipose physiology. For example, cortisol (the primary human glucocorticoid) increases plasma levels of leptin and ghrelin and alters the expression of neuropeptides that regulate energy intake( Reference Maniam and Morris 121 ). Glucocorticoids also inhibit insulin release and decrease insulin sensitivity promoting metabolic oversupply which contributes to the development of hypertension, central obesity and glucose intolerance, key features of metabolic syndrome( Reference Picard, Juster and McEwen 122 ).

The relationship between psychosocial stress and negative metabolic outcomes is gaining increasing recognition. Chronic stress prospectively predicts abdominal fat accumulation( Reference Marniemi, Kronholm and Aunola 123 ), metabolic syndrome( Reference Pyykkonen, Raikkonen and Tuomi 124 ), and obesity( Reference Brunner, Chandola and Marmot 125 ). Stress promotes irregular eating patterns and physical inactivity and can bias food preferences towards high-energy dense food( Reference Picard, Juster and McEwen 122 ). For example, stress increases the intake of sweet high-fat foods( Reference Oliver, Wardle and Gibson 126 ), fast-food( Reference Steptoe, Lipsey and Wardle 127 ), a high-fat diet( Reference Ng and Jeffery 128 ), unhealthy snacking( Reference Conner, Fitter and Fletcher 129 , Reference Oliver and Wardle 130 ), binge eating( Reference Gluck, Geliebter and Hung 131 ) and reduces vegetable intake( Reference O'Connor, Jones and Conner 132 ).

Existing metabolic risk may increase vulnerability to the negative effects of stress on body composition. For example, cumulative stress is associated with higher fasted glucose, insulin and insulin resistance in those with high v. low BMI( Reference Sinha and Jastreboff 119 ). Obese individuals are also more vulnerable to being exposed to elevated glucocorticoid levels. Central obesity is associated with glucocorticoid excess( Reference Bjorntorp and Rosmond 133 ), elevated basal cortisol( Reference Rosmond and Bjorntorp 134 ), and higher cortisol reactivity to acute stress exposure( Reference Epel, McEwen and Seeman 135 ).

Such findings suggest stress promotes an internal milieu and behaviours that increase the risk of metabolic oversupply which have grave long-term consequences for health. Conditions characterised by metabolic oversupply (e.g. obesity and diabetes mellitus) are associated with increased oxidative stress, systemic inflammation, altered gene expression (e.g. shortening of telomeres) and impaired cognitive performance( Reference Picard and Turnbull 136 ).

Stress and cognitive function

Stress significantly impacts upon cognitive function acutely and chronically via deleterious effects on neural structures. The acute effects of stress on performance are bidirectional with examples of both enhanced and impaired function. The direction of the effect is mediated by a number of variables, including cognitive domain, proximity of stress to cognitive processes and individual stress responsivity( Reference Wolf 137 , Reference Lupien, Maheu and Tu 138 ). Stress tends to impair cognitive processes that are not directly relevant to the stressor faced. For example, attentional resources needed to process the stressor faced are prioritised. Similarly, priority is given to memory consolidation of information likely to permit future adaptive coping. Cognitive processes extraneous to the immediate threat (e.g. peripheral attention, retrieval of non-stress relevant information) tend to be impaired. Glucocorticoids have been identified as the primary moderator of the acute effects of stress on cognitive function. Glucocorticoids are also associated with impairments to neural integrity in the long term. For example, chronically raised plasma levels are negatively correlated with hippocampal volume and hippocampal-dependent memory deficits in older adults( Reference Lupien, de Leon and de Santi 139 ). Evidence also suggests that glucocorticoids exert negative effects upon the integrity and function of neurons in the prefrontal cortex( Reference Dai, Buijs and Swaab 140 ).

Stress, obesity and cognitive function

Stress can promote the accumulation of excessive weight, particularly central adiposity, via alterations to energy homeostasis systems and feeding behaviour. Further, obese individuals may be more vulnerable to the cognitive impairing effects of stress due to increased basal and reactive glucocorticoid levels. The combination of increased vulnerability to the deleterious effects of stress, and the risk associated with existing metabolic oversupply that characterises the obese state, suggests obese individuals may be more vulnerable to impaired cognition under conditions of stress. Our laboratory examined the impact of stress exposure on cognitive performance in centrally obese, middle-aged adults. Cortisol responsivity and cognitive performance were assessed after exposure to a laboratory psychosocial stressor or non-stress control in sixty-six high or low WHR adults. Males, particularly of high WHR, tended to exhibit greater cortisol responsivity in response to the stressor. Exposure to the stressor and increasing WHR were associated with poorer performance on tasks of declarative memory; specifically spatial recognition memory and paired associates learning (Cambridge automated neuropsychological test battery( Reference Lasikiewicz, Hendrickx and Talbot 141 )). Our findings tentatively suggest a reduction in cognitive performance in those with central adiposity under conditions of acute stress. Therefore, the increased risk of impaired cognition evident in obese populations may be exacerbated by an increased vulnerability to the negative effects of stress.

Can obesity related deficits in cognitive function be reversed?

Weight loss via diet and/or exercise is advisable to reduce obesity and there is some evidence that such interventions may also restore or prevent further decline in cognitive function. Weight loss maintenance requires enduring behaviour change and is likely to be more successful in those who have the cognitive capacity to do this. Some interventions such as bariatric surgery promote rapid loss, while others such as dietary change result in slower body weight reduction. There have been a number of studies which evaluate cognitive function following these interventions.

Bariatric surgery and cognitive function

Interventions which address cerebrovascular risk factors during middle age may be prophylactic for cognitive ageing. One such intervention is bariatric surgery which has been shown to promote rapid improvements in memory and executive function that persist for several years postoperatively( Reference Spitznagel, Hawkins and Alosco 142 ). This post-operative improvement in memory performance is not seen in individuals with a family history of AD( Reference Alosco, Spitznagel and Strain 143 ), which suggests that genetic vulnerability or family history may attenuate cognitive recovery post bariatric surgery.

Dietary intervention studies and cognitive function

Whilst the effects of specific nutrients on cognitive performance have been investigated experimentally( Reference Hoyland, Lawton and Dye 144 , Reference Lamport, Dye and Wightman 145 ), comparatively less is known about the effects of whole dietary patterns which more accurately reflect the complexity of daily eating behaviour as well as the synergistic effects of nutrients in the food matrix.( Reference Jacobs, Gross and Tapsell 146 ). Experimentally controlled whole diet approaches which reduce postprandial glucose excursions and inflammation by increasing the fibre content of the diet in line with Nordic Nutrition Recommendations have demonstrated cognitive benefits within as little as 1 month in middle-aged adults( Reference Nilsson, Tovar and Johansson 147 ). The Dietary Approaches to Stop Hypertension diet, which is high in fruit and vegetables and low-fat dairy foods and low in saturated fat, showed a positive effect on psychomotor performance in 124 middle-aged adults with hypertension with diet alone( Reference Smith, Blumenthal and Babyak 148 ).

Consumption of a Mediterranean dietary pattern (MDP) rich in olive oil, fruit and vegetables, whole grains, legumes, nuts, low-fat dairy, fish, moderate alcohol (red wine) and low red meat intake has been associated with a reduced risk of pathology and mortality in the general population( Reference Sofi, Macchi and Abbate 149 ). This dietary pattern has been identified as a healthy model of eating that should be promoted in non-Mediterranean populations( 150 , 151 ). The MDP has also been investigated in relation to cognitive health, decline and dementia. Epidemiological, prospective( Reference Tangney, Kwasny and Li 152 Reference Tsivgoulis, Judd and Letter 156 ), cross-sectional( Reference Samieri, Grodstein and Rosner 157 Reference Scarmeas, Stern and Mayeux 160 ), and meta-analytic studies( Reference Singh, Parsaik and Mielke 161 Reference Sofi, Abbate and Gensini 163 ) suggest that adherence to this dietary pattern is associated with less cognitive decline, dementia and AD; although with some disparity amongst the findings( Reference Samieri, Grodstein and Rosner 157 , Reference Kesse-Guyot, Andreeva and Lassale 164 , Reference Cherbuin and Anstey 165 ).

The Prevencion con Dieta Mediterranea( Reference Valls-Pedret, Sala-Vila and Serra-Mir 166 ) study supplemented older Spanish adults (mean age 67 years) prescribed the MDP with either extra virgin olive oil or mixed nuts and compared cognitive function after 4 years with a non-intervention control who received advice to reduce dietary fat. Better verbal memory was found in those consuming olive oil than controls, and composite memory, frontal and global performance was maintained in both MDP arms relative to the controls whose performance declined over the follow-up period. Although cardiovascular risk was also reduced, no synergistic mechanism of the whole MDP definitively explains the maintenance of cognitive function observed. However, different biological mechanisms have been proposed. These include reduction of vascular risk factors and white matter lesions, metabolic abnormalities (e.g. insulin resistance), oxidative stress, inflammation and advanced glycation end products( Reference Singh, Parsaik and Mielke 161 Reference Sofi, Abbate and Gensini 163 ).

Limitations of studies examining the association between obesity and dementia

The measure of adiposity employed to indicate obesity influences the relationship found with risk of dementia. The lack of differentiation between muscle and fat tissue in BMI measurement makes this a problematic assessment of body fat( Reference Stevens, McClain and Truesdale 59 ), which also varies in relation to age and sex. For example, in a cross-sectional validation study, females had a greater percentage of body fat relative to males despite having the same BMI. Furthermore, race, age and race-by-BMI interaction were independently associated with the percentage of body fat for females( Reference Jackson, Stanforth and Gagnon 167 ). The use of self-report data for weight and height at an earlier time of life may be affected by recall bias( Reference Fitzpatrick, Kuller and Lopez 110 ). Length of follow-up period also makes drawing conclusions regarding risk factors difficult when this is limited. Indeed, in those studies that included a longer follow-up time, the association between obesity and risk of dementia has been found to be most consistent( Reference Kloppenborg, van den Berg and Kappelle 55 ). Heterogeneity in study designs further contributes to the difficulty of forming conclusions about relative risk. In a meta-analysis assessing risk of AD in obesity, diabetes and related disorders, differences in study designs may have contributed to statistical heterogeneity seen in the pooled effect size for obesity. Moreover, no conclusion could be made regarding the effects of the timing of exposure to obesity on risk. Additionally, a lack of adjustment for ApoE status in some studies does not reflect the potential of this to modify the relationship between obesity and dementia risk( Reference Profenno, Porsteinsson and Faraone 168 ). Other studies have failed to adjust for other important variables in their analyses including education, cerebrovascular damage and stroke. Also, significant attrition rates are not addressed in some longitudinal studies. Importantly, a meta-regression revealed the association between being underweight in mid-life and risk of later dementia was more likely to have been reported in studies prone to outcome ascertainment bias, selection bias, involving shorter follow-up periods, suffering a greater rate of attrition and where control of potential confounding variables was less adequate( Reference Albanese, Launer and Egger 56 ). Differences in the diagnostic criteria of dementia and diabetes, in the length of follow-up periods, in the sample sizes and in the recruitment of specific populations have all been reported to contribute to heterogeneity in study findings( Reference Gudala, Bansal and Schifano 169 ). Similarly, wide age ranges in respect of study samples and the inclusion of some participants above age 65 years when assessing mid-life exposure further complicates the issues( Reference Albanese, Launer and Egger 56 ). The lack of inclusion of brain imaging outcomes and autopsy reports means that it is not possible to determine how the severity of subclinical vascular disease features in the risk for dementia with obesity( Reference Profenno, Porsteinsson and Faraone 168 ). Cohort effects are also acknowledged in some studies, such as differences in the survival rate due to dementia, obesity-related mortality( Reference Xu, Atti and Gatz 17 ) or perhaps being less vulnerable to the adverse effects of obesity, termed the ‘survivor effect’( Reference Kloppenborg, van den Berg and Kappelle 55 ).

Conclusions

There is clearly an association between mid-life obesity and cognition and cognitive decline in later life. This is an issue of concern against the background of an ageing population and the failure to stem the rise in obesity. The impact of obesity on cognitive decline and risk of dementia merits further investigation. Investigations into the association between obesity and cognitive health should also consider the contributing effects of psychosocial stress on this relationship. More consistent methodological approaches will likely reduce some of the heterogeneity in the existing data. For example, the selection of an appropriate and reliable measure of obesity, adjusting for important variables (e.g. ApoE status, education, cardiovascular risk) and adequately accounting for attrition. Interventions which reduce obesity are promoted for physical health reasons but the demonstration that weight loss and weight loss maintenance at mid-life can prevent cognitive decline may present a persuasive message to the middle aged who generally fear dementia.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank the manuscript reviewers for their time and helpful suggestions to improve the review.

Financial Support

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.

Conflicts of Interest

None.

Authorship

References

1. Eurostat (2014) Population Projections. http://epp.eurostat.ec.europa.eu (accessed June 2015).Google Scholar
2. Yankner, BA, Lu, T & Loerch, P (2008) The aging brain. Ann Rev Pathol: Mech Dis 3, 4166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3. Alzheimer's Disease International (2015) World Alzheimer Report 2015. The Global Impact of Dementia: An analysis of prevalence, incidence, cost and trends. https://www.alz.co.uk/research/WorldAlzheimerReport2015.pdf (accessed June 2017).Google Scholar
4. Mortimer, JA, Borenstein, AR, Gosche, KM et al. (2005) Very early detection of Alzheimer neuropathology and the role of brain reserve in modifying its clinical expression. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 18, 218223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5. Amieva, H, Jacqmin-Gadda, H, Orgogozo, JM et al. (2005) The 9 year cognitive decline before dementia of the Alzheimer type: a prospective population-based study. Brain 128, 10931101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6. Karch, CM, Cruchaga, C & Goate, A (2014) Alzheimer's disease genetics: from the bench to the clinic. Neuron 83, 1126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
7. de la Torre, JC (2010) Alzheimer's disease is incurable but preventable. J Alzheimers Dis 20, 861870.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8. Coley, N, Andrieu, S, Gardette, V et al. (2008) Dementia prevention: methodological explanations for inconsistent results. Epidemiol Rev 30, 3566.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
9. Middleton, LE & Yaffe, K (2009) Promising strategies for the prevention of dementia. Arch Neurol 66, 12101215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
10. Singh-Manoux, A, Kivimaki, M, Glymour, MM et al. (2012) Timing of onset of cognitive decline: results from Whitehall II prospective cohort study. BMJ 344, d7622.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
11. WHO Europe (2014) European Food and Nutrition Action Plan 2015–2020. http://www.euro.who.int/__data/assets/pdf_file/0008/253727/64wd14e_FoodNutAP_140426.pdf (accessed June 2017).Google Scholar
12. Gunstad, J, Lhotsky, A, Wendell, CR et al. (2010) Longitudinal examination of obesity and cognitive function: results from the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging. Neuroepidemiology 34, 222229.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
13. Lamport, DJ, Dye, L, Mansfield, MW et al. (2013) Acute glycaemic load breakfast manipulations do not attenuate cognitive impairments in adults with type 2 diabetes. Clin Nutr 32, 265272.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
14. Lamport, DJ, Lawton, CL, Mansfield, MW et al. (2014) Type 2 diabetes and impaired glucose tolerance are associated with word memory source monitoring recollection deficits but not simple recognition familiarity deficits following water, low glycaemic load, and high glycaemic load breakfasts. Physiol Behav 124, 5460.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
15. Yaffe, K, Kanaya, A, Lindquist, K et al. (2004) The metabolic syndrome, inflammation, and risk of cognitive decline. J Am Med Assoc 292, 22372242.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
16. Flegal, KM, Kit, BK, Orpana, H et al. (2013) Association of all-cause mortality with overweight and obesity using standard body mass index categories: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA 309, 7182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
17. Xu, WL, Atti, AR, Gatz, M et al. (2011) Midlife overweight and obesity increase late-life dementia risk: a population-based twin study. Neurology 76, 15681574.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
18. Whitmer, RA, Gunderson, EP, Barrett-Connor, E et al. (2005) Obesity in middle age and future risk of dementia: a 27 year longitudinal population based study. BMJ 330, 13601362.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
19. Cournot, M, Marquie, JC, Ansiau, D et al. (2006) Relation between body mass index and cognitive function in healthy middle-aged men and women. Neurology 67, 12081214.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
20. Gunstad, J, Paul, RH, Cohen, RA et al. (2006) Obesity is associated with memory deficits in young and middle-aged adults. Eat Weight Disord 11, e15e19.Google Scholar
21. Cheke, LG, Bonnici, HM, Clayton, NS et al. (2017) Obesity and insulin resistance are associated with reduced activity in core memory regions of the brain. Neuropsychologia 96, 137149.Google Scholar
22. Coppin, G, Nolan-Poupart, S, Jones-Gotman, M et al. (2014) Working memory and reward association learning impairments in obesity. Neuropsychologia 65, 146155.Google Scholar
23. Conforto, RM & Gershman, L (1985) Cognitive processing differences between obese and nonobese subjects. Addict Behav 10, 8385.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
24. Fergenbaum, JH, Bruce, S, Lou, W et al. (2009) Obesity and lowered cognitive performance in a Canadian first nations population. Obesity 17, 19571963.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
25. Boeka, AG & Lokken, KL (2008) Neuropsychological performance of a clinical sample of extremely obese individuals. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 23, 467474.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
26. Gonzales, MM, Tarumi, T, Miles, SC et al. (2010) Insulin sensitivity as a mediator of the relationship between BMI and working memory-related brain activation. Obesity (Silver Spring) 18, 21312137.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
27. Fagundo, AB, de la Torre, R, Jiménez-Murcia, S et al. (2012) Executive functions profile in extreme eating/weight conditions: from anorexia nervosa to obesity. PLoS ONE 7, e43382.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
28. Lokken, KL, Boeka, AG, Yellumahanthi, K et al. (2010) Cognitive performance of morbidly obese patients seeking bariatric surgery. Am Surg 76, 5559.Google Scholar
29. Cavedini, P, Bassi, T, Ubbiali, A et al. (2004) Neuropsychological investigation of decision-making in anorexia nervosa. Psychiatry Res 127, 259266.Google Scholar
30. Boeka, AG & Lokkenz, KL (2006) The Iowa gambling task as a measure of decision making in women with bulimia nervosa. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 12, 741745.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
31. Brogan, A, Hevey, D, O'Callaghan, G et al. (2011) Impaired decision making among morbidly obese adults. J Psychosom Res 70, 189196.Google Scholar
32. Davis, C, Patte, K, Curtis, C et al. (2010) Immediate pleasures and future consequences. A neuropsychological study of binge eating and obesity. Appetite 54, 208213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
33. Fitzpatrick, S, Gilbert, S & Serpell, L (2013) Systematic review: are overweight and obese individuals impaired on behavioural tasks of executive functioning? Neuropsychol Rev 23, 138156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
34. Prickett, C, Brennan, L & Stolwyk, R (2015) Examining the relationship between obesity and cognitive function: a systematic literature review. Obes Res Clin Pract 9, 93113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
35. Ward, MA, Carlsson, CM, Trivedi, MA et al. (2005) The effect of body mass index on global brain volume in middle-aged adults: a cross sectional study. BMC Neurol 5, article 23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
36. Gunstad, J, Paul, RH, Cohen, RA et al. (2008) Relationship between body mass index and brain volume in healthy adults. Int J Neurosci 118, 15821593.Google Scholar
37. Shefer, G, Marcus, Y & Stern, N (2013) Is obesity a brain disease? Neurosci Biobehav Rev 37, 24892503.Google Scholar
38. Verstynen, TD, Weinstein, AM, Schneider, WW et al. (2012) Increased body mass index is associated with a global and distributed decrease in white matter microstructural integrity. Psychosom Med 74, 682690.Google Scholar
39. Willeumier, KC, Taylor, DV & Amen, DG (2011) Elevated BMI is associated with decreased blood flow in the prefrontal cortex using SPECT imaging in healthy adults. Obesity 19, 10951097.Google Scholar
40. Ronan, L, Alexander-Bloch, AF, Wagstyl, K et al. (2016) Obesity associated with increased brain age from midlife. Neurobiol Aging 47, 6370.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
41. Fotenos, AF, Snyder, AZ, Girton, LE et al. (2005) Normative estimates of cross-sectional and longitudinal brain volume decline in aging and AD. Neurology 64, 10321039.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
42. Kershaw, EE & Flier, JS (2004) Adipose tissue as an endocrine organ. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 89, 25482556.Google Scholar
43. Makki, K, Froguel, P & Wolowczuk, I (2013) Adipose tissue in obesity-related inflammation and insulin resistance: cells, cytokines, and chemokines. ISRN Inflam 2013: 139239.Google ScholarPubMed
44. Kaser, S, Tatarczyk, T, Stadlmayr, A et al. (2008) Effect of obesity and insulin sensitivity on adiponectin isoform distribution. Eur J Clin Investig 38, 827834.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
45. Nigro, E, Scudiero, O, Monaco, ML et al. (2014) New insight into adiponectin role in obesity and obesity-related diseases. BioMed Res Int 2014: 658913, 114.Google Scholar
46. Ouchi, N, Parker, JL, Lugus, JJ et al. (2011) Adipokines in inflammation and metabolic disease. Nat Rev: Immunol 11, 8597.Google Scholar
47. Bolzenius, JD, Laidlaw, DH, Cabeen, RP et al. (2013) Impact of body mass index on neuronal fiber bundle lengths among healthy older adults. Brain Imaging Behav 7, 300306.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
48. Beydoun, MA, Beydoun, H & Wang, Y (2008) Obesity and central obesity as risk factors for incident dementia and its sub-types: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Obes Rev 9, 204218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
49. Whitmer, RA, Gustafson, DR, Barrett-Connor, E et al. (2008) Central obesity and increased risk of dementia more than three decades later. Neurology 71, 10571064.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
50. Baumgarta, M, Snyderb, HM, Carrillob, MC et al. (2015) Summary of the evidence on modifiable risk factors for cognitive decline and dementia: a population-based perspective. Alzheimers Demen 11, 718726.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
51. Kim, S, Kim, Y & Park, SM (2016) Body mass index and decline of cognitive function. PLoS ONE 11, e0148908.Google Scholar
52. Hughes, TF, Borenstein, AR, Schofield, E et al. (2009) Association between late-life body mass index and dementia. Neurology 72, 17411746.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
53. Wotton, CJ & Goldacre, MJ (2014) Age at obesity and association with subsequent dementia: record linkage study. Postgrad Med J 90, 547551.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
54. Keage, HAD, Gupta, S & Brayne, C (2011) Risk for dementia and age at measurement. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 26, 329330.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
55. Kloppenborg, RP, van den Berg, E, Kappelle, LJ et al. (2008) Diabetes and other vascular risk factors for dementia: which factor matters most? A systematic review. Eur J Pharmacol 585, 97108.Google Scholar
56. Albanese, E, Launer, LJ, Egger, M et al. (2017) Body mass index in midlife and dementia: systematic review and meta-regression analysis of 589,649 men and women followed in longitudinal studies. Alzheimers Demen: Diagn Assess Dis Monitor 8, 165178.Google Scholar
57. Luchsinger, JA, Patel, B, Tang, M-X et al. (2007) Measures of adiposity and dementia risk in elderly persons. Arch Neurol 64, 392398.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
58. Luchsinger, JA & Gustafson, DR (2009) Adiposity and Alzheimer's disease. Curr Opin Clinic Nutr Metab Care 12, 1521.Google Scholar
59. Stevens, J, McClain, JE & Truesdale, KP (2008) Selection of measures in epidemiologic studies of the consequences of obesity. Int J Obes 32, S60S66.Google Scholar
60. Caruana, EJ, Roman, M, Hernández-Sánchez, J et al. (2015) Longitudinal studies. J Thorac Dis 7, E537E540.Google ScholarPubMed
61. Zelinski, E & Burnight, K (1997) Sixteen-year longitudinal and time lag changes in memory and cognition in older adults. Psychol Aging 12, 503513.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
62. Tombaugh, TN (2004) Trail making test A and B: normative data stratified by age and education. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 19, 203214.Google Scholar
63. Sabia, S, Kivimaki, M, Shipley, MJ et al. (2009) Body mass index over the adult life course and cognition in late midlife: the Whitehall II Cohort Study. Am J Clin Nutr 89, 607607.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
64. Besser, LM, Gill, DP, Monsell, SE et al. (2014) Body mass index, weight change, and clinical progression in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 28, 3643.Google Scholar
65. Gu, Y, Scarmeas, N, Cosentino, S et al. (2014) Change in body mass index before and after Alzheimer's disease onset. Curr Alzheimer Res 11, 349356.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
66. Felice, FGD & Ferreira, ST (2014) Inflammation, defective insulin signaling, and mitochondrial dysfunction as common molecular denominators connecting type 2 diabetes to Alzheimer disease. Diabetes 63, 22622272.Google Scholar
67. de Luca, C & Olefsky, JM (2008) Inflammation and insulin resistance. FEBS Lett 582, 97105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
68. Kim, B & Feldman, EL (2015) Insulin resistance as a key link for the increased risk of cognitive impairment in the metabolic syndrome. Exp Mol Med 47, e149.Google Scholar
69. Yates, KF, Sweat, V, Yau, PL et al. (2012) Impact of metabolic syndrome on cognition and brain: a selected review of the literature. Arterioscler Thromb Vasc Biol 32, 20602067.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
70. Felice, FGD, Lourenco, MV & Ferreira, ST (2014) How does brain insulin resistance develop in Alzheimer's disease? Alzheimers Demen 10, S26S32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
71. de la Monte, SM (2017) Insulin resistance and neurodegeneration: progress towards the development of new therapeutics for Alzheimer's disease. Drugs 77, 4765.Google Scholar
72. Mullins, RJ, Mustapic, M, Goetzl, EJ et al. (2017) Exosomal biomarkers of brain insulin resistance associated with regional atrophy in Alzheimer's disease. Hum Brain Mapp 38, 19331940.Google Scholar
73. Shemesh, E, Rudich, A, Harman-Boehm, I et al. (2012) Effect of intranasal insulin on cognitive function: a systematic review. J. Clinical Endocrinol Metab 97, 366376.Google Scholar
74. Claxton, A, Baker, LD, Hanson, A et al. (2015) Long-acting intranasal insulin detemir improves cognition for adults with mild cognitive impairment or early-stage Alzheimer's disease dementia. J Alzheimers Dis 44, 897906.Google Scholar
75. Reger, MA, Watson, GS, Green, PS et al. (2008) Intranasal insulin administration dose-dependently modulates verbal memory and plasma β-amyloid in memory-impaired older adults. J Alzheimers Dis 13, 323331.Google Scholar
76. Taylor, R (2012) Insulin resistance and type 2 diabetes. Diabetes 61, 778779.Google Scholar
77. Luchsinger, JA & Gustafson, DR (2009) Adiposity, type 2 diabetes and Alzheimer's disease. J Alzheimers Dis 16, 693704.Google Scholar
78. Lamport, DJ, Lawton, CL, Mansfield, MW et al. (2009) Impairments in glucose tolerance can have a negative impact on cognitive function: a systematic research review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 33, 394413.Google Scholar
79. Lamport, DJ, Chadwick, HK, Dye, L et al. (2014) A low glycaemic load breakfast can attenuate cognitive impairments observed in middle aged obese females with impaired glucose tolerance. Nutr Metab Cardiovasc Dis 24, 11281136.Google Scholar
80. Crane, PK, Walker, R, Hubbard, RA et al. (2013) Glucose levels and risk of dementia. N Engl J Med 369, 540548.Google Scholar
81. Anstey, KJ, Sargent-Cox, K, Eramudugolla, R et al. (2015) Association of cognitive function with glucose tolerance and trajectories of glucose tolerance over 12 years in the AusDiab study. Alzheimers Res Ther 7, 48.Google Scholar
82. Vagelatos, NT & Eslick, GD (2013) Type 2 diabetes as a risk factor for Alzheimer's disease: the confounders, interactions, and neuropathology associated with this relationship. Epidemiol Rev 35, 152160.Google Scholar
83. Cheng, G, Huang, C, Deng, H et al. (2012) Diabetes as a risk factor for dementia and mild cognitive impairment: a meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Intern Med J 42, 484491.Google Scholar
84. Cukierman, T, Gerstein, HC & Williamson, JD (2005) Cognitive decline and dementia in diabetes – systematic overview of prospective observational studies. Diabetologia 48, 24602469.Google Scholar
85. Kanaya, AM, Barrett-Connor, E, Gildengorin, G et al. (2004) Change in cognitive function by glucose tolerance status in older adults a 4-year prospective study of the Rancho Bernardo Study Cohort. JAMA 164, 13271333.Google Scholar
86. Awad, N, Gagnon, M & Messier, C (2004) The relationship between impaired glucose tolerance, type 2 diabetes, and cognitive function. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 26, 10441080.Google Scholar
87. Messier, C (2005) Impact of impaired glucose tolerance and type 2 diabetes on cognitive aging. Neurobiol Aging 26, 2630.Google Scholar
88. Fontbonne, A, Berr, C, Ducimetière, P et al. (2001) Changes in cognitive abilities over a 4-year period are unfavorably affected in elderly diabetic subjects: results of the epidemiology of vascular aging study. Diab Care 24, 366370.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
89. Huang, R-R, Jia, B-H, Xie, L et al. (2015) Spatial working memory impairment in primary onset middle-age type 2 diabetes mellitus: an ethology and BOLD-fMRI study. J Magn Reson Imaging 43, 7587.Google Scholar
90. Abbatecola, AM, Paolisso, G, Lamponi, M et al. (2004) Insulin resistance and executive dysfunction in older persons. J Am Geriatr Soc 52, 17131718.Google Scholar
91. Munshi, M, Capelson, R, Grande, L et al. (2006) Cognitive dysfunction is associated with poor diabetes control in older adults. Diab Care 29, 17941799.Google Scholar
92. Ravona-Springer, R, Heymann, A, Schmeidler, J et al. (2014) Trajectories in glycemic control over time are associated with cognitive performance in elderly subjects with type 2 diabetes. PLoS ONE 9, e97384.Google Scholar
93. Martín-Timón, I, Sevillano-Collantes, C, Segura-Galindo, A et al. (2014) Type 2 diabetes and cardiovascular disease: have all risk factors the same strength? World J Diab 5, 444470.Google Scholar
94. Mitsuhashi, T, Hibi, K, Kosuge, M et al. (2011) Relation between hyperinsulinemia and nonculprit plaque characteristics in nondiabetic patients with acute coronary syndromes. JACC: Cardiovasc Imaging 4, 392401.Google Scholar
95. Patel, TP, Rawal, K, Bagchi, AK et al. (2016) Insulin resistance: an additional risk factor in the pathogenesis of cardiovascular disease in type 2 diabetes. Heart Fail Rev 21, 1123.Google Scholar
96. Whitmer, RA, Sidney, S, Selby, J et al. (2005) Midlife cardiovascular risk factors and risk of dementia in late life. Nurology 64, 277281.Google Scholar
97. de la Torre, JC (2012) Cardiovascular risk factors promote brain hypoperfusion leading to cognitive decline and dementia. Cardiovasc Psychiatry Neurol 2012: article 367516.Google Scholar
98. Umemura, T, Kawamura, T & Hotta, N (2017) Pathogenesis and neuroimaging of cerebral large and small vessel disease in type 2 diabetes: a possible link between cerebral and retinal microvascular abnormalities. J Diabetes Investig 8, 134148.Google Scholar
99. Wisse, LEM, de Bresser, J, Geerlings, MI et al. (2014) Global brain atrophy but not hippocampal atrophy is related to type 2 diabetes. J Neurol Sci 344, 3236.Google Scholar
100. Kooistra, M, Geerlings, MI, Mali, WPTM et al. (2013) Diabetes mellitus and progression of vascular brain lesions and brain atrophy in patients with symptomatic atherosclerotic disease. The SMART-MR study. J Neurol Sci 332, 6974.Google Scholar
101. Moloney, AM, Griffin, RJ, Timmons, S et al. (2010) Defects in IGF-1 receptor, insulin receptor and IRS-1/2 in Alzheimer's disease indicate possible resistance to IGF-1 and insulin signalling. Neurobiol Aging 31, 224243.Google Scholar
102. Talbot, K, Wang, H-Y, Kazi, H et al. (2012) Demonstrated brain insulin resistance in Alzheimer's disease patients is associated with IGF-1 resistance, IRS-1 dysregulation, and cognitive decline. J Clin Invest 122, 13161338.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
103. Cohen, AC, Tong, M, Wands, JR et al. (2007) Insulin and insulin-like growth factor resistance with neurodegeneration in an adult chronic ethanol exposure model. Alcoholism: Clinical Exper Res 31, 15581573.Google Scholar
104. Apostolatos, A, Song, S, Acosta, S et al. (2012) Insulin promotes neuronal survival via the alternatively spliced protein kinase CδII isoform. J Biol Chem 287, 92999310.Google Scholar
105. de la Monte, SM (2014) Type 3 diabetes is sporadic Alzheimer's disease: mini-review. Euro Neuropsychopharmacol: Euro Coll Neuropsychopharmacol 24, 19541960.Google Scholar
106. Vandal, M, White, PJ, Tremblay, C et al. (2014) Insulin reverses the high-fat diet–induced increase in brain Aβ and improves memory in an animal model of Alzheimer disease. Diabetes 63, 42914301.Google Scholar
107. Son, SM, Song, H, Byun, J et al. (2012) Accumulation of autophagosomes contributes to enhanced amyloidogenic APP processing under insulin-resistant conditions. Autophagy 8, 18421844.Google Scholar
108. Butterfield, DA, Di Domenico, F & Barone, E (2014) Elevated risk of type 2 diabetes for development of Alzheimer disease: a key role for oxidative stress in brain. Biochim Biophys Acta 1842, 16931706.Google Scholar
109. Qizilbash, N, Gregson, J, Johnson, ME et al. (2015) BMI and risk of dementia in two million people over two decades: a retrospective cohort study. Lancet Diab Endocrinol 3, 431436.Google Scholar
110. Fitzpatrick, AL, Kuller, LH, Lopez, OL et al. (2009) Mid- and late-life obesity: risk of dementia in the cardiovascular health cognition study. Arch Neurol 66, 336342.Google Scholar
111. Harrison, JK & Shenkin, SD (2015) Body mass index and the risk of dementia – what do we know and what should we do? J R Coll Physicians Edinb 45, 141142.Google Scholar
112. Snijder, MB, Dekker, JM, Visser, M et al. (2004) Trunk fat and leg fat have independent and opposite associations with fasting and postload glucose levels: the Hoorn study. Diab Care 27, 372377.Google Scholar
113. Chrousos, GP (2009) Stress and disorders of the stress system. Nat Rev Endocrinol 5, 374381.Google Scholar
114. McEwen, BS (1998) Stress, adaptation, and disease – allostasis and allostatic load. In Neuroimmunomodulation: Molecular Aspects, Integrative Systems, and Clinical Advances, vol. 840, pp. 3344 [McCann, SM, Lipton, JM, Sternberg, EM, Chrousos, GP, Gold, PW and Smith, CC, editors]. New York: New York Academy of Sciences.Google Scholar
115. McEwen, BS (2004) Protection and damage from acute and chronic stress – allostasis and allostatic overload and relevance to the pathophysiology of psychiatric disorders. In Biobehavioral Stress Response: Protective and Damaging Effects, vol. 1032, pp. 17 [Yehuda, R and McEwen, B, editors]. New York: New York Academy of Sciences.Google Scholar
116. McEwen, BS (2008) Central effects of stress hormones in health and disease: understanding the protective and damaging effects of stress and stress mediators. Eur J Pharmacol 583, 174185.Google Scholar
117. Kyrou, I, Chrousos, GP & Tsigos, C (2006) Stress, visceral obesity, and metabolic complications. In Stress, Obesity, and Metabolic Syndrome, vol. 1083, pp. 77110 [Chrousos, GP and Tsigos, C, editors]. New York: New York Academy of Sciences.Google Scholar
118. Lupien, SJ, Fiocco, A, Wan, N et al. (2005) Stress hormones and human memory function across the lifespan. Psychoneuroendocrinology 30, 225242.Google Scholar
119. Sinha, R & Jastreboff, AM (2013) Stress as a common risk factor for obesity and addiction. Biol Psychiatry 73, 827835.Google Scholar
120. Rohleder, N & Kirschbaum, C (2007) Effects of nutrition on neuro-endocrine stress responses. Curr Opin Clin Nutr Metab Care 10, 504510.Google Scholar
121. Maniam, J & Morris, MJ (2012) The link between stress and feeding behaviour. Neuropharmacology 63, 97110.Google Scholar
122. Picard, M, Juster, R-P & McEwen, BS (2014) Mitochondrial allostatic load puts the ‘gluc’ back in glucocorticoids. Nat Rev Endocrinol 10, 303310.Google Scholar
123. Marniemi, J, Kronholm, E, Aunola, S et al. (2002) Visceral fat and psychosocial stress in identical twins discordant for obesity. J Intern Med 251, 3543.Google Scholar
124. Pyykkonen, A-J, Raikkonen, K, Tuomi, T et al. (2010) Stressful life events and the metabolic syndrome – the prevalence, prediction and prevention of diabetes (PPP)-Botnia Study. Diab Care 33, 378384.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
125. Brunner, EJ, Chandola, T & Marmot, MG (2007) Prospective effect of job strain on general and central obesity in the Whitehall II study. Am J Epidemiol 165, 828837.Google Scholar
126. Oliver, G, Wardle, J & Gibson, EL (2000) Stress and food choice: a laboratory study. Psychosom Med 62, 853865.Google Scholar
127. Steptoe, A, Lipsey, Z & Wardle, J (1998) Stress, hassles and variations in alcohol consumption, food choice and physical exercise: a diary study. Br J Health Psychol 3, 5163.Google Scholar
128. Ng, DM & Jeffery, RW (2003) Relationships between perceived stress and health behaviors in a sample of working adults. Health Psychol 22, 638642.Google Scholar
129. Conner, M, Fitter, M & Fletcher, W (1999) Stress and snacking: a diary study of daily hassles and between-meal snacking. Psychol Health 14, 5163.Google Scholar
130. Oliver, G & Wardle, J (1999) Perceived effects of stress on food choice. Physiol Behav 66, 511515.Google Scholar
131. Gluck, ME, Geliebter, A, Hung, J et al. (2004) Cortisol, hunger, and desire to binge eat following a cold stress test in obese women with binge eating disorder. Psychosom Med 66, 876881.Google Scholar
132. O'Connor, DB, Jones, F, Conner, M et al. (2008) Effects of daily hassles and eating style on eating behavior. Health Psychol 27, S20S31.Google Scholar
133. Bjorntorp, P & Rosmond, P (2000) Obesity and cortisol. Nutrition 16, 924936.Google Scholar
134. Rosmond, R & Bjorntorp, P (2000) Occupational status, cortisol secretory pattern, and visceral obesity in middle-aged men. Obes Res 8, 445450.Google Scholar
135. Epel, ES, McEwen, B, Seeman, T et al. (2000) Stress and body shape: stress-induced cortisol secretion is consistently greater among women with central fat. Psychosom Med 62, 623632.Google Scholar
136. Picard, M & Turnbull, DM (2013) Linking the metabolic state and mitochondrial DNA in chronic disease, health, and aging. Diabetes 62, 672678.Google Scholar
137. Wolf, OT (2009) Stress and memory in humans: twelve years of progress? Brain Res 1293, 142154.Google Scholar
138. Lupien, SJ, Maheu, F, Tu, M et al. (2007) The effects of stress and stress hormones on human cognition: implications for the field of brain and cognition. Brain Cogn 65, 209237.Google Scholar
139. Lupien, SJ, de Leon, M, de Santi, S et al. (1998) Cortisol levels during human aging predict hippocampal atrophy and memory deficits. Nat Neurosci 1, 6973.Google Scholar
140. Dai, J, Buijs, R & Swaab, D (2004) Glucocorticoid hormone (cortisol) affects axonal transport in human cortex neurons but shows resistance in Alzheimer's disease. Br J Pharmacol 143, 606610.Google Scholar
141. Lasikiewicz, N, Hendrickx, H, Talbot, D et al. (2013) Exploring stress-induced cognitive impairment in middle aged, centrally obese adults. Stress: Int J Biol Stress 16, 4453.Google Scholar
142. Spitznagel, MB, Hawkins, M, Alosco, M et al. (2015) Neurocognitive effects of obesity and bariatric surgery. Eur Eat Disord Rev 23, 488495.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
143. Alosco, ML, Spitznagel, MB, Strain, G et al. (2014) Family history of Alzheimer's disease limits improvement in cognitive function after bariatric surgery. SAGE Open Med 2, 2050312114539477.Google Scholar
144. Hoyland, A, Lawton, CL & Dye, L (2008) Acute effects of macronutrient manipulations on cognitive test performance in healthy young adults: a systematic research review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 32, 7285.Google Scholar
145. Lamport, D, Dye, L, Wightman, J et al. (2012) The effects of flavonoid and other polyphenol consumption on cognitive performance: a systematic research review of human experimental and epidemiological studies. Nutr Aging 1, 525.Google Scholar
146. Jacobs, DR Jr, Gross, MD & Tapsell, LC (2009) Food synergy: an operational concept for understanding nutrition. Am J Clin Nutr 89, S1543S1548.Google Scholar
147. Nilsson, A, Tovar, J, Johansson, M et al. (2013) A diet based on multiple functional concepts improves cognitive performance in healthy subjects. Nutr Metab 10, 49.Google Scholar
148. Smith, PJ, Blumenthal, JA, Babyak, MA et al. (2010) Effects of the dietary approaches to stop hypertension diet, exercise, and caloric restriction on neurocognition in overweight adults with high blood pressure. Hypertension 55, 1331–U1385.Google Scholar
149. Sofi, F, Macchi, C, Abbate, R et al. (2014) Mediterranean diet and health status: an updated meta-analysis and a proposal for a literature-based adherence score. Public Health Nutr 17, 27692782.Google Scholar
150. National Institute for Health and Care Excellence (2014) Maintaining a healthy weight and preventing excess weight gain among children and adults: draft guideline. Available at: http://www.nice.org.uk/guidance/gid-phg78/resources/maintaining-a-healthy-weight-and-preventing-excess-weight-gain-among-children-and-adults-draft-guideline2 (accessed April 2017).Google Scholar
151. Committee of the European Union (2014) Council conclusions on nutrition and physical activity. http://www.consilium.europa.eu/uedocs/cms_data/docs/pressdata/en/lsa/143285.pdf (accessed June 2017).Google Scholar
152. Tangney, CC, Kwasny, MJ, Li, H et al. (2011) Adherence to a Mediterranean-type dietary pattern and cognitive decline in a community population. Am J Clin Nutr 93, 601607.Google Scholar
153. Wengreen, H, Munger, RG, Cutler, A et al. (2013) Prospective study of dietary approaches to stop hypertension- and Mediterranean-style dietary patterns and age-related cognitive change: the Cache County Study on Memory, Health and Aging. Am J Clin Nutr 98, 12631271.Google Scholar
154. Feart, C, Samieri, C, Rondeau, V et al. (2009) Adherence to a Mediterranean diet, cognitive decline, and risk of dementia. Jama-J Am Med Assoc 302, 638648.Google Scholar
155. Scarmeas, N, Stern, Y, Mayeux, R et al. (2009) Mediterranean diet and mild cognitive impairment. Arch Neurol 66, 216225.Google Scholar
156. Tsivgoulis, G, Judd, S, Letter, AJ et al. (2013) Adherence to a Mediterranean diet and risk of incident cognitive impairment. Neurology 80, 16841692.Google Scholar
157. Samieri, C, Grodstein, F, Rosner, BA et al. (2013) Mediterranean diet and cognitive function in older age. Epidemiology 24, 490499.Google Scholar
158. Ye, X, Scott, T, Gao, X et al. (2013) Mediterranean diet, healthy eating index 2005, and cognitive function in middle-aged and older Puerto Rican adults. J Acad Nutr Diet 113, 276281.Google Scholar
159. Gardener, S, Gu, Y, Rainey-Smith, SR et al. (2012) Adherence to a Mediterranean diet and Alzheimer's disease risk in an Australian population. Transl Psychiatry 2, e164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
160. Scarmeas, N, Stern, Y, Mayeux, R et al. (2006) Mediterranean diet, Alzheimer disease, and vascular mediation. Arch Neurol 63, 17091717.Google Scholar
161. Singh, B, Parsaik, AK, Mielke, MM et al. (2014) Association of Mediterranean diet with mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Alzheimers Dis 39, 271282.Google Scholar
162. Psaltopoulou, T, Sergentanis, TN, Panagiotakos, DB et al. (2013) Mediterranean diet, stroke, cognitive impairment, and depression: a meta-analysis. Ann Neurol 74, 580591.Google Scholar
163. Sofi, F, Abbate, R, Gensini, GF et al. (2010) Accruing evidence on benefits of adherence to the Mediterranean diet on health an updated systematic review and meta-analysis. Am J Clin Nutr 92, 11891196.Google Scholar
164. Kesse-Guyot, E, Andreeva, VA, Lassale, C et al. (2013) Mediterranean diet and cognitive function: a French study. Am J Clin Nutr 97, 369376.Google Scholar
165. Cherbuin, N & Anstey, KJ (2012) The Mediterranean diet is not related to cognitive change in a large prospective investigation: the PATH through life study. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 20, 635639.Google Scholar
166. Valls-Pedret, C, Sala-Vila, A, Serra-Mir, M et al. (2015) Mediterranean diet and age-related cognitive decline: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Intern Med 175, 10941103.Google Scholar
167. Jackson, AS, Stanforth, PR, Gagnon, J et al. (2002) The effect of sex, age and race on estimating percentage body fat from body mass index: The Heritage Family Study. Int J Obes 26, 789796.Google Scholar
168. Profenno, LA, Porsteinsson, AP & Faraone, SV (2010) Meta-analysis of Alzheimer's disease risk with obesity, diabetes, and related disorders. Biol Psychiatry 67, 505512.Google Scholar
169. Gudala, K, Bansal, D, Schifano, F et al. (2013) Diabetes mellitus and risk of dementia: a meta-analysis of prospective observational studies. J Diab Invest 4, 640650.Google Scholar