Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dsjbd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T09:18:33.839Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A comprehensive assessment of memory, delay aversion, timing, inhibition, decision making and variability in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: advancing beyond the three-pathway models

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  31 October 2013

D. R. Coghill*
Affiliation:
Division of Neuroscience, Medical Research Institute, University of Dundee, Ninewells Hospital, Dundee, UK
S. Seth
Affiliation:
Division of Neuroscience, Medical Research Institute, University of Dundee, Ninewells Hospital, Dundee, UK NHS Tayside, Child and Adolescent Mental Health Service, Dundee, UK
K. Matthews
Affiliation:
Division of Neuroscience, Medical Research Institute, University of Dundee, Ninewells Hospital, Dundee, UK
*
*Address for correspondence: Dr D. R. Coghill, Division of Neuroscience, Ninewells Hospital, Dundee DD1 9SY, UK. (Email: d.r.coghill@dundee.ac.uk)

Abstract

Background

Although attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) has been associated with a broad range of deficits across various neuropsychological domains, most studies have assessed only a narrow range of neuropsychological functions. Direct cross-domain comparisons are rare, with almost all studies restricted to less than four domains. Therefore, the relationships between these various domains remain undefined. In addition, almost all studies included previously medicated participants, limiting the conclusions that can be drawn. We present the first study to compare a large cohort of medication-naive boys with ADHD with healthy controls on a broad battery of neuropsychological tasks, assessing six key domains of neuropsychological functioning.

Method

The neuropsychological functioning of 83 medication-naive boys with well-characterized ADHD (mean age 8.9 years) was compared with that of 66 typically developing (TYP) boys (mean age 9.0 years) on a broad battery of validated neuropsychological tasks.

Results

Data reduction using complementary factor analysis (CFA) confirmed six distinct neuropsychological domains: working memory, inhibition, delay aversion, decision making, timing and response variability. Boys with ADHD performed less well across all six domains although, for each domain, only a minority of boys with ADHD had a deficit [effect size (% with deficit) ADHD versus TYP: working memory 0.95 (30.1), inhibition 0.61 (22.9), delay aversion 0.82 (36.1), decision making 0.55 (20.5), timing 0.71 (31.3), response variability 0.37 (18.1)].

Conclusions

The clinical syndrome of ADHD is neuropsychologically heterogeneous. These data highlight the complexity of the relationships between the different neuropsychological profiles associated with ADHD and the clinical symptoms and functional impairment.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

APA (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edn. American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Barkley, RA (1997). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, self-regulation, and time: toward a more comprehensive theory. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics 18, 271279.Google Scholar
Bedard, AC, Ickowicz, A, Logan, GD, Hogg-Johnson, S, Schachar, R, Tannock, R (2003). Selective inhibition in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder off and on stimulant medication. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 31, 315327.Google Scholar
Browne, MW, Cudeck, R (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In Testing Structural Equation Models (ed. Bollen, K. A. and Long, J. S.), pp. 136162. Sage: Newbury Park, CA.Google Scholar
Castellanos, FX, Tannock, R (2002). Neuroscience of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: the search for endophenotypes. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 3, 617628.Google Scholar
Clark, L, Manes, F, Antoun, N, Sahakian, BJ, Robbins, TW (2003). The contributions of lesion laterality and lesion volume to decision-making impairment following frontal lobe damage. Neuropsychologia 41, 14741483.Google Scholar
Coghill, D, Nigg, J, Rothenberger, A, Sonuga-Barke, E, Tannock, R (2005). Whither causal models in the neuroscience of ADHD? Developmental Science 8, 105114.Google Scholar
Coghill, DR, Rhodes, SM, Matthews, K (2007). The neuropsychological effects of chronic methylphenidate on drug-naive boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry 62, 954962.Google Scholar
Coghill, DR, Hayward, D, Rhodes, SM, Grimmer, C, Matthews, K (2013). A longitudinal examination of neuropsychological and clinical functioning in boys with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): improvements in executive functioning do not explain clinical improvement. Psychological Medicine. Published online: 19 July 2013 . doi:10.1017/S0033291713001761.Google Scholar
Cohen, J (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd edn. Lawrence Earlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, NJ.Google Scholar
Conners, CK, Sitarenios, G, Parker, JD, Epstein, JN (1998). The revised Conners’ Parent Rating Scale (CPRS-R): factor structure, reliability, and criterion validity. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 26, 257268.Google Scholar
Crosbie, J, Perusse, D, Barr, CL, Schachar, RJ (2008). Validating psychiatric endophenotypes: inhibitory control and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neuroscience and Biobehavoral Reviews 32, 4055.Google Scholar
Dancey, C, Reidy, J (2004). Statistics Without Maths for Psychology: Using SPSS for Windows. Prentice Hall: London.Google Scholar
DeVito, EE, Blackwell, AD, Kent, L, Ersche, KD, Clark, L, Salmond, CH, Dezsery, AM, Sahakian, BJ (2008). The effects of methylphenidate on decision making in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry 64, 636639.Google Scholar
Doshi, JA, Hodgkins, P, Kahle, J, Sikirica, V, Cangelosi, MJ, Setyawan, J, Erder, MH, Neumann, PJ (2012). Economic impact of childhood and adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in the United States. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 51, 9901002.Google Scholar
Dunn, L, Dunn, L, Whetton, C, Burley, J (1997). British Picture Vocabulary Scale, 2nd edn. NFER-Nelson: London.Google Scholar
Fair, DA, Bathula, D, Nikolas, MA, Nigg, JT (2012). Distinct neuropsychological subgroups in typically developing youth inform heterogeneity in children with ADHD. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 109, 67696774.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garon, N, Moore, C, Waschbusch, DA (2006). Decision making in children with ADHD only, ADHD-anxious/depressed, and control children using a child version of the Iowa Gambling Task. Journal of Attentional Disorders 9, 607619.Google Scholar
Geurts, HM, van der Oord, S, Crone, EA (2006). Hot and cool aspects of cognitive control in children with ADHD: decision-making and inhibition. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 34, 813824.Google Scholar
Goodman, R (2001). Psychometric properties of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 40, 13371345.Google Scholar
Holmes, J, Lawson, D, Langley, K, Fitzpatrick, H, Trumper, A, Pay, H, Harrington, R, Thapar, A (2004). The Child Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Teacher Telephone Interview (CHATTI): reliability and validity. British Journal of Psychiatry 184, 7478.Google Scholar
Hyman, SE (2010). The diagnosis of mental disorders: the problem of reification. Annual Reviews of Clinical Psychology 6, 155179.Google Scholar
Ivry, RB, Spencer, RM (2004). The neural representation of time. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 14, 225232.Google Scholar
Kaufman, J, Birmaher, B, Brent, DA, Ryan, ND, Rao, U (2000). K-SADS-PL. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 39, 1208.Google Scholar
Kempton, S, Vance, A, Maruff, P, Luk, E, Costin, J, Pantelis, C (1999). Executive function and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: stimulant medication and better executive function performance in children. Psychological Medicine 29, 527538.Google Scholar
Kline, RB (2005). Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling, 2nd edn. Guilford Press: New York.Google Scholar
Kuntsi, J, Frazier-Wood, AC, Banaschewski, T, Gill, M, Miranda, A, Oades, RD, Roeyers, H, Rothenberger, A, Steinhausen, HC, van der Meere, JJ, Faraone, SV, Asherson, P, Rijsdijk, F (2012). Genetic analysis of reaction time variability: room for improvement? Psychological Medicine 43, 13231333.Google Scholar
Kuntsi, J, Oosterlaan, J, Stevenson, J (2001). Psychological mechanisms in hyperactivity: I. Response inhibition deficit, working memory impairment, delay aversion, or something else? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 42, 199210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Logan, GD, Cowan, WB, Davis, KA (1984). On the ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction time responses: a model and a method. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception 10, 276291.Google Scholar
Miller, G (2010). Psychiatry. Beyond DSM: seeking a brain-based classification of mental illness. Science 327, 1437.Google Scholar
Morris, RC, Evendon, JL, Sahakian, BJ, Robbins, TW (1987). Computer-aided assessment of dementia: comparative studies of neuropsychological deficits in Alzheimer-type dementia and Parkinson's disease. In Cognitive Neurochemistry (ed. Stahl, S. M., Iversen, S. D. and Goodman, E. C.), pp. 2136. Oxford University Press: Oxford.Google Scholar
Nigg, JT, Willcutt, EG, Doyle, AE, Sonuga-Barke, EJ (2005). Causal heterogeneity in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: do we need neuropsychologically impaired subtypes? Biological Psychiatry 57, 12241230.Google Scholar
Office of the Chief Statistician (2009). Scottish Index of Multiple Deprivation (SIMD) 2009. Scottish Government National Statistics: Edinburgh.Google Scholar
Owen, AM, Sahakian, BJ, Semple, J, Polkey, CE, Robbins, TW (1995). Visuo-spatial short-term recognition memory and learning after temporal lobe excisions, frontal lobe excisions or amygdalo-hippocampectomy in man. Neuropsychologia 33, 124.Google Scholar
Petrides, M, Milner, B (1982). Deficits on subject-ordered tasks after frontal- and temporal-lobe lesions in man. Neuropsychologia 20, 249262.Google Scholar
Polanczyk, G, de Lima, MS, Horta, BL, Biederman, J, Rohde, LA (2007). The worldwide prevalence of ADHD: a systematic review and metaregression analysis. American Journal of Psychiatry 164, 942948.Google Scholar
Rhodes, SM, Coghill, DR, Matthews, K (2004). Methylphenidate restores visual memory, but not working memory function in attention deficit-hyperkinetic disorder. Psychopharmacology 175, 319330.Google Scholar
Rhodes, SM, Coghill, DR, Matthews, K (2005). Neuropsychological functioning in stimulant-naive boys with hyperkinetic disorder. Psychological Medicine 35, 11091120.Google Scholar
Rhodes, SM, Coghill, DR, Matthews, K (2006). Acute neuropsychological effects of methylphenidate in stimulant drug-naive boys with ADHD II – broader executive and non-executive domains. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 47, 11841194.Google Scholar
Rhodes, SM, Park, J, Seth, S, Coghill, DR (2012). A comprehensive investigation of memory impairment in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and oppositional defiant disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 53, 128137.Google Scholar
Robbins, TW, Semple, J, Kumar, R, Truman, MI, Shorter, J, Ferraro, A, Fox, B, McKay, G, Matthews, K (1997). Effects of scopolamine on delayed-matching-to-sample and paired associates tests of visual memory and learning in human subjects: comparison with diazepam and implications for dementia. Psychopharmacology 134, 95106.Google Scholar
Rogers, RD, Everitt, BJ, Baldacchino, A, Blackshaw, AJ, Swainson, R, Wynne, K, Baker, NB, Hunter, J, Carthy, T, Booker, E, London, M, Deakin, JF, Sahakian, BJ, Robbins, TW (1999). Dissociable deficits in the decision-making cognition of chronic amphetamine abusers, opiate abusers, patients with focal damage to prefrontal cortex, and tryptophan-depleted normal volunteers: evidence for monoaminergic mechanisms. Neuropsychopharmacology 20, 322339.Google Scholar
Rubia, K, Taylor, A, Taylor, E, Sergeant, JA (1999). Synchronization, anticipation, and consistency in motor timing of children with dimensionally defined attention deficit hyperactivity behaviour. Perceptual and Motor Skills 89, 12371258.Google Scholar
Sahakian, BJ, Morris, RG, Evenden, JL, Heald, A, Levy, R, Philpot, M, Robbins, TW (1988). A comparative study of visuospatial memory and learning in Alzheimer-type dementia and Parkinson's disease. Brain 111, 695718.Google Scholar
Schachar, R, Mota, VL, Logan, GD, Tannock, R, Klim, P (2000). Confirmation of an inhibitory control deficit in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 28, 227235.Google Scholar
Solanto, MV, Abikoff, H, Sonuga-Barke, E, Schachar, R, Logan, GD, Wigal, T, Hechtman, L, Hinshaw, S, Turkel, E (2001). The ecological validity of delay aversion and response inhibition as measures of impulsivity in AD/HD: a supplement to the NIMH multimodal treatment study of AD/HD. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 29, 215228.Google Scholar
Sonuga-Barke, E, Bitsakou, P, Thompson, M (2010). Beyond the dual pathway model: evidence for the dissociation of timing, inhibitory, and delay-related impairments in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 49, 345355.Google Scholar
Sonuga-Barke, EJ (2002). Psychological heterogeneity in AD/HD – a dual pathway model of behaviour and cognition. Behavioral Brain Research 130, 2936.Google Scholar
Sonuga-Barke, EJ (2005). Causal models of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: from common simple deficits to multiple developmental pathways. Biological Psychiatry 57, 12311238.Google Scholar
Sonuga-Barke, EJ, Sergeant, JA, Nigg, J, Willcutt, E (2008). Executive dysfunction and delay aversion in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: nosologic and diagnostic implications. Child Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America 17, 367384, ix.Google Scholar
Sonuga-Barke, EJ, Taylor, E, Heptinstall, E (1992 a). Hyperactivity and delay aversion – II. The effect of self versus externally imposed stimulus presentation periods on memory. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 33, 399409.Google Scholar
Sonuga-Barke, EJ, Taylor, E, Sembi, S, Smith, J (1992 b). Hyperactivity and delay aversion – I. The effect of delay on choice. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 33, 387398.Google Scholar
Toplak, ME, Tannock, R (2005). Tapping and anticipation performance in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Perceptual and Motor Skills 100, 659675.Google Scholar
van Lieshout, M, Luman, M, Buitelaar, J, Rommelse, NN, Oosterlaan, J (2013). Does neurocognitive functioning predict future or persistence of ADHD? A systematic review. Clinical Psychology Review 33, 539560.Google Scholar
Willcutt, EG, Doyle, AE, Nigg, JT, Faraone, SV, Pennington, BF (2005). Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic review. Biological Psychiatry 57, 13361346.Google Scholar
Wilson, HK, Cox, DJ, Merkel, RL, Moore, M, Coghill, D (2006). Effect of extended release stimulant-based medications on neuropsychological functioning among adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 21, 797807.Google Scholar
Winer, BJ, Brown, DR, Michels, KM (1991). Statistical Principles in Experimental Design, 3rd edn. McGraw Hill: New York.Google Scholar
Supplementary material: File

Coghill et al. Supplementary Material

Tables

Download Coghill et al. Supplementary Material(File)
File 107 KB